2,419
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Introduction

Thirty years of Speaking: An introduction to the Special Issue

, &
Pages 1073-1084 | Received 24 Jul 2019, Accepted 29 Jul 2019, Published online: 12 Aug 2019

References

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
  • Bangerter, A., & Clark, H. H. (2003). Navigating joint projects with dialogue. Cognitive Science, 27, 195–225. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2702_3
  • Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 355–387. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(86)90004-6
  • Bock, K., & Miller, C. A. (1991). Broken agreement. Cognitive Psychology, 23(1), 45–93. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(91)90003-7
  • Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(6), 1482–1493. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.22.6.1482
  • Bürki, A., Pellet Cheneval, P., & Laganaro, M. (2015). Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary? ERP comparison of high frequency and novel syllable production. Brain and Language, 150, 90–102. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.08.006
  • Caramazza, A. (1997). How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(1), 177–208. doi: 10.1080/026432997381664
  • Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113(2), 234–272. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.234
  • Cholin, J., Dell, G. S., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2011). Planning and articulation in incremental word production: Syllable-frequency effects in English. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(1), 109–122. doi: 10.1037/a0021322
  • Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(4), 335–359. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80014-3
  • Clark, H. H., & Krych, M. A. (2004). Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(1), 62–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2003.08.004
  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755
  • Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 491–511. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.002
  • de Groot, A. M., & Hagoort, P. (Eds.). (2018). Research methods in psycholinguistics and the neurobiology of language: A practical guide (Vol. 9). John Wiley & Sons.
  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93(3), 283–321. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
  • Dell, G. S., & Ferreira, V. S. (Eds.). (2016). Thirty years of structural priming: An introduction to the special issue. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.05.005
  • Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104(4), 801–838. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.104.4.801
  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
  • de Zubicaray, G. I., & Piai, V. (2019). Investigating the spatial and temporal components of speech production. In G. I. de Zubicaray & N. O. Schiller (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Neurolinguistics (pp. 471–497). Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190672027.013.19
  • Eberhard, K. M., Cutting, J. C., & Bock, K. (2005). Making syntax of sense: Number agreement in sentence production. Psychological Review, 112(3), 531–559. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.3.531
  • Frank, M. C., & Goodman, N. D. (2012). Predicting pragmatic reasoning in language games. Science, 336(6084), 998–998. doi: 10.1126/science.1218633
  • Garrett, M. (1990). Review of Levelt: Speaking. Language and Speech, 33, 273–291. doi: 10.1177/002383099003300303
  • Glaser, W. R., & Düngelhoff, F. J. (1984). The time course of picture-word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 640–654. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.640
  • Hartshorne, J. K., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Pinker, S. (2018). A critical period for second language acquisition: Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers. Cognition, 177, 263–277. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.007
  • Indefrey, P. (2012). Hemodynamic studies of syntactic processing. In M. Faust (Ed.), The Handbook of the Neuropsychology of language (Vol. 1. pp. 209–228). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. doi: 10.1002/9781118432501.ch11
  • Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434–446. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007
  • Jaeger, T. F. (2010). Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density. Cognitive Psychology, 61(1), 23–62. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.02.002
  • Keating, P., & Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2002). A prosodic view of word form encoding for speech production. UCLA working papers in phonetics, 112–156.
  • Kemmerer, D. (2014). Cognitive neuroscience of language. New York: Psychology Press.
  • Konopka, A. E. (2012). Planning ahead: How recent experience with structures and words changes the scope of linguistic planning. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(1), 143–162. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2011.08.003
  • Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). Priming sentence planning. Cognitive Psychology, 73, 1–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.04.001
  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205. doi: 10.1126/science.7350657
  • Levelt, W. J. (1981). The speaker’s linearization problem. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences, 295(1077), 305–315. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1981.0142
  • Levelt, W. J. (1983). Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition, 14(1), 41–104. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90026-4
  • Levelt, W. J. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
  • Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–38. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99001776
  • Levinson, S. C., & Torreira, F. (2015). Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 731. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00731
  • MacDonald, M. C. (2013). How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 226. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00226
  • Mahowald, K., James, A., Futrell, R., & Gibson, E. (2016). A meta-analysis of syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 5–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.03.009
  • Meyer, A. S., Huettig, F., & Levelt, W.J. (Ed.) (2016). Same, different, or closely related: What is the relationship between language production and comprehension? [Special issue]. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 1-7. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.03.002
  • Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17(4), 273–281. doi: 10.1080/17470216508416445
  • Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M., & Raichle, M. E. (1988). Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing. Nature, 331(6157), 585–589. doi: 10.1038/331585a0
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(2), 169–190. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04000056
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–347. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12001495
  • Posner, M. I. (2012). Attention in a social world. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199791217.001.0001
  • Rapp, B., & Goldrick, M. (2000). Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychological Review, 107(3), 460–499. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.460
  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372
  • Roelofs, A. (1992). A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition, 42, 107–142. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90041-F
  • Roelofs, A. (2003). Goal-referenced selection of verbal action: Modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. Psychological Review, 110(1), 88. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.88
  • Roelofs, A. (2014). A dorsal-pathway account of aphasic language production: The WEAVER++/ARC model. Cortex, 59, 33–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.001
  • Roelofs, A., & Ferreira, V. S. (2019). The architecture of speaking. In P. Hagoort (Ed.), Human language: From genes and brains to behavior (pp. 35–50). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Roelofs, A., & Piai, V. (2011). Attention demands of spoken word planning: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 307. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00307
  • Schriefers, H., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. (1990). Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language, 29(1), 86–102. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(90)90011-N
  • Schwartz, M. F., & Dell, G. S. (2010). Case series investigations in cognitive neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27(6), 477–494. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2011.574111
  • Slobin, D. I. (1996). From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking”. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Smith, M., & Wheeldon, L. (1999). High level processing scope in spoken sentence production. Cognition, 73(3), 205–246. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00053-0
  • van Turennout, M., Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1998). Brain activity during speaking: From syntax to phonology in 40 milliseconds. Science, 280(5363), 572–574. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5363.572
  • Vasishth, S., Nicenboim, B., Beckman, M. E., Li, F., & Kong, E. J. (2018). Bayesian data analysis in the phonetic sciences: A tutorial introduction. Journal of Phonetics, 71, 147–161. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2018.07.008
  • Wilkes-Gibbs, D., & Clark, H. H. (1992). Coordinating beliefs in conversation. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(2), 183–194. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(92)90010-U

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.