116
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REGULAR ARTICLES

The impact of language proficiency on neuro-cognitive mechanisms supporting second-language spoken word recognition: an ERP study on Chinese-English bilinguals

&
Pages 552-570 | Received 24 Aug 2023, Accepted 05 Apr 2024, Published online: 23 Apr 2024

References

  • Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., Cappa, S. F., & Costa, A. (2012). Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2076–2086. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr287
  • Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(3), 242–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.10.003
  • Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Nelson, D. L., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The English Lexicon Project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193014
  • Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2013). Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 547–567. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.812093
  • Boutros, N. N., & Belger, A. (1999). Midlatency evoked potentials attenuation and augmentation reflect different aspects of sensory gating. Biological Psychiatry, 45(7), 917–922. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00253-4
  • Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 106–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010
  • Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kucera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.977
  • Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). How native-like is non-native language processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(12), 564–570. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.002
  • Coch, D., Grossi, G., Coffey-Corina, S., Holcomb, P., & Neville, H. (2002). A developmental investigation of ERP auditory rhyming effects. Developmental Science, 5(4), 467–489. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00241
  • Connolly, J. F., & Phillips, N. A. (1994). Event-related potential components reflect phonological and semantic processing of the terminal word of spoken sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(3), 256–266. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1994.6.3.256
  • Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 491–511. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.002
  • Desroches, A. S., Newman, R. L., & Joanisse, M. F. (2009). Investigating the time course of spoken word recognition: Electrophysiological evidence for the influences of phonological similarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 1893–1906. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21142
  • Dick, A. S., Garcia, N. L., Pruden, S. M., Thompson, W. K., Hawes, S. W., Sutherland, M. T., Riedel, M. C., Laird, A. R., & Gonzalez, R. (2019). No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the nationally representative ABCD study. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(7), 692–701. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0609-3
  • Dorman, M. F. (1974). Auditory evoked potential correlates of speech sound discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics, 15(2), 215–220. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213935
  • Duñabeitia, J. A., Dimitropoulou, M., Dowens, M. G., Molinaro, N., & Martin, C. (2016). The electrophysiology of the bilingual brain. In R. R. Heredia, J. Altarriba, & A. B. Cieślicka (Eds.), Methods in bilingual reading comprehension research (pp. 265–312). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-2993-1_11
  • Ehri, L., Nunes, S., Willows, D., Schuster, B., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the national reading panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36(3), 250–287. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.36.3.2
  • Evans, J. L., Gillam, R. B., & Montgomery, J. W. (2018). Cognitive predictors of spoken word recognition in children with and without developmental language disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(6), 1409–1425. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-17-0150
  • Friedman, D., Brown, C., Vaughan, H. G., Cornblatt, B., & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. (1984). Cognitive brain potential components in adolescents. Psychophysiology, 21(1), 83–96. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1984.tb02322.x
  • Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(11), 410–419. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01388-1
  • Giezen, M. R., Blumenfeld, H. K., Shook, A., Marian, V., & Emmorey, K. (2015). Parallel language activation and inhibitory control in bimodal bilinguals. Cognition, 141, 9–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.04.009
  • Golob, E. J., & Starr, A. (2000). Effects of stimulus sequence on event-related potentials and reaction time during target detection in Alzheimer’s disease. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111(8), 1438–1449. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00332-1
  • Gonthier, C., Macnamara, B. N., Chow, M., Conway, A. R., & Braver, T. S. (2016). Inducing proactive control shifts in the AX-CPT. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1822. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01822
  • Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377
  • Hillyard, S. A., & Anllo-Vento, L. (1998). Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 781–787. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.3.781
  • Holcomb, P. J., Coffey, S. A., & Neville, H. J. (1992). Visual and auditory sentence processing: A developmental analysis using event-related brain potentials. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8(2-3), 203–241. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649209540525
  • Huang, X., Yang, J. C., Zhang, Q., & Guo, C. (2014). The time course of spoken word recognition in Mandarin Chinese: A unimodal ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 63, 165–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.015
  • Johnson, R. (1989). Developmental evidence for modality-dependent P300 generators: A normative study. Psychophysiology, 26(6), 651–667. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1989.tb03167.x
  • Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T. (2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 416–430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.001
  • Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 621–647. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123
  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7350657
  • Lagrou, E., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W. (2015). Do semantic sentence constraint and L2 proficiency influence language selectivity of lexical access in native language listening? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(6), 1524–1538. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039782
  • Liang, L., & Chen, B. (2020). The impact of language proficiency on the time course and neural basis of L2 semantic access in bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24(4), 840–860. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006919897851
  • Liu, H., Zhang, Y., Blanco-Elorrieta, E., He, Y., & Chen, B. (2020). The role of proactive control on subcomponents of language control: Evidence from trilinguals. Cognition, 194, 104055. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104055
  • Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–621. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.795574
  • MacGinitie, W. H., MacGinitie, R. K., Maria, K., Dreyer, L. G., & Hughes, K. E. (2006). Gates-MacGinitie reading tests (GMRT). Riverside.
  • Malins, J. G., Gao, D., Tao, R., Booth, J. R., Shu, H., Joanisse, M. F., Liu, L., & Desroches, A. S. (2014). Developmental differences in the influence of phonological similarity on spoken word processing in Mandarin Chinese. Brain and Language, 138, 38–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2014.09.002
  • Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The language experience and proficiency questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940–967. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)
  • Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003a). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(2), 173–193. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716403000092
  • Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003b). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(2), 97–115. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728903001068
  • Mclaughlin, J., Tanner, D., Pitkänen, I., Frenck-Mestre, C., Inoue, K., Valentine, G., & Osterhout, L. (2010). Brain potentials reveal discrete stages of L2 grammatical learning. Language Learning, 60(s2), 123–150. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00604.x
  • Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S.-A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322–352. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026744
  • Midgley, K. J., Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J. (2009). Language effects in second language learners and proficient bilinguals investigated with event-related potentials. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22(3), 281–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2008.08.001
  • Morales, J., Gómez-Ariza, C. J., & Bajo, M. T. (2013). Dual mechanisms of cognitive control in bilinguals and monolinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 531–546. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.807812
  • Morales, J., Yudes, C., Gómez-Ariza, C. J., & Bajo, M. T. (2015). Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 66, 157–169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.014
  • Mueller, J. L. (2005). Electrophysiological correlates of second language processing. Second Language Research, 21(2), 152–174. http://www.jstor.org/stable/43103680
  • Näätänen, R., & Picton, T. (1987). The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: A review and an analysis of the component structure. Psychophysiology, 24(4), 375–425. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1987.tb00311.x
  • Naglieri, J. A. (1985). Matrix analogies test: Short form (MAT-SF). Psychological Corporation.
  • Newman, A. J., Tremblay, A., Nichols, E. S., Neville, H. J., & Ullman, M. T. (2012). The influence of language proficiency on lexical semantic processing in native and late learners of English. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(5), 1205–1223. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00143
  • Newman, R. L., Connolly, J. F., Service, E., & McIvor, K. (2003). Influence of phonological expectations during a phoneme deletion task: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 40(4), 640–647. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00065
  • Nichols, E. S., & Joanisse, M. F. (2017). Individual differences predict ERP signatures of second language learning of novel grammatical rules. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(1), 78–92. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000566
  • Oades, R. D., Dittmann-Balcar, A., & Zerbin, D. (1997). Development and topography of auditory event-related potentials (ERPs): Mismatch and processing negativity in individuals 8–22 years of age. Psychophysiology, 34(6), 677–693. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02143.x
  • Ojima, S., Matsuba-Kurita, H., Nakamura, N., Hoshino, T., & Hagiwara, H. (2011). Age and amount of exposure to a foreign language during childhood: Behavioral and ERP data on the semantic comprehension of spoken English by Japanese children. Neuroscience Research, 70(2), 197–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2011.01.018
  • Ojima, S., Nakata, H., & Kakigi, R. (2005). An ERP study of second language learning after childhood: Effects of proficiency. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(8), 1212–1228. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929055002436
  • Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014
  • Paxton, J. L., Barch, D. M., Racine, C. A., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and prefrontal function in healthy aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1010–1028. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm135
  • Payne, B. R., Ng, S., Shantz, K., & Federmeier, K. D. (2020). Chapter four – Event-related brain potentials in multilingual language processing: The N’s and P’s. In K. D. Federmeier & H.-W. Huang (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 72, pp. 75–118). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2020.03.003
  • Pérez, A., Hansen, L., & Bajo, T. (2018). The nature of first and second language processing: The role of cognitive control and L2 proficiency during text-level comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(5), 930–948. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000846
  • Perrachione, T. K., & Wong, P. C. (2007). Learning to recognize speakers of a non-native language: Implications for the functional organization of human auditory cortex. Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1899–1910. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.015
  • Polich, J., Ladish, C., & Burns, T. (1990). Normal variation of P300 in children: Age, memory span, and head size. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 9(3), 237–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(90)90056-J
  • Praamstra, P., & Stegeman, D. F. (1993). Phonological effects on the auditory N400 event-related brain potential. Cognitive Brain Research, 1(2), 73–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/0926-6410(93)90013-U
  • Pratt, H., Starr, A., Michalewski, H. J., Bleich, N., & Mittelman, N. (2008). The auditory P50 component to onset and offset of sound. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119(2), 376–387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.10.016
  • Ransdell, S., Arecco, R. M., & Levy, M. C. (2001). Bilingual long-term working memory: The effects of working memory loads on writing quality and fluency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22(1), 113–128. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716401001060
  • Reiterer, S., Pereda, E., & Bhattacharya, J. (2009). Measuring second language proficiency with EEG synchronization: How functional cortical networks and hemispheric involvement differ as a function of proficiency level in second language speakers. Second Language Research, 25(1), 77–106. http://www.jstor.org/stable/43104652
  • Rogers, R. L., Papanicolaou, A. C., Baumann, S. B., Saydjari, C., & Eisenberg, H. M. (1990). Neuromagnetic evidence of a dynamic excitation pattern generating the N100 auditory response. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section, 77(3), 237–240. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-5597(90)90043-D
  • Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 934–947. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.934
  • Sun, X., Li, L., Ding, G., Wang, R., & Li, P. (2019). Effects of language proficiency on cognitive control: Evidence from resting-state functional connectivity. Neuropsychologia, 129, 263–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.03.020
  • Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III tests of achievement. Riverside Publishing.
  • Xie, Z. (2018). The influence of second language (L2) proficiency on cognitive control among young adult unbalanced Chinese-English bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 412. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00412
  • Xue, J., Li, B., Yan, R., Gruen, J. R., Feng, T., Joanisse, M. F., & Malins, J. G. (2020). The temporal dynamics of first and second language processing: ERPs to spoken words in Mandarin-English bilinguals. Neuropsychologia, 146, 107562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107562

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.