1,909
Views
11
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular Articles

Predicting turn-ends in discourse context

, &
Pages 615-627 | Received 11 May 2018, Accepted 15 Nov 2018, Published online: 03 Dec 2018

References

  • Alegre, M., Imirizaldu, L., Valencia, M., Iriarte, J., Archocha, J., & Artieda, J. (2006). Alpha and beta changes in cortical oscillatory activity in a go/no go randomly-delayed-response choice reaction time paradigm. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117, 16–25. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.08.030
  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modelling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
  • Bates, D. M., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using “Eigein” and S4 (R package version 1.1-12). Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
  • Bögels, S., & Levinson, S. C. (2017). The brain behind the response: Insights into turn-taking in conversation from neuroimaging. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 50, 71–89. doi: 10.1080/08351813.2017.1262118
  • Bögels, S., & Torreira, F. (2015). Listeners use intonational phrase boundaries to project turn ends in spoken interaction. Journal of Phonetics, 52, 46–57. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2015.04.004
  • Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Cleland, A. A. (2000). Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition, 75, B13–B25. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00081-5
  • Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). Brms: Bayesian regression models using Stan (R package version 1.6.1). Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=brms
  • Cook, A. E., & Meyer, S. (2008). Capacity demands of phoneme selection in word production: New evidence from dual-task experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 886–899.
  • Corps, R. E., Crossley, A., Gambi, C., & Pickering, M. J. (2018). Early preparation during turn-taking: Listeners use content predictions to determine what to say but not when to say it. Cognition, 175, 77–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.015
  • Corps, R. E., Gambi, C., & Pickering, M. J. (2018). Coordinating utterances during turn-taking: The role of prediction, response preparation, and articulation. Discourse Processes, 55, 230–240. doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2017.1330031
  • Dell, G. S., & Chang, F. (2014). The P-chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0394
  • De Ruiter, J. P., Mitterer, H., & Enfield, N. J. (2006). Projecting the end of a speaker’s turn: A cognitive cornerstone of conversation. Language, 82, 515–535. doi: 10.1353/lan.2006.0130
  • Dienes, Z. (2014). Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00781
  • Drake, E., & Corley, M. (2015). Effects in production of word pre-activation during listening: Are listener-generate predictions specified at a speech-sound level? Memory & Cognition, 43, 111–120. doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0451-9
  • Gambi, C., & Pickering, M. J. (2016). Predicting and imagining language. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31, 60–72. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2015.1049188
  • Garrod, S., & Anderson, A. (1987). Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition, 27, 181–218. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90018-7
  • Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2015). The use of content and timing to predict turn transitions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00751
  • Huettig, F. (2015). Four central questions about prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1626, 118–135. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.014
  • Indefrey, P., & Levelt, W. J. (2004). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition, 92, 101–144. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001
  • Inhoff, A. W., Solomon, M., Radach, R., & Seymour, B. (2011). Temporal dynamics of the eye voice span and eye movement control during oral reading. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 543–558. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.546782
  • Laubrock, J., & Kliegel, R. (2015). The eye-voice span during reading aloud. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01432
  • Leone, F. C., Nelson, L. S., & Nottingham, R. B. (1961). The folded normal distribution. Technometrics, 3, 543–550. doi: 10.1080/00401706.1961.10489974
  • Levinson, S. C., & Torreira, F. (2015). Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00731
  • Magyari, L., Bastiaansen, M. C. M., De Ruiter, J. P., & Levinson, S. C. (2014). Early anticipation lies behind speed of response in conversation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 2530–2539. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00673
  • Magyari, L., & De Ruiter, J. P. (2012). Prediction of turn-ends based on anticipation of upcoming words. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00376
  • Magyari, L., De Ruiter, J. P., & Levinson, S. C. (2017). Temporal preparation for speaking in question-answer sequences. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00211
  • Martin, C. D., Branzi, F. M., & Bar, M. (2018). Prediction is production: The missing link between language production and comprehension. Scientific Reports, 8. doi: 10.1038//s41598-018-19499-4
  • Pickering, M. J., & Gambi, C. (2018). Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 1002–1044. doi: 10.1037/bul0000158
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169–190.
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 329–347. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12001495
  • Riest, C., Jorschick, A. B., & De Ruiter, J. P. (2015). Anticipation in turn-taking: Mechanisms and information sources. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00089
  • Rommers, J., & Federmeier, D. (2018). Predictability’s aftermath: Downstream consequences of word predictability as revealed by repetition effects. Cortex, 101, 16–30. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.12.018
  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696–735. doi: 10.1353/lan.1974.0010
  • Sanders, A. F. (1966). Expectancy: Application and measurement. Acta Psychologica, 25, 293–313. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(66)90013-8
  • Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27, 379–423. doi: 10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x
  • Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Engelert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., … Levinson, S. (2009). Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 10587–10592. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903616106
  • Taylor, W. L. (1953). “Cloze procedure”: A new tool for measuring readability. Journalism Quarterly, 30, 415–433.
  • Van Berkum, J. J. (2010). The brain is a prediction machine that cares about good and bad – Any implications for neuropragmatics? Italian Journal of Linguistics, 22, 181–208.
  • Van Berkum, J. J., Brown, C. M., Zwisterlood, P., Koojiman, V., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Anticipating upcoming words in discourse: Evidence from ERPs during reading times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 443–467.
  • Weiss, S., & Mueller, H. M. (2012). “Too many beta do not spoil the broth”: The role of beta brain oscillations in language processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00201