1,569
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REGULAR ARTICLES

Testing the automaticity of syntax using masked visual priming

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 925-949 | Received 16 Jun 2022, Accepted 20 Jan 2023, Published online: 08 Feb 2023

References

  • Ansorge, U., Reynvoet, B., Hendler, J., Oettl, L., & Evert, S. (2013). Conditional automaticity in subliminal morphosyntactic priming. Psychological Research, 77(4), 399–421. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0442-z
  • Baayen, R. H., Dijkstra, T., & Schreuder, R. (1997). Singulars and plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual-route model. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(1), 94–117. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2509
  • Baayen, R. H., Gulikers, L., & Piepenbrock, R. (1995). The CELEX lexical database (WebCelex). University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium.
  • Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Batterink, L., & Neville, H. J. (2013). The human brain processes syntax in the absence of conscious awareness. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(19), 8528–8533. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0618-13.2013
  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
  • Berkovitch, L., & Dehaene, S. (2019). Subliminal syntactic priming. Cognitive Psychology, 109, 26–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.12.001
  • Carello, C., Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1988). Rapid naming is affected by association but not by syntax. Memory & Cognition, 16(3), 187–195. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197751
  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. Mouton.
  • Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. MIT Press.
  • Colé, P., & Segui, J. (1994). Grammatical incongruency and vocabulary types. Memory & Cognition, 22(4), 387–394. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200865
  • Culbertson, J., & Adger, D. (2014). Language learners privilege structured meaning over surface frequency. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(16), 5842–5847. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1320525111
  • Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J.-P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron, 70(2), 200–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018
  • Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec’H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., van de Moortele, P. F., & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395(6702), 597–600. https://doi.org/10.1038/26967
  • Dikker, S., Rabagliati, H., Farmer, T. A., & Pylkkänen, L. (2010). Early occipital sensitivity to syntactic category is based on form typicality. Psychological Science, 21(5), 629–634. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610367751
  • Dikker, S., Rabagliati, H., & Pylkkänen, L. (2009). Sensitivity to syntax in visual cortex. Cognition, 110(3), 293–321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.09.008
  • Everaert, M. B. H., Huybregts, M. A. C., Chomsky, N., Berwick, R. C., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2015). Structures, not strings: Linguistics as part of the cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(12), 729–743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.09.008
  • Faussart, C., Jakubowicz, C., & Costes, M. (1999). Gender and number processing in spoken French and Spanish. The Italian Journal of Linguistics, 11, 75–101.
  • Ferrand, L., & New, B. (2004). Semantic and associative priming in the mental lexicon. In P. Bonin (Ed.), Mental lexicon: “Some words to talk about words” (pp. 25–43). Nova Science Publishers.
  • Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8
  • Friederici, A. D. (2011). The brain basis of language processing: From structure to function. Physiological Reviews, 91(4), 1357–1392. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00006.2011
  • Friederici, A. D., Chomsky, N., Berwick, R. C., Moro, A., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2017). Language, mind and brain. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(10), 713–722. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0184-4
  • Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A., & Mecklinger, A. (1996). Temporal structure of syntactic parsing: Early and late event-related brain potential effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(5), 1219–1248. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.5.1219
  • Friederici, A. D., & Jacobsen, T. (1999). Processing grammatical gender during language comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(5), 467–484. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023264209610
  • Goodman, G. O., McClelland, J. L., & Gibbs, R. W. (1981). The role of syntactic context in word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 9(6), 580–586. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202352
  • Goucha, T., Zaccarella, E., & Friederici, A. D. (2017). A revival of Homo loquens as a builder of labeled structures: Neurocognitive considerations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 81(Pt B), 213–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.036
  • Grant, D. A. (1948). The latin square principle in the design and analysis of psychological experiments. Psychological Bulletin, 45(5), 427–442. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0053912
  • Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Wiley.
  • Greenwald, A. G., Klinger, M. R., & Schuh, E. S. (1995). Activation by marginally perceptible (“subliminal”) stimuli: Dissociation of unconscious from conscious cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124(1), 22–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.124.1.22
  • Grossi, G. (2006). Relatedness proportion effects on masked associative priming: An ERP study. Psychophysiology, 43(1), 21–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00383.x
  • Gurjanov, M., Lukatela, G., Lukatela, K., Savić, M., & Turvey, M. T. (1985). Grammatical priming of inflected nouns by the gender of possessive adjectives. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11(4), 692–701. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.11.1-4.692
  • Gurjanov, M., Lukatela, G., Moskovljević, J., Savić, M., & Turvey, M. T. (1985). Grammatical priming of inflected nouns by inflected adjectives. Cognition, 19(1), 55–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90031-9
  • Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 194–205. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892999563328
  • Hanna, J., Mejias, S., Schelstraete, M. A., Pulvermüller, F., Shtyrov, Y., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2014). Early activation of Broca’s area in grammar processing as revealed by the syntactic mismatch negativity and distributed source analysis. Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(2), 66–76. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2013.860087
  • Hasting, A. S., Kotz, S. A., & Friederici, A. D. (2007). Setting the stage for automatic syntax processing: The mismatch negativity as an indicator of syntactic priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(3), 386–400. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.386
  • Herrmann, B., Maess, B., Hasting, A. S., & Friederici, A. D. (2009). Localization of the syntactic mismatch negativity in the temporal cortex: An MEG study. NeuroImage, 48(3), 590–600. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.082
  • Humphreys, G. W., Evett, L. J., & Taylor, D. E. (1982). Automatic phonological priming in visual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 10(6), 576–590. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202440
  • Hutchison, K. A. (2007). Attentional control and the relatedness proportion effect in semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(4), 645–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.645
  • Iijima, K., & Sakai, K. L. (2014). Subliminal enhancement of predictive effects during syntactic processing in the left inferior frontal gyrus: An MEG study. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 217. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00217
  • JASP Team. (2020). JASP (0.13.0).
  • Jiménez-Ortega, L., Badaya, E., Casado, P., Fondevila, S., Hernández-Gutiérrez, D., Muñoz, F., Sánchez-García, J., & Martín-Loeches, M. (2021). The automatic but flexible and content-dependent nature of syntax. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.651158
  • Jiménez-Ortega, L., García-Milla, M., Fondevila, S., Casado, P., Hernández-Gutiérrez, D., & Martín-Loeches, M. (2014). Automaticity of higher cognitive functions: Neurophysiological evidence for unconscious syntactic processing of masked words. Biological Psychology, 103, 83–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.08.011
  • Kaan, E., Kirkham, J., & Wijnen, F. (2016). Prediction and integration in native and second-language processing of elliptical structures. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000844
  • Katz, L., Boyce, S., Goldstein, L., & Lukatela, G. (1987). Grammatical information effects in auditory word recognition. Cognition, 25(3), 235–263. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(87)80005-7
  • Kiefer, M. (2002). The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: Further evidence for an automatic spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(1), 27–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00085-4
  • Kiefer, M. (2019). Cognitive control over unconscious cognition: Flexibility and generalizability of task set influences on subsequent masked semantic priming. Psychological Research, 83(7), 1556–1570. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1011-x
  • Kiefer, M., Trumpp, N. M., Schaitz, C., Reuss, H., & Kunde, W. (2019). Attentional modulation of masked semantic priming by visible and masked task cues. Cognition, 187, 62–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.02.013
  • Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: A critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 857–875. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2093
  • Krakauer, J. W., Ghazanfar, A. A., Gomez-Marin, A., MacIver, M. A., & Poeppel, D. (2017). Neuroscience needs behavior: Correcting a reductionist bias. Neuron, 93(3), 480–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.041
  • Kroczek, L. O. H., & Gunter, T. C. (2021). The time course of speaker-specific language processing. Cortex, 141, 311–321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2021.04.017
  • Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 32–59. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2015.1102299
  • Lau, E., Stroud, C., Plesch, S., & Phillips, C. (2006). The role of structural prediction in rapid syntactic analysis. Brain and Language, 98(1), 74–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.02.003
  • Lehtonen, M., Monahan, P. J., & Poeppel, D. (2011). Evidence for early morphological decomposition: Combining masked priming with magnetoencephalography. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3366–3379. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00035
  • Leminen, A., Leminen, M., Kujala, T., & Shtyrov, Y. (2013). Neural dynamics of inflectional and derivational morphology processing in the human brain. Cortex, 49(10), 2758–2771. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.08.007
  • Leminen, A., Smolka, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Pliatsikas, C. (2019). Morphological processing in the brain: The good (inflection), the bad (derivation) and the ugly (compounding). Cortex, 116, 4–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.08.016
  • Longtin, C.-M., & Meunier, F. (2005). Morphological decomposition in early visual word processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(1), 26–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.008
  • Longtin, C.-M., Segui, J., & Hallé, P. A. (2003). Morphological priming without morphological relationship. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(3), 313–334. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960244000036
  • Lucchese, G., Hanna, J., Autenrieb, A., Miller, T. M. C., & Pulvermüller, F. (2017). Electrophysiological evidence for early and interactive symbol access and rule processing in retrieving and combining language constructions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(2), 254–266. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01038
  • Lukatela, G., Carello, C., Kostić, A., & Turvey, M. T. (1988). Low-constraint facilitation in lexical decision with single-word contexts. The American Journal of Psychology, 101(1), 15–29. https://doi.org/10.2307/1422790
  • Lukatela, G., Kostić, A., Feldman, L. B., & Turvey, M. T. (1983). Grammatical priming of inflected nouns. Memory & Cognition, 11(1), 59–63. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197662
  • Lukatela, G., Kostić, A., Todorović, D., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1987). Type and number of violations and the grammatical congruency effect in lexical decision. Psychological Research, 49(1), 37–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309201
  • Lukatela, G., Moraca, J., Stojnov, D., Savic, M. D., Katz, L., & Turvey, M. T. (1982). Grammatical priming effects between pronouns and inflected verb forms. Psychological Research, 44(4), 297–311. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309326
  • MacGregor, L. J., Pulvermüller, F., Van Casteren, M., & Shtyrov, Y. (2012). Ultra-rapid access to words in the brain. Nature Communications, 3(1), 711. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms1715
  • Mancini, S., Molinaro, N., Davidson, D. J., Avilés, A., & Carreiras, M. (2014). Person and the syntax–discourse interface: An eye-tracking study of agreement. Journal of Memory and Language, 76, 141–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2014.06.010
  • Maran, M., Friederici, A. D., & Zaccarella, E. (2022). Syntax through the looking glass: A review on two-word linguistic processing across behavioral, neuroimaging and neurostimulation studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 142, 104881. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104881
  • Maran, M., Numssen, O., Hartwigsen, G., & Zaccarella, E. (2022). Online neurostimulation of Broca’s area does not interfere with syntactic predictions: A combined TMS-EEG approach to basic linguistic combination. Frontiers in Psychology, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.968836
  • Marascuilo, L. A. (1970). Extensions of the significance test for one-parameter signal detection hypotheses. Psychometrika, 35(2), 237–243. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02291265
  • Marcus, G. F., Brinkmann, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R., & Pinker, S. (1995). German inflection: The exception that proves the rule. Cognitive Psychology, 29(3), 189–256. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1995.1015
  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1973). Linguistic structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies. Nature, 244(5417), 522–523. https://doi.org/10.1038/244522a0
  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Bozic, M., & Randall, B. (2008). Early decomposition in visual word recognition: Dissociating morphology, form, and meaning. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(3), 394–421. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960701588004
  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (1998). Rules, representations, and the English past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(11), 428–435. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01239-X
  • Matchin, W., Hammerly, C., & Lau, E. (2017). The role of the IFG and pSTS in syntactic prediction: Evidence from a parametric study of hierarchical structure in fMRI. Cortex, 88, 106–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.12.010
  • McClelland, J. L., & Rogers, T. T. (2003). The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(4), 310–322. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1076
  • Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli. Cognition, 80(3), 215–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00139-6
  • Nakamura, K., Makuuchi, M., Oga, T., Mizuochi-Endo, T., Iwabuchi, T., Nakajima, Y., & Dehaene, S. (2018). Neural capacity limits during unconscious semantic processing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 47(8), 929–937. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13890
  • Neophytou, K., Manouilidou, C., Stockall, L., & Marantz, A. (2018). Syntactic and semantic restrictions on morphological recomposition: MEG evidence from Greek. Brain and Language, 183, 11–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2018.05.003
  • Nieuwland, M. S. (2019). Do ‘early’ brain responses reveal word form prediction during language comprehension? A critical review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 96, 367–400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.11.019
  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
  • Ortells, J. J., Kiefer, M., Castillo, A., Megías, M., & Morillas, A. (2016). The semantic origin of unconscious priming: Behavioral and event-related potential evidence during category congruency priming from strongly and weakly related masked words. Cognition, 146, 143–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.012
  • Penke, M., Weyerts, H., Gross, M., Zander, E., Münte, T. F., & Clahsen, H. (1997). How the brain processes complex words: An event-related potential study of German verb inflections. Cognitive Brain Research, 6(1), 37–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(97)00012-8
  • Perea, M., & Rosa, E. (2002a). Does the proportion of associatively related pairs modulate the associative priming effect at very brief stimulus-onset asynchronies? Acta Psychologica, 110(1), 103–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00074-9
  • Perea, M., & Rosa, E. (2002b). The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task. Psychological Research, 66(3), 180–194. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-002-0086-5
  • Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language. Science, 253(5019), 530–535. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1857983
  • Pollock, J.-Y. (1989). Verb movement, Universal Grammar and the structure of IP. Linguistic Inquiry, 20(3), 365–424.
  • Pulvermüller, F., & Assadollahi, R. (2007). Grammar or serial order?: Discrete combinatorial brain mechanisms reflected by the syntactic mismatch negativity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(6), 971–980. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.971
  • Pulvermüller, F., & Shtyrov, Y. (2003). Automatic processing of grammar in the human brain as revealed by the mismatch negativity. NeuroImage, 20(1), 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00261-1
  • Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., & New, B. (2004). The broth in my brother’s brothel: Morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(6), 1090–1098. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196742
  • R core team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (4.1.1.). R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
  • Segui, J., & Grainger, J. (1990). Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: Effects of relative prime-target frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(1), 65–76. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.16.1.65
  • Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Sanders, M., & Langer, P. (1984). Pre- and postlexical loci of contextual effects on word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 12(4), 315–328. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198291
  • Seth, A. K., Dienes, Z., Cleeremans, A., Overgaard, M., & Pessoa, L. (2008). Measuring consciousness: Relating behavioural and neurophysiological approaches. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(8), 314–321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.008
  • Shtyrov, Y., & Lenzen, M. (2017). First-pass neocortical processing of spoken language takes only 30 msec: Electrophysiological evidence. Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(1), 24–38. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2016.1156663
  • Shtyrov, Y., & Macgregor, L. J. (2016). Near-instant automatic access to visually presented words in the human neocortex: Neuromagnetic evidence. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep26558
  • Sonnenstuhl, I., Eisenbeiss, S., & Clahsen, H. (1999). Morphological priming in the German mental lexicon. Cognition, 72(3), 203–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00033-5
  • Staub, A., & Clifton, C. (2006). Syntactic prediction in language comprehension: Evidence from either … or. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(2), 425–436. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.2.425
  • Ullman, M. T. (2001). A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/procedural model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(10), 717–726. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094573
  • Ullman, M. T., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J. H., Koroshetz, W. J., & Pinker, S. (1997). A neural dissociation within language: Evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(2), 266–276. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.2.266
  • Van den Bussche, E., Van den Noortgate, W., & Reynvoet, B. (2009). Mechanisms of masked priming: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 452–477. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015329
  • Weyerts, H., Münte, T. F., Smid, H. G. O. M., & Heinze, H.-J. (1996). Mental representations of morphologically complex words: An event-related potential study with adult humans. Neuroscience Letters, 206(2-3), 125–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(96)12442-3
  • Ye, Z., Luo, Y., Friederici, A. D., & Zhou, X. (2006). Semantic and syntactic processing in Chinese sentence comprehension: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Research, 1071(1), 186–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.085
  • Zaccarella, E., Meyer, L., Makuuchi, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2017). Building by syntax: The neural basis of minimal linguistic structures. Cerebral Cortex, 27(1), 411–421. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv234
  • Zaccarella, E., Papitto, G., & Friederici, A. D. (2021). Language and action in Broca’s area: Computational differentiation and cortical segregation. Brain and Cognition, 147, 105651. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105651