53
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Papers

Communication Functions, Modalities, and Maternal Responses in Children with Severe Speech and Physical Impairment

Pages 27-56 | Published online: 18 Jul 2013

References

  • Aicardi, J., & Box, M. (1992). Cerebral palsy. In J. Aicardi (Ed.), Diseases of the nervous system in childhood (pp. 330–367). London: MacKeith.
  • Akhtar, N., Dunham, F., & Dunham, P. (1991). Directive interactions and early vocabulary development: The role of joint attentional fo-cus. Journal of Child Language, 18, 41–49.
  • American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2005). Roles and responsibilities of speech-language pathologists with respect to augmentative communication: Position statement. ASHA, Supplement 25.
  • Basil, C. (1992). Social interaction and learned helplessness in severely disabled children. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 8, 188–199.
  • Bates, E., Beningni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in Infancy. New York: Academic Press.
  • Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205–226.
  • Beukelman, D., & Yorkston, K. (1980). Nonvocal communication: Performance evaluation. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 61, 272–275.
  • Bharatraj, J. (1983). Manual on Developmental Screening test. Mysore. Padmashree Publishers.
  • Blackstone, S., & Cassatt, E. (1984). Communication competence in communication aid users and their partners. Paper presented at the Third International Conference on Aug-mentative and Alternative Communication, Boston.
  • Blake, J., & Dolgoy, S. J. (1993). Gestural development and its relation to cognition during the transition to language. Journal of Non-verbal Behavior, 17, 87–102.
  • Blockberger, S., & Sutton, A. (2003). Towards linguistic competence: The language experienc- es and knowledge of children with extremely limited speech. In J. Light, D. Beukelman, &J. Reichle (Eds.), Communicative competence for individuals who use AAC. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
  • Bode, T. (1997). Overt co-construction: Speaking adult communication partners in interaction with young children who use AAC systems. Unpublished master's thesis, Univer-sity of Melbourne, Australia.
  • Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., Rock, S. L., Ramey, C. T., Barnard, K. E., Gray, C., et al. (1989). Home environment and cognitive development in the first 3 years: A collabora-tive study involving six sites and three ethnic groups in North America. Developmental Psychology, 25, 217–235.
  • Calculator, S. (1997). Fostering early language acquisition and AAC use: Exploring recipro-cal influences between children and their en-vironments. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 13, 149–157.
  • Calculator, S., & Dollaghan, C. (1982). The use of communication boards in a residential set-ting: An evaluation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 281–287.
  • Calculator, S., & Luchko, C. (1983). Evaluating the effectiveness of a communication board training program. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 185–191.
  • Calendrella, A., & Wilcox, M. (2000). Predicting language outcomes for young prelinguistic children with developmental delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 1061–1071.
  • Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E., & Volterra, V. (1996). Communicative gestures during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language, 23, 645–673.
  • Carpenter, R., Mastergeorge, A., & Coggins, T. (1983). The acquisition of communicative in-tentions in infants eight to fifteen months of age. Language and Speech, 26, 101–116.
  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, join attention and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63 (4, serial no. 255).
  • Caselli, M. (1990). Communicative gestures and first words: In V. Volterra & C. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 56–67). New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Chan, J., & Iacono, T. (2001). Gesture and word production in children with Down syndrome. Augmentative and Alternative Communica-tion, 17, 73–87.
  • Coggins, T., & Carpenter, R. (1981). The communicative intention inventory: A systems for observing and coding children's early in-tentional communication. Applied Psycholin-guistics, 2, 235–251.
  • Conti-Ramsden, G. (1994). Interaction with atypical learners. In C. Gallaway & B. J. Richards (Eds.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (pp. 183–196). New York: Cam-bridge University Press.
  • Conti-Ramsden, G., & FrielPatti, S. (1984). Moth-er-child dialogues: A comparison of normal and language impaired children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 17, 19–35.
  • Crais, E., Day Douglas, D., & Cox Campbell, C. (2004). The intersection of the development of gestures and intentionality. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 678–694.
  • Cress, C., Linke, M., Moskal, L., Benal, A., Andeson, V., & La Montagne, J. (2000). Play and parent interaction in young children with physical impairments. Paper presented at the conference of the American Speech-Lan-guage Hearing Association, Washington, DC.
  • Dore, J. (1974). A pragmatic description of early language development. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 343–350.
  • Falkman, K., Sandberg, A., & Hjelmquist, E. (2002). Preferred communication modes: prelinguistic and linguistic communication in non-speaking preschool children with ce-rebral palsy. International Journal of Lan-guage and Communication Disorders, 37, 59–68.
  • Folven, R., & Bonvillian, J. D. (1991). The transition from nonreferential to referential language in children acquiring American Sign Language. Developmental Psychology, 27, 806–816.
  • Franco, F., & Butterworth, G. (1996) Pointing and social awareness: declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language, 23, 307–336.
  • Franco, F., & Wishart, J., (1995). The use of pointing and other gestures by children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 100, 160–182.
  • Geetha, Y. (2007). Assessment Checklist for speech and language skills. Unpublished ARF project report. AIISH, Mysore.
  • Girolametto, L., Bonifacio, S., Visini, C., Weitz-man, E., Zocconi, E., & Pearce, P. S. (2002). Mother-child interactions in Canada and Italy: Linguistic responsiveness to late talk-ing toddlers. International Journal of Lan-guage and Communication Disorders, 37, 153–171.
  • Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Wiigs, M., & Pearce, P. S. (1999). The relationship be-tween maternal language measures and language development in toddlers with expres-sive vocabulary delays. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 364–374.
  • Halliday, M. (1975) Learning how to mean. London: Edward Arnold.
  • Houghton, J., Bronicki, B., & Guess, D. (1987). Opportunities to express preferences and make choices among students with severe disabilities in classroom settings. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 12, 18–27.
  • Hoff-Ginsburg, E. (1991): Mother-child interaction in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62, 782–796.
  • Iacono, T., Carter, M., & Hook, J. (1998). Identification of intentional communication in students with severe and multiple disabilities. Augmentative and Alternative Communica-tion, 14, 102–114.
  • Iverson, J., Capirci, O., & Casella, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9, 23–43.
  • Iverson, J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16, 367–373.
  • Iverson, J., & Thal, D. (1998). Communicative transitions: There's more to the hand than meets the eye. In A. Wetherby, S. Warren, & J. Reichle (Eds.), Transitions in prelinguistic communication (pp. 59–86) Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
  • Johnston, S., Reichle, J., & Evans, J. (2004). Supporting augmentative and alternative communication use by beginning communicators with severe disabilities. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13, 20–30.
  • Kraat, A. (1985). Communication interaction between aided and natural speakers: A state of the art report. Toronto: Canadian Rehabili-tation Council for the Disabled.
  • Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Light, J., Collier, B., & Parnes, P. (1985a). Communicative interaction between young non-speaking physically disabled children and pri-mary caregivers: Part 1. Discourse patterns. Augmentative and Alternative Communica-tion, 1, 74–83.
  • Light, J., Collier, B., & Parnes, P. (1985b). Communicative interaction between young non-speaking physically disabled children and primary caregivers: Part II. Communicative functions. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 1, 98–107.
  • Light, J., Collier, B., & Parnes, P. (1985c). Communicative interaction between young non-speaking physically disabled children and primary caregivers: Part III. Modes of com-munication. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 1, 125–133.
  • Locke, J. (1989). Babbling and early speech: Continuity and individual differences. First Lan-guage, 9,191–206.
  • Locke, J. (1993). The child's path to spoken language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Luinge, M., Post, W., Wit, H., & Goorhuis-Brouwer, S. (2006) The ordering of milestones in language development for children from 1 to 6 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 923–940.
  • Mace, F., & Roberts, M., (1993). Factors affecting the selection of behavioral treatments. In J. Reichle & D. Wacker (Eds.), Communicative alternatives to challenging behavior. Balti-more: Paul H. Brookes.
  • Marcos, H. (1991). Reformulating a request at 18 months: Gestures, vocalizations and words. First Language, 11, 361–375.
  • Marcos, H., & Kornhaber-le Chanu, M. (1992). Learning how to insist and clarify in the sec-ond year: reformulation of requests in differ-ent contexts. International Journal of Be-haviour Development, 15, 359–376.
  • McCune-Nicolich, L. (1981). Toward symbolic functioning: Structure of early pretend games and potential parallels with language. Child Development, 52, 785–797.
  • McGregor, K., & Capone, N. (2004). Genetic and environmental interactions in determin-ing the early lexicon: Evidence from a set of trizygotic quadruplets. Journal of Child Lan-guage 31, 311–337.
  • McLean, J., & Snyder-McLean, L. (1978). A transactional approach to early language training: Derivation of a model system. Colum-bus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.
  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Nicoladis, May-berry, & Genesee.
  • Mundy, P., & Gnomes, A. (1998). Individual differences in joint attention skill development in the second year. Infant Behavior and De-velopment, 21, 469–482.
  • National Institute of Mental Health ( NIMH). (1994). National Institute of Mental Health Socio-Economic Scale NIMH, Secunderabad.
  • Nicoladis, E., Mayberry, R., & Genesee, F. (1999). Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 514–526.
  • Pennington, L., & McConachie, H. (1999). Mother-child interaction revisited: Communication with nonspeaking physically disabled children. International Journal of Lan-guage and Communication Disorders, 34, 391–416.
  • Pennington, L., & McConachie, H. (2001). Predicting patterns of interaction between children with cerebral palsy and their mothers. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurol-ogy, 43, 83–90.
  • Romski, M., & Sevcik, R. (1996). Breaking the speech barrier. Language development through augmented means. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.
  • Roth, F., & Speakman, N. (1984). Assessing the pragmatic abilities of children. Part 1. Organizational framework and assessment param-eters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor-ders, 49, 2–11.
  • Schweigert, P. (1989). Use of microswitch technology to facilitate social contingency awareness as a basis for early communication skills. Augmentative and Alternative Communica-tion, 5, 192–198.
  • Sigafoos, J., Woodyatt, G., Keen, D., Tait, K., Tucker, M., RobertsPenell, D., & Pittendreigh, N. (2000). Identifying potential com-municative acts in children with developmental and physical disabilities. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 21, 77–86.
  • Smith, M. (1994). Speech by any other name: The role of communication aids in interaction. European Journal of Disorders of Com-munication, 29, 225–240.
  • Smith, M. (1997). The bimodal situation of chil-dren developing alternative modes of language. In E. Bjorck-Akesson & P. Lindsay ( Eds.), Communication naturally: Theoretical and methodological issues in augmentative and alternative communication. Proceed-ings of the Fourth ISAAC Research Sympo-sium (12–18).Vasteras: Malardalen University Press.
  • Tamis-LeMonda, C., Bornstein, M., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.
  • Thal, D., & Tobias, S. (1992). Communicative gestures in children with delayed onset of expressive vocabulary. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1281–1289.
  • Trad, P. (1994). Previewing as a remedy for interpersonal failure in the parent-infant dyad. Infants and Young Children, 7, 1–13.
  • Udwin, O., & Yule, W. (1991). Augmentative communication systems taught to cerebral-palsied children-a longitudinal study. II. Pragmatic features sign and symbol use. Brit-ish Journal of Disorders of Communication, 26, 137–148.
  • Venkatesan, S. (2003). Toy kit for kids with developmental disabilities. Mysore: AIISH.
  • Wallace F., Roberts, J., & Lodder, D. (1998). Interactions of African American infants and their mothers: Relations with development at 1 year of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 900–912.
  • Warren, S. F., & Yoder, P. J. (1998). Facilitating the transition from preintentional to intentional communication. In A. M. Wetherby, S. F. Warren, &J. Reichle (Eds.), Transitions in prelinguistic communication (pp. 365–416). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
  • Weinberg, M., Gianino, A., & Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Facial expressions of emotion and social and object oriented behavior are spe-cifically related in 6-month-old infants. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD) biannual conven-tion, Kansas City, Missouri.
  • Wetherby, A., & Rodriguez, G. (1992). Measurement of communicative intentions in normal developing children during structures and un-structured contexts. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 130–138.
  • Wetherby, A. M., Cain, D. H., Yonclas, D. G., & Walker, V.. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 210–252.
  • Wilcox, M., Kouri, T. A., & Caswell, S. (1990). Partner sensitivity to communication behavior of young children with developmental disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 679–693.
  • Wilder, J., & Granlund, M. (2003). Behavior style and interaction between seven children with multiple disabilities and their caregivers. Child: Care Health and Development, 29, 559–567.
  • Yoder, P., & Warren, S. (1999). Maternal respon-sivity mediates the relationship between prelinguistic intentional communication and later language. Journal of Early Intervention, 22, 126–136.
  • Yoder, P., Warren, F., Kim, K., & Gazdag, G. (1994). Facilitating prelinguistic communication skills in young children with develop-mental delay II: Systematic replication and extension. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37,841–851.
  • Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (2001) Intentional communication elicits language facilitating maternal responses in dyads with children who have developmental disabilities. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 106, 327–335.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.