591
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REGULAR ARTICLES

Speech-in-noise perception is linked to rhythm production skills in adult percussionists and non-musicians

, , , , &
Pages 710-717 | Received 18 Nov 2016, Accepted 25 Oct 2017, Published online: 12 Dec 2017

References

  • Abercrombie, D. (1967). Elements of general phonetics. Chicago, IL: Aldine Publishing Company.
  • Alain, C., & Du, Y. (2015). Recruitment of the speech motor system in adverse listening conditions. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 137(4), 2211–2211. doi: 10.1121/1.4920049
  • Anderson, S., White-Schwoch, T., Parbery-Clark, A., & Kraus, N. (2013). A dynamic auditory-cognitive system supports speech-in-noise perception in older adults. Hearing Research, 300, 18–32. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.03.006
  • Andreou, L.-V., Kashino, M., & Chait, M. (2011). The role of temporal regularity in auditory segregation. Hearing Research, 280(1), 228–235. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2011.06.001
  • Ashley, R. (2002). Do [n’t] change a hair for me: The art of jazz rubato. Music Perception, 19(3), 311–332. doi: 10.1525/mp.2002.19.3.311
  • Boebinger, D., Evans, S., Rosen, S., Lima, C. F., Manly, T., & Scott, S. K. (2015). Musicians and non-musicians are equally adept at perceiving masked speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 137(1), 378–387. doi: 10.1121/1.4904537
  • Brown, L., Sherbenou, R., & Johnsen, S. K. (1997). Test of nonverbal intelligence. A language free measure of cognitive ability. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
  • Cameron, D. J., & Grahn, J. A. (2014). Enhanced timing abilities in percussionists generalize to rhythms without a musical beat. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 226. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01003
  • Choi, J. Y., Hasegawa-Johnson, M., & Cole, J. (2005). Finding intonational boundaries using acoustic cues related to the voice source. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118(4), 2579–2587. doi: 10.1121/1.2010288
  • Cross, I. (1999). Is music the most important thing we ever did? Music, development and evolution. In Yi Suk Won (Ed.), Music, Mind and Science (Vol. 29, pp. 10–39). Seoul: Seoul National University Press.
  • Dalla Bella, S., Bialunska, A., & Sowinski, J. (2013). Why movement is captured by music, but less by speech: Role of temporal regularity. PLoS One, 8(8), e71945. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071945
  • Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalyzed. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 51–62.
  • Ding, N., Melloni, L., Zhang, H., Tian, X., & Poeppel, D. (2016). Cortical tracking of hierarchical linguistic structures in connected speech. Nature Neuroscience, 19(1), 158–164. doi: 10.1038/nn.4186
  • Ehrlé, N., & Samson, S. (2005). Auditory discrimination of anisochrony: Influence of the tempo and musical backgrounds of listeners. Brain and Cognition, 58(1), 133–147. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.014
  • Fear, B. D., Cutler, A., & Butterfield, S. (1995). The strong/weak syllable distinction in English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97(3), 1893–1904. doi: 10.1121/1.412063
  • Festen, J. M., & Plomp, R. (1990). Effects of fluctuating noise and interfering speech on the speech-reception threshold for impaired and normal hearing. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 88(4), 1725–1736. doi: 10.1121/1.400247
  • Gordon, R. L., Shivers, C. M., Wieland, E. A., Kotz, S. A., Yoder, P. J., & Devin McAuley, J. (2015). Musical rhythm discrimination explains individual differences in grammar skills in children. Developmental Science, 18(4), 635–644. doi: 10.1111/desc.12230
  • Graybiel, A. M. (1997). The basal ganglia and cognitive pattern generators. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23(3), 459–469. doi: 10.1093/schbul/23.3.459
  • Herdener, M., Humbel, T., Esposito, F., Habermeyer, B., Cattapan-Ludewig, K., & Seifritz, E. (2014). Jazz drummers recruit language-specific areas for the processing of rhythmic structure. Cerebral Cortex, 24(3), 836–843. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs367
  • Iversen, J. R., & Patel, A. D. (2008). The beat alignment test (BAT): surveying beat processing abilities in the general population. In K. Miyazaki (Ed.), 10th International conference on music perception and cognition (pp. 465–468). Adelaide, SA: Causal Productions.
  • Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(2), 136–152. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1989.1.2.136
  • Killion, M. C., Niquette, P. A., Gudmundsen, G. I., Revit, L. J., & Banerjee, S. (2004). Development of a quick speech-in-noise test for measuring signal-to-noise ratio loss in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116(4 Pt 1), 2395–2405. doi: 10.1121/1.1784440
  • Klatt, D. H. (1976). Linguistic uses of segmental duration in English: Acoustic and perceptual evidence. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 59(5), 1208–1221. doi: 10.1121/1.380986
  • Kotz, S. A., Schwartze, M., & Schmidt-Kassow, M. (2009). Non-motor basal ganglia functions: A review and proposal for a model of sensory predictability in auditory language perception. Cortex, 45(8), 982–990. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.010
  • Krause, V., Schnitzler, A., & Pollok, B. (2010). Functional network interactions during sensorimotor synchronization in musicians and non-musicians. Neuroimage, 52(1), 245–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.081
  • Lehiste, I. (1977). Isochrony reconsidered. Journal Phonetics, 5, 253–263.
  • Liberman, M., & Prince, A. (1977). On stress and linguistic rhythm. Linguistic Inquiry, 8(2), 249–336.
  • Manning, F. C., & Schutz, M. (2016). Trained to keep a beat: Movement-related enhancements to timing perception in percussionists and non-percussionists. Psychological Research, 80(4), 532–542. doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0678-5
  • Matthews, T. E., Thibodeau, J. N., Gunther, B. P., & Penhune, V. B. (2016). The impact of instrument-specific musical training on rhythm perception and production. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 23. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00069
  • Mattys, S. L., Davis, M. H., Bradlow, A. R., & Scott, S. K. (2012). Speech recognition in adverse conditions: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(7–8), 953–978. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2012.705006
  • Palmer, C. (1997). Music performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 48(1), 115–138. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.115
  • Parbery-Clark, A., Lam, C., & Kraus, N. (2009). Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear and Hearing, 30(6), 653–661. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181b412e9
  • Parbery-Clark, A., Tierney, A., Strait, D. L., & Kraus, N. (2012). Musicians have fine-tuned neural distinction of speech syllables. Neuroscience, 219, 111–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.05.042
  • Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, language, and the brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Patel, A. D. (2011). Why would musical training benefit the neural encoding of speech? The OPERA hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 142. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00142
  • Pichora-Fuller, M. K., Schneider, B. A., & Daneman, M. (1995). How young and old adults listen to and remember speech in noise. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97(1), 593–608. doi: 10.1121/1.412282
  • Povel, D.-J., & Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2(4), 411–440. doi: 10.2307/40285311
  • Rammsayer, T., & Altenmüller, E. (2006). Temporal information processing in musicians and nonmusicians. Music Perception, 24(1), 37–48. doi: 10.1525/mp.2006.24.1.37
  • Repp, B. H. (1992). Diversity and commonality in music performance: An analysis of timing microstructure in Schumann’s “Traumerei”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 92(5), 2546–2568. doi: 10.1121/1.404425
  • Repp, B. H. (1995). Expressive timing in Schumann’s “‘Träumerei:”’An analysis of performances by graduate student pianists. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98(5), 2413–2427. doi: 10.1121/1.413276
  • Ruggles, D. R., Freyman, R. L., & Oxenham, A. J. (2014). Influence of musical training on understanding voiced and whispered speech in noise. PLoS One, 9(1), e86980. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086980
  • Salvi, R. J., Lockwood, A. H., Frisina, R. D., Coad, M. L., Wack, D. S., & Frisina, D. R. (2002). PET imaging of the normal human auditory system: Responses to speech in quiet and in background noise. Hearing Research, 170(1–2), 96–106. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(02)00386-6
  • Schmidt-Kassow, M., & Kotz, S. A. (2008). Entrainment of syntactic processing? ERP-responses to predictable time intervals during syntactic reanalysis. Brain Research, 1226, 144–155. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.06.017
  • Scott, D. R. (1982). Duration as a cue to the perception of a phrase boundary. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 71(4), 996–1007. doi: 10.1121/1.387581
  • Shamma, S. A., Elhilali, M., & Micheyl, C. (2011). Temporal coherence and attention in auditory scene analysis. Trends in Neurosciences, 34(3), 114–123. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2010.11.002
  • Slater, J., & Kraus, N. (2016). The role of rhythm in perceiving speech in noise: A comparison of percussionists, vocalists and non-musicians. Cognitive Processing, 17(1), 79–87. doi: 10.1007/s10339-015-0740-7
  • Slater, J., Tierney, A., & Kraus, N. (2013). At-risk elementary school children with one year of classroom music instruction are better at keeping a beat. PLoS One, 8(10), e77250. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077250
  • Smith, M. R., Cutler, A., Butterfield, S., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1989). The perception of rhythm and word boundaries in noise-masked speech. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 32(4), 912–920. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3204.912
  • Strait, D. L., & Kraus, N. (2011). Can you hear me now? Musical training shapes functional brain networks for selective auditory attention and hearing speech in noise. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 113. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00113
  • Swaminathan, J., Mason, C. R., Streeter, T. M., KiddJr, G., & Patel, A. D. (2014). Spatial release from masking in musicians and non-musicians. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 135(4), 2281–2282. doi: 10.1121/1.4877482
  • Teki, S., Grube, M., Kumar, S., & Griffiths, T. D. (2011). Distinct neural substrates of duration-based and beat-based auditory timing. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(10), 3805–3812. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5561-10.2011
  • Thompson, E. C., White-Schwoch, T., Tierney, A., & Kraus, N. (2015). Beat synchronization across the lifespan: Intersection of development and musical experience. PLoS One, 10(6), e0128839. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128839
  • Tierney, A., & Kraus, N. (2015). Evidence for multiple rhythmic skills. PLoS One, 10(9), e0136645. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136645
  • Turk, A. E., & Sawusch, J. R. (1997). The domain of accentual lengthening in American English. Journal of Phonetics, 25(1), 25–41. doi: 10.1006/jpho.1996.0032
  • Vuust, P., Pallesen, K. J., Bailey, C., van Zuijen, T. L., Gjedde, A., Roepstorff, A., & Østergaard, L. (2005). To musicians, the message is in the meter: Pre-attentive neuronal responses to incongruent rhythm are left-lateralized in musicians. Neuroimage, 24(2), 560–564. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.039
  • Vuust, P., Roepstorff, A., Wallentin, M., Mouridsen, K., & Østergaard, L. (2006). It don’t mean a thing … : keeping the rhythm during polyrhythmic tension, activates language areas (BA47). Neuroimage, 31(2), 832–841. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.037

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.