921
Views
13
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Prospectives / Prospectivas

Model based reasoning and the learning of counter-intuitive science concepts

Razonamiento basado en modelos y el aprendizaje de conceptos científicos contra intuitivos

Pages 5-33 | Received 01 Jul 2012, Accepted 20 Sep 2012, Published online: 23 Jan 2014

References

  • Acher, A., Arcá, M. & Sanmartí, N. (2007). Modeling as a teaching learning process for understanding materials. A case study in primary education. Science Education, 91, 398–418.
  • Bowker, G. & Star, S. L. (1999). Sorting things out: Classification and its consequences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Carey, S., Evans, R., Honda, M., Unger, C. & Jay, E. (1989). An experiment is when you try and see if it works: middle school conception of science. International Journal of Science Education, 11, 514–529.
  • Chi, M., Feltovitch, P. & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation in physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152.
  • Clement, J. (2008). The role of explanatory models in teaching for conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), The international handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 417–452), New York: Routledge.
  • Duschl, R. A., Schweinruber, H. A. & Shouse, A. W. (2007). Taking science to school: Learning and teaching science in grades K-8. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
  • Ehrlen, K. (2007). Conceptions and artefacts: Children's understanding of the earth in the presence of visual representations. Doctoral Thesis in Education. Stockholm University, Sweden.
  • Giere, R. (1988). Explaining science: A cognitive approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Greeno J. (1988). Situations, mental models and generative knowledge. Palo Alto, CA: Institute for Research on Learning.
  • Grosslight, L., Unger, C., Jay, E. & Smith, C. (1991). Understanding models and their use in science. Conceptions of middle and high school students and experts. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28, 799–822.
  • Halldén, O., Scheja, M. & Haglund, L. (2008). The contextuality of knowledge. An intentional approach to meaning making and conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 509–532). London: Taylor & Francis Group, Inc.
  • Hatano, G. (1994). Introduction. Human Development, 37, 189–197.
  • Hatano, G. & Inagaki, K. (2003). When is conceptual change intended? A cognitive-sociocultural view. In G. M. Sinatra & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Intentional conceptual change (pp. 407–427). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Hesse, M. (1966). Models and analogies in science. Notre Dame, IN: University of Indiana Press.
  • Hoffer, B. & Pintrich, P. (1997). The development of epistemological theories: Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67 (1), 88–140.
  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Ivarsson, T., Gillberg, C., Arvidsson, T. & Broberg, A. G. (2002). The Youth Self-Report (YSR) and the Depression Self-Rating Scale (DSRS) as measures of depression and suicidality among adolescents. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 11, 31–37.
  • Kobayashi, A. (1994). Coloring the field: gender, ‘race’ and the politics of fieldwork. The Professional Geographer 46, 73–80.
  • Kozma, R., Russell, J., Jones, J., Marx, N. & Davis, J. (1996). The use of multiple, linked representations to facilitate science understanding. In S. Vosniadou, E. de Corte, R. Glaser & H. Mandl (Eds.), International perspectives on the design of technology-supported learning Environments (pp. 41–60). Mahawh, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Kuhn, D., Amsel E. & O'loughlin, M. (1988). The development of scientific thinking skills. Orlando, FL: Academic.
  • Kyriakopoulou, N. & Vosniadou, S. (2012). The relation between conceptual change in physical science, theory of mind and personal epistemology and implications for science instruction. Paper presented in the 8th International Conference on Conceptual Change, September 1–4, University of Trier, Germany.
  • Larkin, J. (1983). The role of problem representation in physics. In D. Gentner & A. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models (pp. 75–98). Hillsdale: NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Lehrer, R., Carpenter, S., Shauble, L. & Putz, A. (2000). Designing classrooms that support inquiry. In J. Ministrel L & E. van Zee (Eds.), Inquiring into inquiry learning and teaching in science (pp. 80–99). Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science.
  • Lehrer, R., Schäuble, L., Strom, D. & Pligge, M. (2001). Similarity of form and substance: Modeling material kind. In S. Carver & D. Klahr (Eds.), Cognition and instruction: Twenty-five years of progress (pp. 39–74). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Merenluoto, K. & Lehtinen, E. (2002). Conceptual change in mathematics: Understanding the real numbers. In M. Limón & L. Mason (Eds.), Reconsidering conceptual change: Issues in theory and practice (pp. 233–258). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
  • Merenluoto, K. & Palonen, T. (2007). When we clashed with the real numbers: Complexity of conceptual change in number concept. In S. Vosniadou, A. Baltas & X. Vamvakoussi (Eds.), Reframing the conceptual change approach in learning and instruction (pp. 247–263). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
  • Morgan, M. S. & Morrison, M. (Eds.) (1999). Models as mediators: Perspectives on natural and social science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Nersessian, N. J. (2008). Mental modelling, in conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change (pp. 391–416). New York: Routledge.
  • Raghavan, K. & Glaser, R. (1995). Model-based analysis and reasoning in science: The MARS curriculum. Science Education, 79, 37–61.
  • Rogoff, B. (2012). Learning without lessons: Opportunities to expand knowledge. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 35 (2), 233–241. [Trans. into Spanish by A. Bautista & B. Rogoff, Aprender sin lecciones: oportunidades para expandir el conocimiento. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 35 (2), 242–250].
  • Roth, W. M. (2001). Situating Cognition. The Journal of Learning Sciences, 10 (1), 27–61.
  • Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E. & Williams, R. J. (1986). Learning represen-tations by back-propagating errors. Nature 323, 533–536.
  • Säljö, R. (1994). Minding action: conceiving of the world versus participating in cultural practices. Journal of Nordic Educational Research, 14, 71–80.
  • Säljö, R., Schoultz, J. & Wyndhamn, J. (2001). Heavenly talk. A discursive approach to conceptual knowledge and conceptual change in children's understanding of elementary astronomy. Human Development, 44, 103–118.
  • Samarapungavan, A., Vosniadou, S. & Brewer, W. F. (1998). mental models of the earth, sun and the moon: Indian children's cosmologies. Cognitive Development, 11, 491–521.
  • Schoultz, J., Säljö, R. & Wyndhamn, J. (2001). Heavenly talk: discourse, artifacts, and children's understanding of elementary astronomy. Human Development, 44, 103–118.
  • Smith, C. L., Maclin, D., Houghton, C. & Hennesey, M. G. (2000). Sixth-grade students' epistemologies of science: The impact of school experiences on epistemological development. Cognition and Instruction, 18 (3), 349–422.
  • Smith, C., & Wenk, L. (2006). Relations among three aspects of first-year college students epistemologies of science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43 (8), 747–785.
  • Smith, C., Maclin, D., Grosslight, L. & Davis, H. (1997). Teaching for understanding: A comparison of two approaches to teaching students about matter and density. Cognition and Instruction, 15 (3), 317–393.
  • Smith, C., Snir, J. & Grosslight, L. (1992). Using conceptual models to facilitate conceptual change: The case of weight/density differentiation. Cognition and Instruction, 9 (3), 221–83.
  • Vamvakoussi,. & Vosniadou, S. (2010). How many decimals are there between two fractions? Aspects of secondary school students' reasoning about rational numbers and their notation. Cognition and Instruction, 28 (2), 181–209.
  • Vosniadou, S. (2003). Exploring the relationships between conceptual change and intentional learning. In G. M. Sinatra & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Intentional conceptual change (pp. 377–406). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Vosniadou, S. & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535–585.
  • Vosniadou, S. & Brewer, W. F. (1994). Mental models of the day/night cycle. Cognitive Science, 18, 123–183.
  • Vosniadou, S., Ioannides, CH., Dimitrakopoulou, A. & Papademitriou, E. (2001). Designing Learning Environments to Promote Conceptual Change in Science, Learning and Instruction, 11 (4–5), 381–419.
  • Vosniadou, S., Skopeliti, I. & Ikospentaki, K. (2004.) Modes of Knowing and Ways of Reasoning in Elementary Astronomy. Cognitive Development, 19, 203–222
  • Vosniadou, S., Skopeliti, I. & Ikospentaki, K. (2005). reconsidering the role of artifacts in reasoning: childrens' understanding of the globe as a model of the earth. Learning and Instruction, 15, 333–351.
  • Wiser, M. & Smith, C. L. (2008). Learning and teaching about matter in grades K-8: When should the atomic-molecular theory be introduced? In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), The international handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 205–239). New York: Routledge.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.