230
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The exaptive potential of (object) play behavior

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 40-52 | Received 20 Nov 2021, Accepted 18 Aug 2022, Published online: 19 Dec 2022

References

  • Andrews, P., Gangestad, S., & Matthews, D. (2002). Adaptationism – How to carry out an exaptationist program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(4), 489–504. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X02000092
  • Auerbach, J., Kanarek, A., & Burghardt, G. (2015). To play or not to play? That’s a resource abundance question. Adaptive Behavior, 23(6), 354–361. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712315611188
  • Bateson, P. (2006). The adaptability driver: Links between behavior and evolution. Biological Theory, 1(4), 342–345. https://doi.org/10.1162/biot.2006.1.4.342
  • Bateson, P., & Martin, P. (2013). Play, playfulness, creativity and innovation. Cambridge University Press.
  • Biben, M., & Champoux, M. (1999). Play and stress: Cortisol as a negative correlate of play in Saimiri. In S. Reifel (Ed.), Play and culture studies vol. 2 (pp. 191–208). Ablex.
  • Birch, H. (1945). The relation of previous experience to insightful problem-solving. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 38(6), 367–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0056104
  • Bjorklund, D., & Gardiner, A. (2011). Object play and tool use: Developmental and evolutionary perspectives. In A. Pellegrini (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the development of play (pp. 153–171). Oxford University Press.
  • Boyer, P., & Liénard, P. (2020). Ingredients of ‘rituals’ and their cognitive underpinnings. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1805), 20190439. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0439
  • Bruner, J. (1972). Nature and uses of immaturity. American Psychologist, 27(8), 687–708. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033144
  • Burghardt, G. (1984). On the origins of play. In P. Smith (Ed.), Play in animals and humans (pp. 5–41). Basil Blackwell.
  • Burghardt, G. (2004). Play: How evolution can explain the most mysterious behavior of all. In A. Moya, & E. Font (Eds.), Evolution: From molecules to ecosystems (pp. 231–246). Oxford University Press.
  • Burghardt, G. (2005). The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits. MIT Press.
  • Burghardt, G. (2018). The origins, evolution, and interconnections of play and ritual: Setting the stage. In C. Renfrew, I. Morley, & M. Boyd (Eds.), Ritual, play and belief, in evolution and early human societies (pp. 23–39). Cambridge University Press.
  • Burghardt, G., & Pellis, S. (2019). New directions in studying the evolution of play. In P. Smith, & J. Roopnarine (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of play: Developmental and disciplinary perspectives (pp. 11–29). Cambridge University Press.
  • Campbell, D. (1960). Blind variation and selective retentions in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67(6), 380–400. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040373
  • Campbell, D. (1965). Variation and selective retention in socio-cultural evolution. In H. Barringer, G. Blanksten, & R. Mack (Eds.), Social change in developing areas (pp. 19–49). Schenkman.
  • Caro, T. (1995). Short-term costs and correlates of play in cheetahs. Animal Behaviour, 49(2), 333–345. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1995.9999
  • Cenni, C., Casarrubea, M., Gunst, N., Vasey, P., Pellis, S., Wandia, N., & Leca, J.-B. (2020). Inferring functional patterns of tool use behavior from the temporal structure of object play sequences in a non-human primate species. Physiology & Behavior, 222, 112938. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112938
  • Cenni, C., & Leca, J.-B. (2020). Tool use. In J. Vonk, & T. Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of animal cognition and behavior. Springer International Publishing. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1541-1.
  • Cenni, C., Pellis, S., Wandia, N., & Leca, J.-B. (2021). Stone affordances as potential for action expression in object play in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 135(3), 430–438. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000283
  • Chotard, H., Ioannou, S., & Davila-Ross, M. (2018). Infrared thermal imaging: Positive and negative emotions modify the skin temperatures of monkey and ape faces. American Journal of Primatology, 80(5), e22863. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22863
  • Coppinger, R., & Smith, C. (1989). A model for understanding the evolution of mammalian behavior. In H. Genoways (Ed.), Current mammalogy. Vol. 2 (pp. 335–374). Plenum Press.
  • Fagen, R. (1974). Selective and evolutionary aspects of animal play. The American Naturalist, 108(964), 850–858. https://doi.org/10.1086/282960
  • Fairbanks, L. (2000). The developmental timing of primate play: A neural selection model. In S. Parker, J. Langer, & M. McKinney (Eds.), Biology, brains, and behavior: The evolution of human development (pp. 131–158). School of American Research Press.
  • Fisher, R. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Clarendon Press.
  • Gajdon, G., Lichtnegger, M., & Huber, L. (2014). What a parrot’s mind adds to play: The urge to produce novelty fosters tool use acquisition in kea. Open Journal of Animal Sciences, 4(2), 51–58. https://doi.org/10.4236/ojas.2014.42008
  • Gould, S. (1996). Creating the creators. Discover, 17(10), 42–49.
  • Gould, S., & Vrba, E. (1982). Exaptation—a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology, 8(1), 4–15. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0094837300004310
  • Gumert, M., Kluck, M., & Malaivijitnond, S. (2009). The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand. American Journal of Primatology, 71(7), 594–608. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20694
  • Heldstab, S., Kosonen, Z., Koski, S., Burkart, J., van Schaik, C., & Isler, K. (2016). Manipulation complexity in primates coevolved with brain size and terrestriality. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 24528. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep24528
  • Huffman, M. (1984). Stone-play of Macaca fuscata in Arashiyama B troop: Transmission of a non-adaptive behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 13(8), 725–735. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2484(84)80022-6
  • Huffman, M., & Quiatt, D. (1986). Stone handling by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): Implications for tool use of stone. Primates, 27(4), 413–423. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02381887
  • Leca, J.-B. (2020a). Evolutionary by-products. In T. K. Shackelford, & V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science. Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2850-1.
  • Leca, J.-B. (2020b). By-products of adaptations. In T. K. Shackelford, & V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science. Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_599-1.
  • Leca, J.-B., Gunst, N., & Huffman, M. A. (2007a). Japanese macaque cultures: Inter- and intra-troop behavioural variability of stone handling patterns across 10 troops. Behaviour, 144(3), 251–281. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853907780425712
  • Leca, J.-B., Gunst, N., & Huffman, M. A. (2007b). Age-related differences in the performance, diffusion, and maintenance of stone handling, a behavioral tradition in Japanese macaques. Journal of Human Evolution, 53(6), 691–708. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.05.009
  • Leca, J.-B., Gunst, N., & Huffman, M. A. (2008a). Food provisioning and stone handling tradition in Japanese macaques: a comparative study of ten troops. American Journal of Primatology, 70(8), 803–813. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20551
  • Leca, J.-B., Gunst, N., & Huffman, M. A. (2010). Indirect social influence in the maintenance of the stone-handling tradition in Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata. Animal Behaviour, 79(1), 117–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.035
  • Leca, J.-B., Gunst, N., & Huffman, M. A. (2011). Complexity in object manipulation by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): a cross-sectional analysis of manual coordination in stone handling patterns. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125(1), 61–71. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020868
  • Leca, J.-B., Gunst, N., & Huffman, M. A. (2012). Thirty years of stone handling tradition in Arashiyama-Kyoto macaques: Implications for cumulative culture and tool use in non-human primates. In J.-B. Leca, M. A. Huffman, & P. L. Vasey (Eds.), The monkeys of stormy mountain: 60 years of primatological research on the Japanese macaques of Arashiyama (pp. 223–257). Cambridge University Press.
  • Leca, J.-B., Nahallage, C., Gunst, N., & Huffman, M. A. (2008). Stone-throwing by Japanese macaques: Form and functional aspects of a group-specific behavioral tradition. Journal of Human Evolution, 55(6), 989–998. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.06.004
  • Linde-Medina, M. (2017). A taxonomy of non-fitness. Biological Theory, 12(1), 1–3. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-016-0255-1
  • Lockman, J. (2000). A perception-action perspective on tool use development. Child Development, 71(1), 137–144. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00127
  • Lorenz, K. (1971). Studies in animal and human behaviour (Volume II). Harvard University Press.
  • Martin, P. (1984). The (four) whys and wherefores of cat play: A review of functional, evolutionary, developmental and causal issues. In P. K. Smith (Ed.), Play in animals and humans (pp. 71–94). Blackwell.
  • Martin, P., & Caro, T. M. (1985). Advances in the study of behavior. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 15, 59–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60487-8
  • Miller, S. (1973). Ends, means, and galumphing: Some leitmotifs of play. American Anthropologist, 75(1), 87–98. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1973.75.1.02a00050
  • Montgomery, S. (2014). The relationship between play, brain growth, and behavioural flexibility in primates. Animal Behaviour, 90, 281–286. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.004
  • Müller-Schwarze, D. (1978). Evolution of play behavior. Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, Inc.
  • Nahallage, C., Leca, J.-B., & Huffman, M. (2016). Stone handling, an object play behaviour in macaques: Welfare and neurological health implications of a bio-culturally driven tradition. Behaviour, 153(6-7), 845–869. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003361
  • Nakagawa, N., Matsubara, M., Shimooka, Y., & Nishikawa, M. (2015). Embracing in a wild group of Yakushima macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) as an example of social customs. Current Anthropology, 56(1), 104–120. https://doi.org/10.1086/679448
  • Pagel, M., & Harvey, P. (1993). Evolution of the juvenile period in mammals. In M. Pereira, & L. Fairbanks (Eds.), Juvenile primates: Life history, development, and behavior (pp. 28–37). Oxford University Press.
  • Parker, S., & Gibson, K. (1977). Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in Cebus monkeys and great apes. Journal of Human Evolution, 6(7), 623–641. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2484(77)80135-8
  • Pellegrini, A., Horvat, M., & Huberty, P. (1998). The relative cost of children’s physical play. Animal Behaviour, 55(4), 1053–1061. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0658
  • Pelletier, A., Kaufmann, T., Mohak, S., Milan, R., Nahallage, C., Huffman, M., Gunst, N., Rompis, A., Wandia, N., Arta Putra, G., Pellis, S., & Leca, J.-B. (2017). Behavior systems approach to object play: Stone handling repertoire as a measure of propensity for complex foraging and percussive tool use in the genus Macaca. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 4(4), 455–473. https://doi.org/10.26451/abc.04.04.05.201
  • Pellis, S., Burghardt, G., Palagi, E., & Mangel, M. (2015). Modeling play: Distinguishing between origins and current functions. Adaptive Behavior, 23(6), 331–339. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712315596053
  • Pellis, S., Pellis, V., Pelletier, A., & Leca, J.-B. (2019). Is play a behavior system, and, if so, what kind? Behavioural Processes, 160, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.12.011
  • Power, T. (2000). Play and exploration in children and animals. Erlbaum.
  • Ridley, M. (2004). Evolution. Blackwell Publishing.
  • Schiller, P. (1952). Innate constituents of complex responses in primates. Psychological Review, 59(3), 177–191. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0062854
  • Sharpe, L. (2019). Fun, fur, and future fitness: The evolution of play in mammals. In P. Smith, & J. Roopnarine (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of play: Developmental and disciplinary perspectives (pp. 49–66). Cambridge University Press.
  • Sharpe, L., Clutton-Brock, T., Brotherton, P., Cameron, E., & Cherry, M. (2002). Experimental provisioning increases play in free-ranging meerkats. Animal Behaviour, 64(1), 113–121. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.3031
  • Siviy, S., & Atrens, D. (1992). The energetic costs of rough-and-tumble play in the juvenile rat. Developmental Psychobiology, 25(2), 137–148. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420250206
  • Smaldino, P., Palagi, E., Burghardt, G., & Pellis, S. (2019). The evolution of two types of play. Behavioral Ecology, 30(5), 1388–1397. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arz090
  • Smith, P. (2010). Children and play. Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Sutton-Smith, B. (1997). The ambiguity of play. Harvard University Press.
  • Sylva, K. (1977). Play and learning. In B. Tizard, & D. Harvey (Eds.), Biology of play (pp. 59–73). SIMP/Heinemann.
  • Sylva, K., Bruner, J., & Genova, P. (1976). The role of play in the problem-solving of children 3–5 years old. In J. Bruner, A. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution (pp. 244–257). Penguin.
  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2405–2415. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0052
  • Vanderschuren, L. (2010). How the brain makes play fun. American Journal of Play, 2(3), 315–337.
  • Vasey, P. (2007). Function and phylogeny: The evolution of same-sex sexual behaviour in primates. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 18(2-3), 215–244. https://doi.org/10.1300/J056v18n02_07
  • Williams, G. (1992). Natural selection: Domains, levels, and challenges. Oxford University Press.
  • Zedelius, C., & Schooler, J. (2020). Capturing the dynamics of creative daydreaming. In D. Preiss, D. Cosmelli, & J. Kaufman (Eds.), Creativity and the wandering mind: Spontaneous and controlled cognition (pp. 55–72). Academic Press.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.