2,161
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review Article

The merits of self-handicap: The handicap principle as an explanation of altruism compared to reciprocal altruism

& | (Reviewing Editor)
Article: 1140857 | Received 20 Oct 2015, Accepted 04 Jan 2016, Published online: 23 Feb 2016

References

  • Alvard, M. (2005). Comments on Bliege Bird and Smith’s signaling theory. Strategic Interaction, and Symbolic Capital, Current Anthropology, 46, 238–239.
  • Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  • Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211, 1390–1396.10.1126/science.7466396
  • Barkow, J. H. (1989). Darwin, sex, and status: Biological approaches to mind and culture. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
  • Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Hove: Erlbaum.
  • Batson, D. (1992). Experimental tests for the existence of altruism. PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association, 2, 69–78.
  • Behar, D. M., Richard, V., Soodyall, H., Blue-Smith, J., Pereira, L., Metspalu, E., … Rosset, S. (2008). The dawn of human matrilineal diversity. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 1–11.
  • Bliege Bird, R., & Smith, E. A. (2005). Signaling theory, strategic interaction, and symbolic capital. Current Anthropology, 46, 221–248.
  • Bliege Bird, R., Smith, E. A., & Bird, D. W. (2001). The hunting handicap: Costly signaling in human foraging strategies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50, 9–19.10.1007/s002650100338
  • Boone, J. L. (1998). The evolution of magnanimity. Human Nature, 9(1), 1–21.10.1007/s12110-998-1009-y
  • Bowser, B. J. (2000). From pottery to politics: An ethnoarchaeological study of political factionalism, the Ecuadorian Amazon. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 7, 219–248.10.1023/A:1026510620824
  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1990). Group selection among alternative evolutionarily stable strategies. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 145, 331–342.10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80113-4
  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13, 171–195.10.1016/0162-3095(92)90032-Y
  • Bshary, R., & Bergmüller, R. (2008). Distinguishing four fundamental approaches to the evolution of helping. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21, 405–420.10.1111/jeb.2008.21.issue-2
  • Carlisle, T. R., & Zahavi, A. (1986). Helping at the nest, allofeeding and social status in immature Arabian babblers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 18, 339–351.10.1007/BF00299665
  • Carpenter, C. R. (1942). Sexual behavior of free ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). I. Specimens, procedures and behavioral characteristics of estrus. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 33, 113–142.10.1037/h0058655
  • Churchill, S. E., Formicola, V., Holliday, T. W., Holt, B. M., & Schumann, B. A. (2000). The upper palaeolithic population of Europe in an evolutionary perspective. In W. Roebroeks, M. Mussi, J. Svoboda, & K. Fennema (Eds.), Hunters of the golden age: The mid upper palaeolithic of Eurasia, 30,000–20,000 BP (pp. 31–57). Leiden: University of Leiden.
  • Connor, R. C. (1986). Pseudo-reciprocity: Investing in mutualism. Animal Behaviour, 34, 1562–1566.10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80225-1
  • Darwin, C. (1859). On the origins of species. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Dattner, A. (2005). Allopreening in the Arabian babbler ( MSc thesis). Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv (in Hebrew, summary in English).
  • Dattner, A., Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (2015). Competition over guarding in the Arabian babbler (Turdoides squamiceps), a cooperative breeder. F1000Research, 4, 618. doi:10.12688/f1000research.6739.1
  • Dawkins, R. (1979). Twelve Misunderstandings of Kin selection. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 51, 184–200.
  • Denault, L. K., & McFarlane, D. A. (1995). Reciprocal altruism between male vampire bats, Desmodus rotundus. Animal Behaviour, 49, 855–856.10.1016/0003-3472(95)90062-4
  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1982). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes (Rev. ed.). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1989). Food sharing and reciprocal obligations among chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 18, 433–459.10.1016/0047-2484(89)90074-2
  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals (7th ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). The chimpanzee’s service economy: Evidence for cognition-based reciprocal exchange. In E. Ostrom & J. Walker (Eds.), Trust and reciprocity (pp. 128–143). New Haven, CT: Foundation Russell Sage.
  • Dugatkin, L. A. (1988). Do guppies play TIT FOR TAT during predator inspection visits? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 23, 395–399.10.1007/BF00303714
  • Dugatkin, L. A., & Alfieri, M. (1991). Guppies and the tit-for-tat strategy - Preference based on past interaction. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 28, 243–246.
  • Dugatkin, L. A. (1997). The evolution of cooperation. BioScience, 47, 355–362.10.2307/1313150
  • Ey, E., Pfefferle, D., & Fischer, J. (2007). Do age- and sex-related variations reliably reflect body size in non-human primate vocalizations? A review Primates, 48, 253–267.10.1007/s10329-006-0033-y
  • Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425, 785–791.10.1038/nature02043
  • Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415, 137–140.10.1038/415137a
  • Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. New York, NY: Norton.
  • Gintis, H. (2000). Strong reciprocity and human sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 206, 169–179.10.1006/jtbi.2000.2111
  • Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., & Fehr, E. (2003). Explaining altruistic behavior in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 153–172.10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00157-5
  • Gintis, H., Smith, E. A., & Bowles, S. (2001). Costly signaling and cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 213, 103–119.10.1006/jtbi.2001.2406
  • Grafen, A. (1990a). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144, 517–546.10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8
  • Grafen, A. (1990b). Sexual selection unhandicapped by the fisher process. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144, 473–516.10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80087-6
  • Grammer, K. (1996). Symptoms of power: The function and evolution of social status. In P. W. Wiessner & W. Schiefenheovel (Eds.), Food and the status quest: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 68–85). Providence, RI: Berghahn Books.
  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–16.10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4
  • Hammerstein, P. (2003). Why is reciprocity so rare in social animals? A protestant appeal. In P. Hammerstein (Ed.), Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation (pp. 83–93). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Harper, D. G. C. (2006). Maynard Smith: Amplifying the reasons for signal reliability. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 239, 203–209.10.1016/j.jtbi.2005.08.034
  • Harrison, C. J. O. (1965). Allopreening as agonistic behaviour. Behaviour, 24, 161–208.10.1163/156853965X00011
  • Hart, B. L., & Hart, L. A. (1992). Reciprocal allogrooming in impala, Aepyceros melampus. Animal Behaviour, 44, 1073–1083.10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80319-7
  • Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12, 29–54.10.1016/0162-3095(91)90011-E
  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. B. (2001). Hadza meat sharing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 113–142.10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00066-0
  • Hold-Cavell, B. (1996). The ethological basis of status hierarchies. In P. W. Wiessner & W. Schiefenheovel (Eds.), Food and the status quest: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 19–31). Providence, RI: Berghahn Books.
  • Johnstone, R. A. (1995). Sexual selection, honest advertisement and the handicap principle: Reviewing the evidence. Biological Reviews, 70, 1–65.10.1111/j.1469-185X.1995.tb01439.x
  • Kalishov, A., Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (2005). Allofeeding in Arabian babblers (Turdoides squamiceps). Journal of Ornithology, 146, 141–150.10.1007/s10336-005-0073-x
  • Kaplan, H., & Hill, K. (1985a). Food sharing among ache foragers: Tests of explanatory hypotheses [and comments and reply]. Current Anthropology, 26, 223–246.10.1086/203251
  • Kaplan, H., & Hill, K. (1985b). Hunting ability and reproductive success among male ache foragers: Preliminary results. Current Anthropology, 26, 131–133.10.1086/203235
  • Kober, K., & Gaston, A. (2003). Social interactions among breeding Brünnich’s Guillemots Uria lomvia suggest constraints in relation to offspring vulnerability. Ibis, 145, 413–418.10.1046/j.1474-919X.2003.00179.x
  • Koenig, W. D. (1988). Reciprocal altruism in birds: A critical review. Ethology and Sociobiology, 9, 73–84.10.1016/0162-3095(88)90014-3
  • Liao, X., Rong, S., & Queller, D. C. (2015). Relatedness, conflict, and the evolution of eusociality. PLOS Biology, 13, e1002098. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002098
  • Lee, R. B. (1979). The !Kung San: Men, women, and work in a foraging society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Matheson, M. D., & Bernstein, I. S. (2000). Grooming, social bonding, and agonistic aiding in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 51, 177–186.10.1002/(ISSN)1098-2345
  • Mellars, P. (2006). Going east: New genetic and archaeological perspectives on the modern human colonization of Eurasia. Science, 313, 796–800.10.1126/science.1128402
  • Milinski, M. (1987). Tit for tat in sticklebacks and the evolution of cooperation. Nature, 325, 433–435.10.1038/325433a0
  • Nowak, M. A., & Allen, B. (2015). Inclusive fitness theorizing invokes phenomena that are not relevant for the evolution of eusociality. PLoS Biology, 13(4), 1–5.
  • Nowak, M. A., Tarnita, C. E., & Wilson, E. O. (2010). The evolution of eusociality. Nature, 466, 1057–1062.10.1038/nature09205
  • Nowak, M., & Sigmund, K. (1998). Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature, 393, 373–377.
  • O’Brien, T. G. (1993). Allogrooming behaviour among adult female wedge-capped capuchin monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 46, 499–510.10.1006/anbe.1993.1218
  • Packer, C. (1977). Reciprocal altruism in Papio anubis. Nature, 265, 441–443.10.1038/265441a0
  • Pilot, M. (2005). Altruism as an advertisement—A model of the evolution of cooperation based on Zahavi’s handicap principle. Ethology Ecology and Evolution, 17, 217–231.10.1080/08927014.2005.9522593
  • Pusey, A., Williams, J., & Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science, 277, 828–831.10.1126/science.277.5327.828
  • Queller, D. C., Rong, S., & Liao, X. (2015). Some agreement on Kin selection and eusociality? PLOS Biology, 13(4), e1002133. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002133
  • Ridley, A. R.: 2003, The causes and consequences of helping behaviour in the cooperatively breeding Arabian babbler ( PhD Thesis). London: University of Cambridge.
  • Roberts, G. (1998). Competitive altruism: From reciprocity to the handicap principle. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265, 427–431.10.1098/rspb.1998.0312
  • Seyfarth, R., & Cheney, D. L. (1984). Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature, 308, 541–543.10.1038/308541a0
  • Smith, E. A. (2004). Why do good hunters have higher reproductive success? Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective, 15, 343–364.
  • Smith, E. A., Bliege Bird, R., & Bird, D. W. (2003). The benefits of costly signaling: Meriam turtle hunters. Behavioral Ecology, 14, 116–126.10.1093/beheco/14.1.116
  • Smith, E. A., & Bird, R. (2000). Turtle hunting and tombstone opening. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 245–261.10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00031-3
  • Smith, J. (1964). Group selection and Kin selection. Nature, 201, 1145–1147.10.1038/2011145a0
  • Smith, J. (1976). Group selection group selection in predator-prey communities M. E. Gilpin. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 51, 277–283.10.1086/409311
  • Smith, J. (1991). Honest signalling: The Philip Sidney game. Animal Behaviour, 42, 1034–1035.10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80161-7
  • Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (2000). Summary of “Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior”. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 185–206.
  • Sosis, R. (2000). Costly signaling and torch fishing on Ifaluk atoll. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 223–244.10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00030-1
  • Stammbach, E., & Kummer, H. (1982). Individual contributions to a dyadic interaction: An analysis of baboon grooming. Animal Behaviour, 30, 964–971.10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80184-X
  • Taylor, C. E., & McGuire, M. T. (1988). Reciprocal altruism: 15 years later. Ethology and Sociobiology, 9, 67–72.10.1016/0162-3095(88)90013-1
  • Terry, R. L. (1970). Primate grooming as a tension reduction mechanism. The Journal of Psychology, 76, 129–136.10.1080/00223980.1970.9916830
  • Trivers, R. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.10.1086/406755
  • Trivers, R. (1974). Parent-offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249–264.10.1093/icb/14.1.249
  • Turke, P. W., & Betzig, L. L. (1985). Those who can do: Wealth, status, and reproductive success on Ifaluk. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 79–87.10.1016/0162-3095(85)90001-9
  • West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007a). Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Current Biology, 17, R661–R672.10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.004
  • West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007b). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 20, 415–432.10.1111/jeb.2007.20.issue-2
  • West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2008). Social semantics: How useful has group selection been? Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21, 374–385.
  • Wiessner, P. (1984). Reconsidering the behavioral basis for style: A case study among the Kalahari San. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 3, 190–234.10.1016/0278-4165(84)90002-3
  • Wiessner, P. (1996). Introduction: Food, status, culture, and nature. In P. W. Wiessner & W. Schiefenheovel (Eds.), Food and the status quest: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 171–191). Providence, RI: Berghahn Books.
  • Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature, 308, 181–184.10.1038/308181a0
  • Wilkinson, G. S. (1988). Reciprocal altruism in bats and other mammals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 9, 85–100.10.1016/0162-3095(88)90015-5
  • Wilson, D. S. (1977). Structured demes and the evolution of group-advantageous traits. The American Naturalist, 111, 157–185.10.1086/283146
  • Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking the theoretical foundations of sociobiology. Quarterly Review of Biology, 82, 328–348.
  • Wynne-Edwards, V. C. (1962). Animal dispersion in relation to social behaviour. Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd.
  • Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection—A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–214.10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3
  • Zahavi, A. (1977a). Reliability in communication systems and the evolution of altruism. In B. Stonehouse & C. Perrins (Eds.), Evolutionary ecology (pp. 253–259). London: Macmillan.
  • Zahavi, A. (1977b). The testing of a bond. Animal Behaviour, 25, 246–247.10.1016/0003-3472(77)90089-6
  • Zahavi, A. (1995). Altruism as a handicap: The limitations of Kin selection and reciprocity. Journal of Avian Biology, 26(1), 1–3.10.2307/3677205
  • Zahavi, A. (1999). Babbler altruism and Kin selection: A reply to Jon. Journal of Avian Biology, 30, 115.10.2307/3677251
  • Zahavi, A. (2003). Indirect selection and individual selection in sociobiology: My personal views on theories of social behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 65, 859–863.10.1006/anbe.2003.2109
  • Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Zacharias, V. J., & Mathew, D. N. (1998). Behaviour of the whiteheaded babbler (Turdoides affinis). Journal of Bombay Natural History Society, 95, 8–14.