342
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REGULAR ARTICLE

Preverbal syntactic complexity leads to local coherence effects

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 359-389 | Received 09 Dec 2020, Accepted 19 Aug 2022, Published online: 06 Oct 2022

References

  • Altmann, G. T., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73(3), 247–264. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00059-1
  • Altmann, G. T., & Kamide, Y. (2007). The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(4), 502–518. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.12.004
  • Anand, P., Chung, S., & Wagers, M. (2020). Widening the net: Challenges for gathering linguistic data in the digital age. Response to NSF SBE.
  • Apurva, & Husain, S. (2021). Revisiting anti-locality effects: Evidence against prediction-based accounts. Journal of Memory and Language, 121, Article 104280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2021.104280
  • Apurva, & Husain, S. (2022). Effect of noncanonical word order and argument proximity on processing of SOV languages. In P. Chandra (Ed.), Variation in south asian languages: From macro to micro-differences. Springer Nature.
  • Avetisyan, S., Lago, S., & Vasishth, S. (2020). Does case marking affect agreement attraction in comprehension? Journal of Memory and Language, 112, Article 104087. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104087
  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
  • Bates, D., Machler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using LME4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Bhatia, S., & Dillon, B. (2022). Processing agreement in Hindi: When agreement feeds attraction. Journal of Memory and Language, 125, Article 104322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2022.104322
  • Bhatt, R., Narasimhan, B., Palmer, M., Rambow, O., Sharma, D. M., & Xia, F. (2009). A multi-representational and multi-layered treebank for Hindi/Urdu. In Proceedings of the third linguistic annotation workshop (pp. 186–189). Association for Computational Linguistics.
  • Bicknell, K., & Levy, R. (2009, June). A model of local coherence effects in human sentence processing as consequences of updates from bottom-up prior to posterior beliefs. In Proceedings of human language technologies: The 2009 annual conference of the North American chapter of the association for computational linguistics (pp. 665–673). Association for Computational Linguistics. https://www.aclweb.org/anthology/N09-1075
  • Brothers, T., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2021). Word predictability effects are linear, not logarithmic: Implications for probabilistic models of sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 116, Article 104174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104174
  • Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (1999). Toward a connectionist model of recursion in human linguistic performance. Cognitive Science, 23(2), 157–205. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2302_2.
  • Christiansen, M. H., & MacDonald, M. C. (2009). A usage-based approach to recursion in sentence processing. Language Learning, 59(s1), 126–161. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.2009.59.issue-s1
  • Christianson, K., Dempsey, J., Tsiola, A., & Goldshtein, M. (2022). What if they're just not that into you (or your experiment)? On motivation and psycholinguistics. In Psychology of learning and motivation-advances in research and theory. Academic Press Inc.
  • Drummond, A. (2013). Ibex farm. http://spellout.net/ibexfarm
  • Engelmann, F., & Vasishth, S. (2009). Processing grammatical and ungrammatical center embeddings in English and German: A computational model. In A. Howes, D. Peebles, & R. Cooper (Eds.), Proceedings of 9th international conference on cognitive modeling.
  • Ferreira, F. (2003). The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 47(2), 164–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00005-7
  • Ferreira, F., Bailey, K. G., & Ferraro, V. (2002). Good-enough representations in language comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 11–15. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00158
  • Ferreira, F., & Patson, N. D. (2007). The ‘good enough’ approach to language comprehension. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(1–2), 71–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnco.2007.1.issue-1-2.
  • Fischler, I. S., & Bloom, P. A. (1979). Automatic and attentional processes in the effects of sentence contexts on word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90534-6
  • Frank, S. L., Trompenaars, T., & Vasishth, S. (2016). Cross-linguistic differences in processing double-embedded relative clauses: Working-memory constraints or language statistics? Cognitive Science, 40(3), 554–578. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.2016.40.issue-3
  • Frazier, L. (1985). Syntactic complexity. In L. K. D. Dowty, & A. Zwicky (Eds.), Natural language parsing (pp. 129–189). Cambridge University Press.
  • Futrell, R., Gibson, E., & Levy, R. (2020). Lossy-context surprisal: An information-theoretic model of memory effects in sentence processing. Cognitive Science, 44(3). https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12814.
  • Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68(1), 1–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00034-1
  • Gibson, E. (2006). The interaction of top-down and bottom-up statistics in the resolution of syntactic category ambiguity. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(3), 363–388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.005
  • Gibson, E., Piantadosi, S. T., Brink, K., Bergen, L., Lim, E., & Saxe, R. (2013). A noisy-channel account of crosslinguistic word-order variation. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1079–1088. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612463705
  • Gibson, E., & Thomas, J. (1999). Memory limitations and structural forgetting: The perception of complex ungrammatical sentences as grammatical. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(3), 225–248. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909699386293
  • Grodner, D., & Gibson, E. (2005). Consequences of the serial nature of linguistic input for sentenial complexity. Cognitive Science, 29(2), 261–290. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_7
  • Hale, J. (2001). A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. In Proceedings of the second meeting of the North American chapter of the association for computational linguistics on language technologies (pp. 1–8). Association for Computational Linguistics.
  • Halekoh, U., & Højsgaard, S. (2014). A Kenward-Roger approximation and parametric bootstrap methods for tests in linear mixed models – the R package pbkrtest. Journal of Statistical Software, 59(9), 1–32. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v059.i09
  • Häussler, J., & Bader, M. (2015). An interference account of the missing-VP effect. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 766. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00766
  • Hsiao, Y., & MacDonald, M. C. (2013). Experience and generalization in a connectionist model of Mandarin Chinese relative clause processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 767. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00767
  • Huettig, F. (2015). Four central questions about prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1626, 118–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.014
  • Husain, S., Vasishth, S., & Srinivasan, N. (2014). Strong expectations cancel locality effects: Evidence from Hindi. PloS One, 9(7), Article e100986. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100986
  • Jäger, L., Chen, Z., Li, Q., Lin, C. J. C., & Vasishth, S. (2015). The subject-relative advantage in Chinese: Evidence for expectation-based processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 79–80, 97–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2014.10.005
  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122–149. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.122
  • Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Woolley, J. D. (1982). Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111(2), 228. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.111.2.228
  • Kachru, Y. (2006). Hindi. John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Kaiser, E. (2014). Experimental paradigms in psycholinguistics. In R. J. Podesva, & D. Sharma (Eds.), Research methods in linguistics (pp. 135–168). Cambridge University Press.
  • Kamide, Y., & Kukona, A. (2018). The influence of globally ungrammatical local syntactic constraints on real-time sentence comprehension: Evidence from the visual world paradigm and reading. Cognitive Science, 42(8), 2976–2998. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.2018.42.issue-8
  • Karimi, H., & Ferreira, F. (2016). Good-enough linguistic representations and online cognitive equilibrium in language processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(5), 1013–1040. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1053951
  • Keshtiari, N., & Vasishth, S. (2012). Reactivation of antecedents by overt vs. null pronouns: Evidence from Persian. Journal of Language Modelling, 1(2), 243–266. https://doi.org/10.15398/jlm.v1i2.54.
  • Kliegl, R., Grabner, E., Rolfs, M., & Engbert, R. (2004). Length, frequency, and predictability effects of words on eye movements in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16(1-2), 262–284. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440340000213
  • Konieczny, L. (2000). Locality and parsing complexity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(6), 627–645. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026528912821
  • Konieczny, L., Müller-Feldmeth, D., Hachmann, W., Schwarzkopf, S., & Wolfer, S. (2009, January). Local syntactic coherence interpretation. Evidence from a visual world study. In Proceedings of the 31th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1133–1138). Cognitive Science Society.
  • Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 32–59. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2015.1102299
  • Kurumada, C., & Jaeger, T. F. (2015). Communicative efficiency in language production: Optional case-marking in Japanese. Journal of Memory and Language, 83, 152–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2015.03.003
  • Kutas, M., DeLong, K. A., & Smith, N. J. (2011). A look around at what lies ahead: Prediction and predictability in language processing. In M. Bar (Ed.), Predictions in the brain: Using our past to generate a future (pp. 190–207). Oxford University Press.
  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7350657.
  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307(5947), 161–163. https://doi.org/10.1038/307161a0.
  • Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106(3), 1126–1177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.006
  • Levy, R. (2013). Memory and surprisal in human sentence comprehension. In R. P. G. van Gompel (Ed.), Sentence processing. Psychology Press.
  • Levy, R., & Keller, F. (2013). Expectation and locality effects in German verb-final structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(2), 199–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.02.005
  • Lewis, R. L., & Vasishth, S. (2005). An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science, 29(3), 375–419. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_25
  • Lewis, S., & Phillips, C. (2015). Aligning grammatical theories and language processing models. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44(1), 27–46. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9329-z
  • Logačev, P., & Vasishth, S. (2016). Understanding underspecification: A comparison of two computational implementations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(5), 996–1012. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1134602
  • MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. H. (2002). Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109(1), 35–54. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.35
  • Marslen-Wilson, W. (1973). Linguistic structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies. Nature, 244(5417), 522–523. https://doi.org/10.1038/244522a0.
  • Miller, G. A., & Chomsky, N. (1963). Finitary models of language users. In R. Bush, R. D. Luce, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 419–492). Wiley.
  • Nakatani, K., & Gibson, E. (2010). An on-line study of japanese nesting complexity. Cognitive Science, 34(1), 94–112. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01067.x
  • Norcliffe, E., Harris, A. C., & Jaeger, T. F. (2015). Cross-linguistic psycholinguistics and its critical role in theory development: Early beginnings and recent advances. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(9), 1009–1032. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2015.1080373
  • Paape, D., & Vasishth, S. (2016). Local coherence and preemptive digging-in effects in German. Language and Speech, 59(3), 387–403. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830915608410.
  • Pandharipande, R., & Kachru, Y. (1977). Relational grammar, ergativity, and Hindi-Urdu. Lingua, 41(3-4), 217–238. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(77)90080-8
  • Parker, D. (2019). Two minds are not always better than one: Modeling evidence for a single sentence analyzer. Glossa a Journal of General Linguistics, 4(1), 64. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.766
  • Piantadosi, S. T., Tily, H., & Gibson, E. (2011). Word lengths are optimized for efficient communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(9), 3526–3529. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1012551108
  • Piantadosi, S. T., Tily, H., & Gibson, E. (2012). The communicative function of ambiguity in language. Cognition, 122(3), 280–291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.004
  • Pickering, M. J., & Gambi, C. (2018). Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10), 1002–1044. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000158
  • Rayner, K., Slattery, T. J., Drieghe, D., & Liversedge, S. P. (2011). Eye movements and word skipping during reading: effects of word length and predictability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(2), 514–528. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020990
  • R Core Team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software manual]. http://www.R-project.org/.
  • Ros, I., Santesteban, M., Fukumura, K., & Laka, I. (2015). Aiming at shorter dependencies: The role of agreement morphology. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(9), 1156–1174. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.994009
  • Schütze, C. T., & Sprouse, J. (2014). Judgment data. In R. J. Podesva, & D. Sharma (Eds.), Research methods in linguistics (pp. 27–50). Cambridge University Press.
  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Lacount, K. L. (1988). Semantic relatedness and the scope of facilitation for upcoming words in sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 14(2), 344–354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.14.2.344
  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Shoben, E. J. (1985). The influence of sentence constraint on the scope of facilitation for upcoming words. Journal of Memory and Language, 24(2), 232–252. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(85)90026-9
  • Sharma, K., Futrell, R., & Husain, S. (2020). What determines the order of verbal dependents in Hindi? Effects of efficiency in comprehension and production. In Proceedings of cognitive modeling and computational linguistics (CMCL). Association of Computational Linguistics.
  • Slioussar, N. (2018). Forms and features: The role of syncretism in number agreement attraction. Journal of Memory and Language, 101, 51–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.03.006
  • Smith, G. (2018). A theory of timing effects in a self-organizing model of sentence processing [Doctoral dissertation, University of Connecticut].
  • Staub, A. (2015). The effect of lexical predictability on eye movements in reading: Critical review and theoretical interpretation. Language and Linguistics Compass, 9(8), 311–327. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12151.
  • Staub, A., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2006). Syntactic prediction in language comprehension: Evidence from either …or. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(2), 425–436. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.2.425
  • Stowe, L. A., Kaan, E., Sabourin, L., & Taylor, R. C. (2018). The sentence wrap-up dogma. Cognition, 176, 232–247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.011
  • Swets, B., Desmet, T., Clifton, C., & Ferreira, F. (2008). Underspecification of syntactic ambiguities: Evidence from self-paced reading. Memory & Cognition, 36(1), 201–216. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.1.201
  • Tabor, W., Galantucci, B., & Richardson, D. (2004). Effects of merely local syntactic coherence on sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 355–370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.01.001
  • Taylor, W. (1953). ‘Cloze’ procedure: A new tool for measuring readability. Journalism Quarterly, 30(4), 415–433. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769905303000401
  • Ueno, M., & Polinsky, M. (2009). Does headedness affect processing? A new look at the VO–OV contrast. Journal of Linguistics, 45(3), 675–710. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226709990065
  • Van Gompel, R. P., Pickering, M. J., & Traxler, M. J. (2000). Unrestricted race: A new model of syntactic ambiguity resolution. In Reading as a perceptual process (pp. 621–648). Elsevier.
  • Vasishth, S., & Lewis, R. L. (2006). Argument-head distance and processing complexity: Explaining both locality and antilocality effects. Language, 82(4), 767–794. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2006.0236
  • Vasishth, S., Suckow, K., Lewis, R. L., & Kern, S. (2010). Short-term forgetting in sentence comprehension: Crosslinguistic evidence from verb-final structures. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(4), 533–567. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960903310587
  • Villata, S., Tabor, W., & Franck, J. (2018). Encoding and retrieval interference in sentence comprehension: Evidence from agreement. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00002
  • von der Malsburg, T., & Vasishth, S. (2013). Scanpaths reveal syntactic underspecification and reanalysis strategies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(10), 1545–1578. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.728232
  • Yadav, H., Mittal, S., & Husain, S. (2022). A reappraisal of dependency length minimization as a linguistic universal. Open Mind, 6, 147–168. https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00060.
  • Yadav, H., Vaidya, A., Shukla, V., & Husain, S. (2020). Word order typology interacts with linguistic complexity: A cross-linguistic corpus study. Cognitive Science, 44(4), Article e12822. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.v44.4
  • Yngve, V. H. (1960). A model and an hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 104(5), 444–466. https://www.jstor.org/stable/985230
  • Zola, D. (1984). Redundancy and word perception during reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 36(3), 277–284. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206369

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.