281
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Validation of the ToMas-child Scale for the Assessment of the Theory of Mind in a Group of Spanish Speaking Children Aged 3 to 7 Years from Spain

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 232-245 | Received 23 Dec 2019, Accepted 25 Apr 2020, Published online: 18 May 2020

References

  • Aboulafia‐Brakha, T., Christe, B., Martory, M.-D., & Annoni, J.-M. (2011). Theory of mind tasks and executive functions: A systematic review of group studies in neurology. Journal of Neuropsychology, 5(1), 39–55. doi:10.1348/174866410X533660
  • Achim, A. M., Guitton, M., Jackson, P. L., Boutin, A., & Monetta, L. (2013). On what ground do we mentalize? Characteristics of current tasks and sources of information that contribute to mentalizing judgments. Psychological Assessment, 25(1), 117–126. doi:10.1037/a0029137
  • Adams, C., Baxendale, J., Lloyd, J., & Aldred, C. (2005). Pragmatic language impairment: Case studies of social and pragmatic language therapy. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 21(3), 227–250. doi: 10.1191/0265659005ct290oa
  • Amsterlaw, J., Lagattuta, K. H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2009). Young children’s reasoning about the effects of emotional and physiological states on academic performance. Child Development, 80(1), 115–133. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01249.x
  • Andrich, D., Lyne, A., Sheridan, B., & Luo, G. (2003). RUMM2020. RUMM Laboratory. http://www.rummlab.com.au
  • Aryadoust, V., Tan, H. A. H., & Ng, L. Y. (2019). a scientometric review of rasch measurement: The rise and progress of a specialty. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02197
  • Astington, J. W., & Baird, J. A. (Eds.). (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Baghaei, P. (2008). Local Dependency and rasch measures. Rasch Measurement Transactions, 21(3), 1105–1156.
  • Banerjee, M. (1997). Hidden emotions: Preschoolers’ knowledge of appearance-reality and emotion display rules. Social Cognition, 15(2), 107–132. doi: 10.1521/soco.1997.15.2.107
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8
  • Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of Faux Pas by normally developing children and children with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(5), 407–418. doi:10.1023/A:1023035012436
  • Bakhshipour, A., Mahmood Aliloo, M., Shahrokhi, H., Hashemi, T., Amiri, S., Mehdizadeh Fanid, L., Yadegari, N., & Hagnazari, F. (2012). Sequences of Mind Development in Boys with Autism Spectrum Disorder. ISRN Neurology, 2012, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.5402/2012/637453
  • Beaudoin, C., Leblanc, É., Gagner, C., & Beauchamp, M. H. (2020). Systematic review and inventory of theory of mind measures for young children. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2905. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02905
  • Bode, R. K., Heinemann, A. W., & Semik, P. (2000). Measurement properties of the Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test (GOAT) and improvement patterns during inpatient rehabilitation. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 15(1), 637–655. doi:10.1097/00001199-200002000-00004
  • Burack, J. A., Charman, T., Yirmiya, N., & Zelazo, P. R. (2001). The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research. Mahwah, NJ: Routledge.
  • Cano, S. J., & Hobart, J. C. (2011). The problem with health measurement. Patient Preference and Adherence, 5, 279–290. doi:10.2147/PPA.S14399
  • Caracuel, A., Bateman, A., Teasdale, T. W., Verdejo-García, A., & Pérez-García, M. (2011). Spanish, French, and British cross-cultural validation of the European brain injury questionnaire. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 26(6), Scopus, 478–488. doi:10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181fc042c
  • Cassel, A., McDonald, S., Kelly, M., & Togher, L. (2016). Learning from the minds of others: A review of social cognition treatments and their relevance to traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 1–34. doi:10.1080/09602011.2016.1257435
  • Corcoran, R., Mercer, G., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: Investigating “theory of mind” in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 17(1), 5–13. doi:10.1016/0920-9964(95)00024-G
  • Davis, T. L. (2001). Children’s understanding of false beliefs in different domains: Affective vs. physical. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19(1), 47–58. doi: 10.1348/026151001165958
  • de Rosnay, M., Pons, F., Harris, P. L., & Morrell, J. M. B. (2004). A lag between understanding false belief and emotion attribution in young children: Relationships with linguistic ability and mothers’ mental-state language. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 197–218. doi:10.1348/026151004323044573
  • Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3(2), Scopus, 71–100. doi:10.1177/1534582304267187
  • Ebert, S. (2020). Theory of mind, language, and reading: Developmental relations from early childhood to early adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 191, 104739. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104739
  • Elder, H., Czuba, K., Kersten, P., Caracuel, A., & McPherson, K. (2017). Te Waka Kuaka, Rasch analysis of a cultural assessment tool in traumatic brain injury in Māori. F1000Research, 6, 1034. doi:10.12688/f1000research.11500.1
  • Embretson, S. E., & Reise, S. P. (2000). Item response theory for psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Fernández, C. (2011). Mindful storytellers: Emerging pragmatics and theory of mind development. First Language. doi:10.1177/0142723711422633
  • Fisher, W., Jr.. (1992). Reliability Statistics. Rasch Measurement Transactions, 6(3), 238.
  • Gardner, D., Harris, P. L., & Ohmoto, M. (1988). Japanese children’s understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11(2), 203–218. Scopus. https://doi.org/10.1177/016502548801100204
  • Grimby, G., Tennant, A., & Tesio, L. (2012). The use of raw scores from ordinal scales: Time to end malpractice? Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 44(2), 97–98. doi:10.2340/16501977-0938
  • Hagquist, C., Bruce, M., & Gustavsson, J. P. (2009). Using the Rasch model in nursing research: An introduction and illustrative example. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 46(3), 380–393. doi:10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2008.10.007
  • Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154. doi:10.1007/BF02172093
  • Harris, P. L., Donnelly, K., Guz, G. R., & Pitt-Watson, R. (1986). Children’s understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. Child Development, 57(4), JSTOR, 895–909. doi:10.2307/1130366
  • Harris, P. L., Johnson, C. N., Hutton, D., Andrews, G., & Cooke, T. (1989). Young Children’s Theory of Mind and Emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 3(4), 379–400. doi:10.1080/02699938908412713
  • Hayward, E. O., & Homer, B. D. (2017). Reliability and validity of advanced theory-of-mind measures in middle childhood and adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 35(3), 454–462. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12186
  • Hiller, R. M., Weber, N., & Young, R. L. (2014). The validity and scalability of the theory of mind scale with toddlers and preschoolers. Psychological Assessment, 26(4), 1388–1393. doi:10.1037/a0038320
  • Howe, N., Ross, H. S., & Recchia, H. (2011). Sibling relations in early and middle childhood. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood social development (2nd ed.). (356–372). New York: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9781444390933.ch19.
  • Hughes, C., Devine, R. T., & Wang, Z. (2018). Does Parental Mind-Mindedness Account for Cross-Cultural Differences in Preschoolers’ Theory of Mind? Child Development, 89(4), 1296–1310. doi:10.1111/cdev.12746
  • Joshi, M. S., & MacLean, M. (1994). Indian and English children’s understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. Child Development, 65(5), 1372–1384. JSTOR. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131504 5 doi:10.2307/1131504
  • Kuntoro, I. A., Peterson, C. C., & Slaughter, V. (2017). Culture, parenting, and children’s theory of mind development in Indonesia. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 48(9), 1389–1409. doi:10.1177/0022022117725404
  • Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 259–289. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654
  • Linacre, J. M. (1994). Sample size and item calibration stability. Rasch Measurement Transactions, 7(4), 328.
  • Linacre, J. M. (2002). Understanding rasch measurement: Optimizing rating scale category effectiveness. Journal of Applied Measurement, 3(1), 85–106.
  • Matthews, D., Biney, H., & Abbot-Smith, K. (2018). Individual differences in children’s pragmatic ability: A review of associations with formal language, social cognition, and executive functions. Language Learning and Development, 14(3), 186–223. doi:10.1080/15475441.2018.1455584
  • Mcdonald, S., & Cassel, A. (2017). Rehabilitation of social cognition disorders. In B. A. Wilson, J. Winegardner, & T. Ownsworth (Eds.), Neuropsychological rehabilitation: The International handbook (pp. 266–281). London: Taylor & Francis.
  • Miller, S. A. (2012). Theory of mind: Beyond the preschool years. New York: Psychology Press.
  • Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), Scopus, 622–646. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x
  • Molenberghs, P., Johnson, H., Henry, J. D., & Mattingley, J. B. (2016). Understanding the minds of others: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 65, 276–291. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.020
  • Molina, P., Bulgarelli, D., Henning, A., & Aschersleben, G. (2014). Emotion understanding: A cross-cultural comparison between Italian and German preschoolers. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(5), 592–607. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2014.890585
  • O’Connor, A. M., & Evans, A. D. (2019). The role of theory of mind and social skills in predicting children’s cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 179, 337–347. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2018.11.018
  • Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5(2), 125–137. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1987.tb01048.x
  • Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory-of-mind development for children with deafness or autism. Child Development, 76(2), 502–517. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00859.x
  • Pillow, B. H. (1989). Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47(1), 116–129. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(89)90066-0
  • Pons, F., & Harris, P. (2000). Test of emotion comprehension: TEC. University of Oxford.
  • Pons, F., Harris, P. L., & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1(2), 127–152. doi:10.1080/17405620344000022
  • Pons, F., Lawson, J., Harris, P. L., & de Rosnay, M. (2003). Individual differences in children’s emotion understanding: Effects of age and language. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 44(4), 347–353. doi:10.1111/1467-9450.00354
  • Pratt, C., & Bryant, P. (1990). Young children understand that looking leads to knowing (So long as they are looking into a single barrel). Child Development, 61(4), JSTOR, 973–982. doi:10.2307/1130869
  • Prime, H., Plamondon, A., Pauker, S., Perlman, M., & Jenkins, J. M. (2016). Sibling cognitive sensitivity as a moderator of the relationship between sibship size and children’s theory of mind: A longitudinal analysis. Cognitive Development, 39, 93–102. Scopus. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.03.005
  • Puyuelo Sanclemente, M. (Ed.). (2007). BLOC screening-R: Evaluación del lenguaje (Nueva ed. revisada y ampliada). Barcelona: Elsevier Masson.
  • Rasch, G. (1980). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Repacholi, B., & Slaughter, V. (Eds.). (2003). Individual differences in theory of mind: Implications for typical and atypical development. Taylor & Francis Group. http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/ugr/detail.action?docID=183308
  • Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 12–21. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12
  • Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., & Wellman, H. M. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1239–1247. doi:10.1037/a0023899
  • Smith, R. M. (2000). Fit analysis in latent trait measurement models. Journal of Applied Measurement, 1(2), 199–218.
  • Sprung, M. (2010). Clinically relevant measures of children’s theory of mind and knowledge about thinking: Non-standard and advanced measures. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 15(4), 204–216. doi:10.1111/j.1475-3588.2010.00568.x
  • Tang, Y., Harris, P. L., Pons, F., Zou, H., Zhang, W., & Xu, Q. (2017). The Understanding of emotion among young Chinese Children. International Journal of Behavioral Development. doi: 10.1177/0165025417741366.
  • Tennant, A., & Conaghan, P. G. (2007). The rasch measurement model in rheumatology: What is it and why use it? When should it be applied, and what should one look for in a rasch paper? Arthritis & Rheumatism, 57(8), 1358–1362. doi:10.1002/art.23108
  • Tennant, A., Penta, M., Tesio, L., Grimby, G., Thonnard, J.-L., Slade, A., … Phillips, S. (2004). Assessing and adjusting for cross-cultural validity of impairment and activity limitation scales through differential item functioning within the framework of the rasch model. Medical CAre, 42(1 Supplement), 1–37. doi:10.1097/01.mlr.0000103529.63132.77
  • Tesio, L. (2003). Measuring behaviours and perceptions: Rasch analysis as a tool for rehabilitation research. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine: Official Journal of the UEMS European Board of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 35(3), 105–115. doi:10.1080/16501970310010448
  • Vonk, J., Jett, S. E., Tomeny, T. S., Mercer, S. H., & Cwikla, J. (2020). Young children’s theory of mind predicts more sharing with friends over time. Child Development, 91(1), 63–77. doi:10.1111/cdev.13112
  • Walz, N. C., Yeates, K. O., Taylor, H. G., Stancin, T., & Wade, S. L. (2009). First-order theory of mind skills shortly after traumatic brain injury in 3- to 5-year-old children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(4), 507–519. doi:10.1080/87565640902964490
  • Wellman, H. M., & Bartsch, K. (1988). Young children’s reasoning about beliefs. Cognition, 30(3), 239–277. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(88)90021-2
  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00304
  • Wellman, H. M., Fang, F., Liu, D., Zhu, L., & Liu, G. (2006). Scaling of theory-of-mind understandings in chinese children. Psychological Science, 17(12), 1075–1081. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01830.x
  • Wellman, H. M., Fang, F., & Peterson, C. C. (2011). Sequential progressions in a theory-of-mind scale: Longitudinal perspectives: Longitudinal sequences of development. Child Development, 82(3), 780–792. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01583.x
  • Wellman, H. M., Hollander, M., & Schult, C. A. (1996). Young children’s understanding of thought bubbles and of thoughts. Child Development, 67(3), JSTOR, 768–788. doi:10.2307/1131860
  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x
  • Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35(3), 245–275. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90024-E
  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
  • Wright, B. D., & Masters, G. N. (1982). Rating scale analysis. Chicago: MESA Press.
  • Wright, B. D., & Stone, M. (1979). Best test design. Chicago: MESA Press.
  • Zilber, A. (2017). Teorías acerca de la Teoría de la Mente. El rol de los procesos cognitivos y emocionales. Neuropsicologia Latinoamericana, 9(3), Article 3. http://206.167.239.107/index.php/Neuropsicologia_Latinoamericana/article/view/397

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.