878
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REGULAR ARTICLE

Phonotactics and syntax: investigating functional specialisation during structured sequence processing

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 346-358 | Received 22 Oct 2021, Accepted 15 Aug 2022, Published online: 09 Sep 2022

References

  • Acheson, D. J., & Hagoort, P. (2014). Twisting tongues to test for conflict-monitoring in speech production. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 206. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00206
  • Ardila, A., Bernal, B., & Rosselli, M. (2017). Should Broca’s area include Brodmann area 47? Psicothema, 29(1), 73–77. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.11
  • Badre, D., & D’Esposito, M. (2009). Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(9), 659–669. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2667
  • Badre, D., & Nee, D. E. (2018). Frontal cortex and the hierarchical control of behavior. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(2), 170–188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.005
  • Bahlmann, J., Blumenfeld, R. S., & D'Esposito, M. (2015). The rostro-caudal axis of frontal cortex Is sensitive to the domain of stimulus information. Cerebral Cortex, 25(7), 1815–1826. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht419
  • Bahlmann, J., Schubotz, R. I., & Friederici, A. D. (2008). Hierarchical artificial grammar processing engages Broca’s area. Neuroimage, 42(2), 525–534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.249
  • Bates, E., Wilson, S. M., Saygin, A. P., Dick, F., Sereno, M. I., Knight, R. T., & Dronkers, N. F. (2003). Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Nature Neuroscience, 6(5), 448–450. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1050
  • Berwick, R. C., & Chomsky, N. (2017). Why only US: Language and evolution. The MIT Press.
  • Bookheimer, S. (2002). Functional MRI of language: New approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25(1), 151–188. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142946
  • Chomsky, N. (1956). Three models for the description of language. IEEE Transactions on Information Theory, 2(3), 113–124. http://doi.org/10.1109/TIT.1956.1056813
  • Craig, A. D. B. (2009). How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 59–70. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555
  • Dell, G. S., & Chang, F. (2014). The P-chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120394. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0394
  • den Ouden, H. E., Daunizeau, J., Roiser, J., Friston, K. J., & Stephan, K. E. (2010). Striatal prediction error modulates cortical coupling. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(9), 3210–3219. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4458-09.2010
  • Dronkers, N. F. (1996). A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature, 384(6605), 159–161. https://doi.org/10.1038/384159a0
  • Fedorenko, E., Duncan, J., & Kanwisher, N. (2012). Language-selective and domain-general regions lie side by side within Broca’s area. Current Biology, 22(21), 2059–2062. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.011
  • Forkstam, C., Hagoort, P., Fernandez, G., Ingvar, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2006). Neural correlates of artificial syntactic structure classification. Neuroimage, 32(2), 956–967. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.057
  • Friederici, A. D., & Alter, K. (2004). Lateralization of auditory language functions: A dynamic dual pathway model. Brain and Language, 89(2), 267–276. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00351-1
  • Friederici, A. D., Chomsky, N., Berwick, R. C., Moro, A., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2017). Language, mind and brain. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(10), 713–722. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0184-4
  • Gow, D. W., Jr. (2012). The cortical organization of lexical knowledge: A dual lexicon model of spoken language processing. Brain and Language, 121(3), 273–288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.03.005
  • Grodzinsky, Y., & Amunts, K. (2006). Broca’s region. Oxford university press.
  • Hagoort, P. (2005). On broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 416–423. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.004
  • Hagoort, P., & Indefrey, P. (2014). The neurobiology of language beyond single words. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 37(1), 347–362. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-013847
  • Hagoort, P., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2009). The speaking brain. Science, 326(5951), https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1181675
  • Hashimoto, Y., Yokoyama, S., & Kawashima, R. (2014). Neural differences in processing of case particles in Japanese: An fMRI study. Brain and Behavior, 4(2), 180–186. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.201
  • Heim, S., van Ermingen, M., Huber, W., & Amunts, K. (2010). Left cytoarchitectonic BA 44 processes syntactic gender violations in determiner phrases. Human Brain Mapping, 31(10), 1532–1541. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20957
  • Heinz, J., & Idsardi, W. (2011). Sentence and word complexity. Science, 333(6040), 295–297. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1210358
  • Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393–402. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2113
  • Hsu, N. S., Jaeggi, S. M., & Novick, J. M. (2017). A common neural hub resolves syntactic and non-syntactic conflict through cooperation with task-specific networks. Brain and Language, 166, 63–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.12.006
  • Jackendoff, R. (2009). Parallels and nonparallels between language and music. Music Perception, 26(3), 195–204. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2009.26.3.195
  • Jacquemot, C., Pallier, C., LeBihan, D., Dehaene, S., & Dupoux, E. (2003). Phonological grammar shapes the auditory cortex: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(29), 9541–9546. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-29-09541.2003
  • Jeon, H. A. (2014). Hierarchical processing in the prefrontal cortex in a variety of cognitive domains. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 223. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00223
  • Jeon, H. A., & Friederici, A. D. (2013). Two principles of organization in the prefrontal cortex are cognitive hierarchy and degree of automaticity. Nature Communications, 4(1), 2041. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms3041
  • Kittredge, A. K., & Dell, G. S. (2016). Learning to speak by listening: Transfer of phonotactics from perception to production. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 8–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2015.08.001
  • Koechlin, E., & Summerfield, C. (2007). An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(6), 229–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.005
  • Kouneiher, F., Charron, S., & Koechlin, E. (2009). Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 12(7), 939–945. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2321
  • Longworth, C. E., Keenan, S. E., Barker, R. A., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (2005). The basal ganglia and rule-governed language use: Evidence from vascular and degenerative conditions. Brain, 128(Pt 3), 584–596. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh387
  • Margulies, D. S., Ghosh, S. S., Goulas, A., Falkiewicz, M., Huntenburg, J. M., Langs, G., Bezgin, G., Eickhoff, S. B., Castellanos, F. X., Petrides, M., Jefferies, E., & Smallwood, J. (2016). Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(44), 12574–12579. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1608282113
  • Matchin, W., Hammerly, C., & Lau, E. (2017). The role of the IFG and pSTS in syntactic prediction: Evidence from a parametric study of hierarchical structure in fMRI. Cortex, 88, 106–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.12.010
  • Matchin, W., & Hickok, G. (2020). The cortical organization of syntax. Cerebral Cortex, 30(3), 1481–1498. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz180
  • Moro, A. (2014). Response to Pulvermuller: The syntax of actions and other metaphors. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(5), 221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.012
  • Moro, A., Tettamanti, M., Perani, D., Donati, C., Cappa, S. F., & Fazio, F. (2001). Syntax and the brain: Disentangling grammar by selective anomalies. Neuroimage, 13(1), 110–118. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0668
  • Nichols, T., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T., & Poline, J. B. (2005). Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage, 25(3), 653–660. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005
  • Nieuwland, M. S., Martin, A. E., & Carreiras, M. (2012). Brain regions that process case: Evidence from Basque. Human Brain Mapping, 33(11), 2509–2520. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21377
  • Papitto, G., Friederici, A. D., & Zaccarella, E. (2020). The topographical organization of motor processing: An ALE meta-analysis on six action domains and the relevance of Broca’s region. Neuroimage, 206, 116321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116321
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2007). Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 105–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.002
  • Pulvermuller, F. (2014). The syntax of action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(5), 219–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.001
  • Sakai, K., & Passingham, R. E. (2003). Prefrontal interactions reflect future task operations. Nature Neuroscience, 6(1), 75–81. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn987
  • Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., Reiss, A. L., & Greicius, M. D. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 2349–2356. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007
  • Segaert, K., Menenti, L., Weber, K., Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2012). Shared syntax in language production and language comprehension – An FMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 22(7), 1662–1670. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr249
  • Snijders, T. M., Vosse, T., Kempen, G., Van Berkum, J. J., Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2009). Retrieval and unification of syntactic structure in sentence comprehension: An FMRI study using word-category ambiguity. Cerebral Cortex, 19(7), 1493–1503. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn187
  • Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., Mazoyer, B., & Joliot, M. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage, 15(1), 273–289. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978
  • Udden, J., & Bahlmann, J. (2012). A rostro-caudal gradient of structured sequence processing in the left inferior frontal gyrus. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 2023–2032. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0009
  • Udden, J., de Jesus Dias Martins, M., Zuidema, W., & Tecumseh Fitch, W. (2020). Hierarchical structure in sequence processing: How to measure it and determine Its neural implementation. Topics in Cognitive Science, 12(3), 910–924. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12442
  • Udden, J., & Männel, C..2018). Artificial grammar learning and its neurobiology in relation to language processing and development. In S.-A. Rueschemeyer & M. G. Gaskell (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics (2nd ed, pp. 755–783). Oxford University Press.
  • Ullman, M. T. (2001). The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(1), 37–69. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005204207369
  • Ullman, M. T., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J. H., Koroshetz, W. J., & Pinker, S. (1997). A neural dissociation within language: Evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(2), 266–276. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.2.266
  • Vaden, K. I., Jr., Piquado, T., & Hickok, G. (2011). Sublexical properties of spoken words modulate activity in Broca’s area but not superior temporal cortex: Implications for models of speech recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2665–2674. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2011.21620
  • Vosse, T., & Kempen, G. (2000). Syntactic structure assembly in human parsing: A computational model based on competitive inhibition and a lexicalist grammar. Cognition, 75(2), 105–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00063-9
  • Yokoyama, S., Maki, H., Hashimoto, Y., Toma, M., & Kawashima, R. (2012). Mechanism of case processing in the brain: An fMRI study. PLoS One, 7(7), e40474. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040474