407
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular Articles

When does lexical availability influence phonology? Evidence from Jargon reading and repetition

, , &
Pages 521-540 | Received 11 Feb 2019, Accepted 15 Sep 2019, Published online: 21 Oct 2019

References

  • Ackerman, T., & Ellis, A. W. (2007). Case study: Where do aphasic perseverations come from? Aphasiology, 21(10–11), 1018–1038. doi: 10.1080/02687030701198361
  • Adlam, A. L., Patterson, K., Bozeat, S., & Hodges, J. R. (2010). The Cambridge semantic memory test battery: Detection of semantic deficits in semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurocase, 16(3), 193–207. doi: 10.1080/13554790903405693
  • Alajouanine, T. (1956). Verbal realisation in aphasia. Brain, 79, 1–28.
  • Baars, B. J., Motley, M. T., & MacKay, D. G. (1975). Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Verbal Learning and Behaviour, 14, 382–391.
  • Balota, D., Yap, M. J., Hutchinson, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., … Treiman, R. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459. doi: 10.3758/BF03193014
  • Baron, J., & Strawson, C. (1976). Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2(3), 386–393. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.2.3.386
  • Bayles, K. A., Tomoeda, C. K., Kaszniak, A. W., Stern, L. Z., & Eagans, K. K. (1985). Verbal perseveration of dementia patients. Brain and Language, 25(1), 102–116.
  • Binder, J. R. (2017). Current controversies on Wernicke’s area and its role in language. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 17(8), 58. doi: 10.1007/s11910-017-0764-8
  • Blumstein, S. E., Cooper, W. E., Goodglass, H., Statlender, S., & Gottlieb, J. (1980). Production deficits in aphasia: A voice-onset time analysis. Brain and Language, 9(2), 153–170. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(80)90137-6
  • Bose, A. (2013). Phonological therapy in jargon aphasia: Effects on naming and neologisms. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48(5), 582–595.
  • Bose, A., van Lieshout, P., & Squares, P. A. (2007). Word frequency and bigram frequency effects on linguistic processing and speech motor performance in individuals with aphasia and normal speakers. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(1), 65–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.05.001
  • Buchsbaum, B. R., Baldo, J., Okada, K., Berman, K. F., Dronkers, N., D’Esposito, M., & Hickok, G. (2011). Conduction aphasia, sensory-motor integration, and phonological short-term memory – an aggregate analysis of lesion and fMRI data. Brain and Language, 119(3), 119–128. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.12.001
  • Buchsbaum, B. R., Hickok, G., & Humphries, C. (2001). Role of left posterior superior temporal gyrus in phonological processing for speech perception and production. Cognitive Science, 25, 663–678. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2505_2
  • Buckingham, H. W. (1977). The conduction theory and neologistic Jargon. Language and Speech, 20(2), 174–184. doi: 10.1177/002383097702000209
  • Buckingham, H. W. (1990). Abstruse neologisms, retrieval deficits and the random generator. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 5(2–3), 215–235. doi:10.1016/0911-6044(90)90012-N
  • Buckingham, H. W., & Buckingham, S. S. (2011). Is recurrent perseveration a product of deafferented functional systems with otherwise normal post-activation decay rates? Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 25(11–12), 1066–1073. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2011.616982
  • BuckinghamJrH. W., & Kertesz, A. (1974). A linguistic analysis of fluent aphasia. Brain and Language, 1(1), 43–61. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(74)90025-X
  • Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (1998). Competition between past and present. Assessment and interpretation of verbal perseverations. Brain, 121(9), 1641–1659.
  • Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Specialization within the ventral stream: The case for the visual word form area. Neuroimage, 22(1), 466–476. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.049
  • Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Vinckier, F., Jobert, A., & Montavont, A. (2008). Reading normal and degraded words: Contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways. Neuroimage, 40, 353–366.
  • Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 33(4), 497–505. doi: 10.1080/14640748108400805
  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed- processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100(4), 589–608.
  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204–256.
  • Corbett, F., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2008). The use of cueing to alleviate recurrent verbal perseverations: Evidence from transcortical sensory aphasia. Aphasiology, 22(4), 363–382. doi: 10.1080/02687030701415245
  • Crisp, J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Unlocking the nature of the phonological-deep dyslexia continuum: The keys to reading aloud are in phonology and semantics. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(3), 348–362. doi: 10.1162/089892906775990543
  • Dell, G. S., Martin, N., & Schwartz, M. F. (2007). A case-series test of the interactive two-step model of lexical access: Predicting word repetition from picture naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(4), 490–520. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.05.007
  • Dell, G. S., & Reich, P. A. (1981). Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 20, 611–629.
  • Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104(4), 801–838.
  • Demeyere, N., Riddoch, M. J., Slavkova, E. D., Bickerton, W.-L., & Humphreys, G. W. (2015). The Oxford Cognitive Screen (OCS): Validation of a stroke-specific short cognitive screening tool. Psychological Assessment, 27(3), 883–894.
  • Eaton, E., Marshall, J., & Pring, T. (2011). Mechanisms of change in the evolution of Jargon aphasia. Aphasiology, 25(12), 1543–1561. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2011.624584
  • Fischer-Baum, S., & Rapp, B. (2012). Underlying cause(s) of letter perseveration errors. Neuropsychologia, 50(2), 305–318. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.001
  • Foygel, D., & Dell, G. S. (2000). Models of impaired lexical access in speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(2), 182–216. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2716
  • Frankel, T., Penn, C., & Ormond-Brown, D. (2007). Executive dysfunction as an explanatory basis for conversation symptoms of aphasia: A pilot study. Aphasiology, 21(6–8), 814–828.
  • Gerhand, S., & Barry, C. (1998). Word frequency effects in oral reading are not merely age-of-acquisition effects in disguise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(2), 267–283. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.24.2.267
  • Godbold, C. (2016). Non-word errors in Jargon aphasia: Exploring the underlying mechanisms. (Thesis PhD.), University of Reading, School of Psychology & Clinical Language Sciences.
  • Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., & Barresi, B. (2001). The Boston diagnostic aphasia examination. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott.
  • Gotts, S. J., della Rocchetta, A. I., & Cipolotti, L. (2002). Mechanisms underlying perseveration in aphasia: Evidence from a single case study. Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 1930–1947. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00067-2
  • Grainger, J., & Segui, J. (1990). Neighborhood frequency effects in visual word recognition: A comparison of lexical decision and masked identification latencies. Perception & Psychophysics, 47(2), 191–198. doi: 10.3758/BF03205983
  • Halpern, H. (1965). Effect of stimulus variables on verbal perseveration of dysphasic subjects. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 20(2), 421–429. doi: 10.2466/pms.1965.20.2.421
  • Hanley, J. R., & Kay, J. R. (1997). An effect of imageability on the production of phonological errors in auditory repetition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(8), 1065–1084. doi: 10.1080/026432997381277
  • Hanley, J. R., Kay, J., & Edwards, M. (2002). Imageability effects, phonological errors, and the relationship between auditory repetition and picture naming: Implications for models of auditory repetition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19(3), 193–206. doi: 10.1080/02643290143000132
  • Hillis, A. E. (2007). Aphasia: Progress in the last quarter of a century. Neurology, 69(2), 200.
  • Hillis, A. E., Boatman, D., Hart, J., & Gordon, B. (1999). Making sense out of Jargon: A neurolinguistic and computational account of Jargon aphasia. Neurology, 53(8), 1813.
  • Hirsh, K. W. (1998). Perseveration and activation in aphasic speech production. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15(4), 377–388. doi: 10.1080/026432998381140
  • Hoffman, P., Jefferies, B., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). Remembering ‘zeal’ but not ‘thing’: Reverse frequency effects as a consequence of deregulated semantic processing. Neuropsychologia, 49(3), 580–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.036
  • Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Schweickert, R., Brown, G. D., Martin, M., & Stuart, G. (1997). Word-frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: Evidence for a redintegration process in immediate serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(5), 1217–1232.
  • Jefferies, E., Crisp, J., & Ralph, M. A. L. (2006). The impact of phonological or semantic impairment on delayed auditory repetition: Evidence from stroke aphasia and semantic dementia. Aphasiology, 20(9), 963–992. doi: 10.1080/02687030600739398
  • Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., Jones, R. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2009). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in semantic dementia. Neuropsychology, 23(4), 492–499. doi: 10.1037/a0015452
  • Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1996). Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia (PALPA): An introduction. Aphasiology, 10(2), 159–180. doi: 10.1080/02687039608248403
  • Kertesz, A. (1981). Chapter 4 – the anatomy of Jargon A2. In J. W. Brown (Ed.), Jargona phasia (pp. 63–112). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Kertesz, A., & Benson, D. F. (1970). Neologistic Jargon: A clinicopathological study. Cortex, 6(4), 362–386. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(70)80002-8
  • Kinsbourne, M., & Warrington, E. (1963). Jargon aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 1(1), 27–37. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(63)90010-1
  • Kittredge, A. K., Dell, G. S., Verkuilen, J., & Schwartz, M. F. (2008). Where is the effect of frequency in word production? Insights from aphasic picture-naming errors. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25(4), 463–492. doi: 10.1080/02643290701674851
  • Kohn, S. E., Smith, K. L., & Alexander, M. P. (1996). Differential recovery from impairment to the phonological lexicon. Brain and Language, 52(1), 129–149. doi: 10.1006/brln.1996.0007
  • Laszlo, S., & Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Better the DVL you know: Acronyms reveal the contribution of familiarity to single-word reading. Psychological Science, 18(2), 122–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01859.x
  • Marshall, J. (2006). Jargon aphasia: What have we learned? Aphasiology, 20(5), 387–410. doi: 10.1080/02687030500489946
  • Marshall, J., Chiat, S., Robson, J., & Pring, T. (1996). Calling a salad a federation: An investigation of semantic Jargon. Paper 2, verbs. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 9, 251–260.
  • Marshall, J., Pring, T., Chiat, S., & Robson, J. (2001). When ottoman is easier than chair: An inverse frequency effect in Jargon aphasia. Cortex, 37(1), 33–53. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70556-2
  • Marshall, J., Robson, J., Pring, T., & Chiat, S. (1998). Why does monitoring fail in Jargon aphasia? Comprehension, judgement and therapy evidence. Brain and Language, 63(1), 79–107. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1936
  • Martin, N., & Dell, G. S. (2007). Common mechanisms underlying perseverative and non-perseverative sound and word substitutions. Aphasiology, 21(10–11), 1002–1017. doi: 10.1080/02687030701198346
  • Martin, N., & Saffran, E. M. (1997). Language and auditory-verbal short-term memory impairments: Evidence for common underlying processes. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(5), 641–682. doi: 10.1080/026432997381402
  • McCarthy, R., & Warrington, E. K. (1984). A two-route model of speech production. Evidence from aphasia. Brain, 107(Pt 2), 463–485.
  • McCloskey, M., Macaruso, P., & Rapp, B. (2006). Grapheme-to-lexeme feedback in the spelling system: Evidence from a dysgraphic patient. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(2), 278–307. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000518
  • Mesgarani, N., Cheung, C., Johnson, K., & Chang, E. F. (2014). Phonetic feature encoding in human superior temporal gyrus. Science, 343(6174), 1006–1010. doi: 10.1126/science.1245994
  • Monsell, S., Doyle, M. C., & Haggard, P. N. (1989). Effects of frequency on visual word recognition tasks: Where are they? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(1), 43–71.
  • Moses, M. S., Nickels, L. A., & Sheard, C. (2004). Disentangling the web: Neologistic perseverative errors in Jargon aphasia. Neurocase, 10(6), 452–461. doi: 10.1080/13554790490894057
  • Moses, M. S., Sheard, C., & Nickels, L. A. (2007). Insights into recurrent perseveration errors in aphasia: A case series approach. Aphasiology, 21, 10–11, 975–1001. doi:10.1080/02687030701198312.
  • Nickels, L., & Howard, D. (2004). Dissociating effects of number of phonemes, number of syllables, and syllabic complexity on word production in aphasia: It’s the number of phonemes that counts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21(1), 57–78.
  • Nozari, N., & Dell, G. S. (2013). How damaged brains repeat words: A computational approach. Brain and Language, 126(3), 327–337. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.07.005
  • Nozari, N., Kittredge, A. K., Dell, G. S., & Schwartz, M. F. (2010). Naming and repetition in aphasia: Steps, routes, and frequency effects. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(4), 541–559. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.08.001
  • O’Connell, P. F. (1981). Neologistic Jargon aphasia: A case report. Brain and Language, 12(2), 292–302. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(81)90020-1
  • Olson, A., Halloran, E., & Romani, C. (2015). Target/error overlap in Jargon aphasia: The case for a one-source model, lexical and non-lexical summation, and the special status of correct responses. Cortex, 73, 158–179. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.028
  • Olson, A. C., Romani, C., & Halloran, L. (2007). Localizing the deficit in a case of Jargon aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(2), 211–238. doi: 10.1080/02643290601137017
  • Papagno, C., & Basso, A. (1996). Perseveration in two aphasic patients. Cortex, 32(1), 67–82.
  • Patterson, K., Graham, N., & Hodges, J. R. (1994). The impact of semantic memory loss on phonological representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(1), 57–69. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.1.57
  • Pekkala, S., Albert, M. L., Spiro Iii, A., & Erkinjuntti, T. (2008). Perseveration in Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 25(2), 109–114.
  • Perecman, E., & Brown, J. W. (1981). Phonemic jargon: A case report. In J. Brown (Ed.), Jargonaphasia (1st ed, pp. 177–257). London: Academic Press.
  • Pilkington, E., Keidel, J., Kendrick, L. T., Saddy, J. D., Sage, K., & Robson, H. (2017). Sources of phoneme errors in repetition: Perseverative, neologistic, and lesion patterns in Jargon aphasia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 225. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00225
  • Pilkington, E., Sage, K., Saddy, D., & Robson, H. (2019). What can repetition, reading and naming tell us about Jargon aphasia? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 49, 45–56. ISSN 0911-6044. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.08.003
  • Plaut, D. C., & Kello, C. T. (1999). The emergence of phonology from the interplay of speech comprehension and production: A distributed connectionist approach. In The emergence of language (pp. 381–415). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Purcell, R., Lambon-Ralph, M., & Sage, K. (2018). Investigating the language, cognition and self-monitoring abilities of speakers with Jargon output. Aphasiology. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2018.1532070
  • Rayner, K., & Duffy, S. A. (1986). Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & Cognition, 14(3), 191–201. doi: 10.3758/BF03197692
  • Richardson, F. M., Seghier, M. L., Leff, A. P., Thomas, M. S. C., & Price, C. (2011). Multiple routes from occipital to temporal cortices during reading. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(22), 8239–8247. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6519-10.2011
  • Robin, D. A., & Schienberg, S. (1990). Subcortical lesions and aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55(1), 90–100.
  • Robinson, G. A., Butterworth, B., & Cipolotti, L. (2015). “My mind is doing it all”: No “brake” to stop speech generation in Jargon aphasia. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 28(4), 229–241.
  • Robson, H., Grube, M., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Griffiths, T. D., & Sage, K. (2013). Fundamental deficits of auditory perception in Wernicke’s aphasia. Cortex, 49(7), 1808–1822. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.11.012
  • Robson, H., Pilkington, E., Evans, L., DeLuca, V., & Keidel, J. (2017). Phonological and semantic processing during comprehension in Wernicke’s aphasia: A N400 and phonological mapping negativity study. Neuropsychologia, 100, 144–154. ISSN 0028-3932. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.04.012
  • Robson, J., Pring, T., Marshall, J., & Chiat, S. (2003). Phoneme frequency effects in Jargon aphasia: A phonological investigation of nonword errors. Brain and Language, 85(1), 109–124.
  • Robson, H., Sage, K., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012) Wernicke’s aphasia reflects a combination of acoustic-phonological and semantic control deficits: A case-series comparison of Wernicke’s aphasia, semantic dementia and semantic aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 50(2), 266–275. ISSN 0028-3932. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.021
  • Robson, H., Specht, K., Beaumont, H., Parkes, L. M., Sage, K., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Zahn, R. (2017). Arterial spin labelling shows functional depression of non-lesion tissue in chronic Wernicke’s aphasia. Cortex, 92, 249–260.
  • Romani, C., Galluzzi, C., & Olson, A. (2011). Phonological–lexical activation: A lexical component or an output buffer? Evidence from aphasic errors. Cortex, 47(2), 217–235. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.004
  • Rorden, C., & Brett, M. (2000). Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behavioural Neurology, 12(4), 191–200.
  • Sampson, M., & Faroqi-Shah, Y. (2011). Investigation of self-monitoring in fluent aphasia with Jargon. Aphasiology, 25(4), 505–528. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2010.523471
  • Sandson, J., & Albert, M. L. (1984). Varieties of perseveration. Neuropsychologia, 22(6), 715–732.
  • Sandson, J., & Albert, M. L. (1987). Perseveration in behavioral neurology. Neurology, 37(11), 1736–1736.
  • Santo Pietro, M. J., & Rigrodsky, S. (1986). Patterns of oral-verbal perseveration in adult aphasics. Brain and Language, 29(1), 1–17.
  • Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., Bloch, D. E., & Dell, G. S. (1994). Disordered speech production in aphasic and normal speakers. Brain and Language, 47, 52–88.
  • Schwartz, M. F., Wilshire, C. E., Gagnon, D. A., & Polansky, M. (2004). Origins of nonword phonological errors in aphasic picture naming. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21(2-4), 159–186. doi: 10.1080/02643290342000519
  • Stark, J. (2007). A review of classical accounts of verbal perseveration and their modern-day relevance. Aphasiology, 21(10–11), 928–959.
  • Strain, E., Patterson, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1995). Semantic effects in single-word naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(5), 1140–1154. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.5.1140
  • Strain, E., Patterson, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2002). Theories of word naming interact with spelling–sound consistency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(1), 207–214. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.28.1.207
  • Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (2004). A web-based interface to calculate phonotactic probability for words and nonwords in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 481. doi: 10.3758/BF03195594
  • Warrington, E. K., & James, M. (1991). Visual object and space perception battery (VOSP). Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.
  • Weekes, B. S. (1997). Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 50(2), 439–456. doi: 10.1080/713755710
  • Yamadori, A. (1981). Verbal perseveration in aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 19(4), 591–594.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.