473
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Verbal and visual cognition: Individual differences in the lab, in the brain, and in the classroom

&

References

  • Alloway, T. P. (2009). Working memory, but not IQ, predicts subsequent learning in children with learning difficulties. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25(2), 92–98. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.25.2.92
  • Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77(6), 1698–1716. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00968.x
  • Archibald, L. M. D., & Alloway, T. P. (2008). Comparing language profiles: Children with specific language impairment and developmental coordination disorder. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(2), 165–180. doi:10.1080/13682820701422809
  • Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short‐term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(6), 675–693. doi:10.1080/13682820500442602
  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829–839.
  • Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 1–29. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422
  • Baldo, J., Dronkers, N., Wilkins, D., Ludy, C., Raskin, P., & Kim, J. (2005). Is problem solving dependent on language? Brain and Language, 92(3), 240–250. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.06.103
  • Buzzell, G. A., Roberts, D. M., Baldwin, C. L., & McDonald, C. G. (2013). An electrophysiological correlate of conflict processing in an auditory spatial Stroop task: The effect of individual differences in navigational style. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 90(2), 265–271. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.08.008
  • Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies (1St Edition). Cambridge University Press.
  • Chiou, R., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2016). The anterior temporal cortex is a primary semantic source of top-down influences on object recognition. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 79, 75–86. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.007
  • Colom, R., Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2006). Distributed brain sites for the g-factor of intelligence. NeuroImage, 31(3), 1359–1365. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.006
  • Coutanche, M. N., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2015). Creating concepts from converging features in human cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 25(9), 2584–2593. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu057
  • Deary, I. J., Penke, L., & Johnson, W. (2010). The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(3), 201–211. doi:10.1038/nrn2793
  • Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith, P., & Fernandes, C. (2007). Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence, 35(1), 13–21. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001
  • Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., Van Valin, R. D. Jr,, Redfern, B. B., & Jaeger, J. J. (2004). Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition, 92(1–2), 145–177.
  • Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In dividual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence. In A. Miyake, & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 102–134). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139174909.007
  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 309–331. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309
  • Fedorenko, E., Behr, M. K., & Kanwisher, N. (2011). Functional specificity for high-level linguistic processing in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(39), 16428–16433. doi:10.1073/pnas.1112937108
  • Fedorenko, E., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). Reworking the language network. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3), 120–126. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.006
  • Fiebach, C. J., Rissman, J., & D’Esposito, M. (2006). Modulation of inferotemporal cortex activation during verbal working memory maintenance. Neuron, 51(2), 251–261. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.007
  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2000). Differential roles for visuospatial and verbal working memory in situation model construction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), 61–83. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.129.1.61
  • Fürst, A. J., & Hitch, G. J. (2000). Separate roles for executive and phonological components of working memory in mental arithmetic. Memory & Cognition, 28(5), 774–782. doi:10.3758/BF03198412
  • Gathercole, S. E., & Alloway, T. P. (2008). Working memory and learning: A practical guide for teachers. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
  • Gathercole, S. E., Tiffany, C., Briscoe, J., Thorn, A. S. C., Team, A., et al. (2005). Developmental consequences of phonological loop deficits during early childhood: A longitudinal study. Journal Children Psychologist Psychiatry, 46(6), 598–611.
  • Gathercole, S. E., Lamont, E., & Alloway, T. P. (2006). Working memory in the classroom. In Working memory and education (pp. 219–240). Elsevier. doi:10.1016/B978-012554465-8/50010-7.
  • Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6(3), 316–322. doi:10.1038/nn1014
  • Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S., & Funnell, E. (1992). Semantic dementia. Progressive Fluent Aphasia with Temporal Lobe Atrophy. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 115(Pt 6), 1783–1806.
  • Hsu, N. S., Kraemer, D. J. M., Oliver, R. T., Schlichting, M. L., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2011). Color, context, and cognitive style: Variations in color knowledge retrieval as a function of task and subject variables. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2544–2557. doi:10.1162/jocn.2011.21619
  • Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(2), 135. doi:10.1017/S0140525X07001185
  • Kirchhoff, B. A., & Buckner, R. L. (2006). Functional-anatomic correlates of individual differences in memory. Neuron, 51(2), 263–274. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.006
  • Klein, I., Paradis, A.-L., Poline, J.-B., Kosslyn, S. M., & Le Bihan, D. (2000). Transient activity in the human calcarine cortex during visual-mental imagery: An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(supplement 2), 15–23. doi:10.1162/089892900564037
  • Kozhevnikov, M., Kosslyn, S., & Shephard, J. (2005). Spatial versus object visualizers: A new characterization of visual cognitive style. Memory & Cognition, 33(4), 710–726.
  • Kraemer, D. J. M., Hamilton, R. H., Messing, S. B., DeSantis, J. H., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). Cognitive style, cortical stimulation, and the conversion hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–9. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00015
  • Kraemer, D. J. M., Rosenberg, L. M., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). The neural correlates of visual and verbal cognitive styles. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(12), 3792–3798. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4635-08.2009
  • Kraemer, D. J. M., Schinazi, V. R., Cawkwell, P. B., Epstein, R. A., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2016). Verbalizing, visualizing, and navigating: The effect of strategies on encoding a large-scale virtual environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(4), 611.
  • Lee, K. H., Choi, Y. Y., Gray, J. R., Cho, S. H., Chae, J.-H., Lee, S., & Kim, K. (2006). Neural correlates of superior intelligence: Stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. NeuroImage, 29(2), 578–586. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.036
  • Li, S., Gong, D., Jia, S., Zhang, W., & Ma, Y. (2011). Object and spatial visualizers have different object-processing patterns: Behavioral and ERP evidence. NeuroReport, 22(17), 860–864. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834b6d3b
  • Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock, R. (2005). A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(4), 377–384.
  • Martinussen, R., & Tannock, R. (2006). Working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with and without comorbid language learning disorders. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(7), 1073–1094.
  • Massa, L. J., & Mayer, R. E. (2006). Testing the ATI hypothesis: Should multimedia instruction accommodate verbalizer-visualizer cognitive style? Learning and Individual Differences, 16(4), 321–335. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2006.10.001
  • McLean, J. F., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(3), 240–260. doi:10.1006/jecp.1999.2516
  • Miller, M. B., Donovan, C.-L., Bennett, C. M., Aminoff, E. M., & Mayer, R. E. (2012). Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between individuals. NeuroImage, 59(1), 83–93. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.060
  • Miller, M. B., Donovan, C.-L., Van Horn, J. D., German, E., Sokol-Hessner, P., & Wolford, G. L. (2009). Unique and persistent individual patterns of brain activity across different memory retrieval tasks. NeuroImage, 48(3), 625–635. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.033
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Rettinger, D. A., Shah, P., & Hegarty, M. (2001). How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 621–640. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.621
  • Motes, M. A., Malach, R., & Kozhevnikov, M. (2008). Object-processing neural efficiency differentiates object from spatial visualizers. NeuroReport, 19(17), 1727–1731. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e328317f3e2
  • Oliveri, S., Incorpora, C., Genevini, M., Santagostino, L., Tettamanti, L., Antonietti, A., & Risoli, A. (2012). Clinical investigation of cognitive styles in patients with acquired brain damage. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 22(3), 362–373. doi:10.1080/09602011.2011.647416
  • Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., … Ballard, C. G. (2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465(7299), 775–778. doi:10.1038/nature09042
  • Palmiero, M., Nori, R., & Piccardi, L. (2016). Visualizer cognitive style enhances visual creativity. Neuroscience Letters, 615, 98–101. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2016.01.032
  • Park, G., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2007). Contrasting intellectual patterns predict creativity in the arts and sciences: tracking intellectually precocious youth over 25 years. Psychological Science, 18(11), 948–952. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02007.x
  • Pashler, H., McDaniel, M., Rohrer, D., & Bjork, R. (2008). Learning styles: Concepts and evidence. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9(3), 105–119.
  • Ranganath, C., Cohen, M. X., Dam, C., & D’Esposito, M. (2004). Inferior temporal, prefrontal, and hippocampal contributions to visual working memory maintenance and associative memory retrieval. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(16), 3917–3925. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5053-03.2004
  • Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000). Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Section 3: The standard progressive matrices. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
  • Rogers, T. T., Graham, K. S., & Patterson, K. (2015). Semantic impairment disrupts perception, memory, and naming of secondary but not primary colours. Neuropsychologia, 70, 296–308. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.010
  • Rogers, T. T., Patterson, K., & Graham, K. (2007). Colour knowledge in semantic dementia: It is not all black and white. Neuropsychologia, 45(14), 3285–3298. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.020
  • Rogowsky, B. A., Calhoun, B. M., & Tallal, P. (2015). Matching learning style to instructional method: Effects on comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 64–78. doi:10.1037/a0037478
  • Rohde, T. E., & Thompson, L. A. (2007). Predicting academic achievement with cognitive ability. Intelligence, 35(1), 83–92.
  • Rourke, B. P., & Finlayson, M. A. (1978). Neuropsychological significance of variations in patterns of academic performance: Verbal and visual-spatial abilities. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6(1), 121–133.
  • Ryan, R. S., & Schooler, J. W. (1998). Whom do words hurt? Individual differences in susceptibility to verbal overshadowing. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12(7), S105–S125.
  • Sahyoun, C. P., Belliveau, J. W., & Mody, M. (2010). White matter integrity and pictorial reasoning in high-functioning children with autism. Brain and Cognition, 73(3), 180–188. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.05.002
  • Sahyoun, C. P., Belliveau, J. W., Soulières, I., Schwartz, S., & Mody, M. (2010). Neuroimaging of the functional and structural networks underlying visuospatial vs. linguistic reasoning in high-functioning autism. Neuropsychologia, 48(1), 86–95. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.013
  • Sahyoun, C. P., Soulières, I., Belliveau, J. W., Mottron, L., & Mody, M. (2009). Cognitive differences in pictorial reasoning between high-functioning autism and asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(7), 1014–1023. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0712-9
  • Salthouse, T. A. (2004). Localizing age-related individual differences in a hierarchical structure. Intelligence, 32, 6. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.003
  • Seufert, T., Schütze, M., & Brünken, R. (2009). Memory characteristics and modality in multimedia learning: An aptitude–treatment–interaction study. Learning and Instruction, 19(1), 28–42. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.01.002
  • Shah, P., & Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 125(1), 4–26.
  • Shin, G., & Kim, C. (2015). Neural correlates of cognitive style and flexible cognitive control. NeuroImage, 113, 78–85. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.046
  • Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 628–654. doi:10.1037/a0027473
  • Soulières, I., Dawson, M., Samson, F., Barbeau, E. B., Sahyoun, C. P., Strangman, G. E., … Mottron, L. (2009). Enhanced visual processing contributes to matrix reasoning in autism. Human Brain Mapping, 30(12), 4082–4107. doi:10.1002/hbm.20831
  • Swanson, H. L., & Howell, M. (2001). Working memory, short-term memory, and speech rate as predictors of children’s reading performance at different ages. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(4), 720–734. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.4.720
  • Sweller, J., Merrienboer, J. J. G. V., & Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.
  • Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of semantic memory: Inferring “how” from “where. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 280–292. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00161-6
  • Turley-Ames, K., & Whitfield, M. M. (2003). Strategy training and working memory task performance. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(4), 446–468. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00095-0
  • Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28(2), 127–154. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(89)90040-5
  • Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2005). Creativity and occupational accomplishments among intellectually precocious youths: An age 13 to age 33 longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(3), 484–492. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.484
  • Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 817.
  • Wai, J., Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P., & Steiger, J. H. (2010). Accomplishment in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) and its relation to STEM educational dose: A 25-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(4), 860.
  • Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson, 22, 498.
  • Zarnhofer, S., Braunstein, V., Ebner, F., Koschutnig, K., Neuper, C., Ninaus, M., … Ischebeck, A. (2013). Individual differences in solving arithmetic word problems. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 9(1), 28.
  • Zarnhofer, S., Braunstein, V., Ebner, F., Koschutnig, K., Neuper, C., Reishofer, G., & Ischebeck, A. (2012). The influence of verbalization on the pattern of cortical activation during mental arithmetic. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 8(1), 13.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.