433
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Maternal recasts and activity variations: A comparison of mother--child dyads involving children with and without SLI

, , , &
Pages 223-240 | Received 16 Sep 2012, Accepted 30 Apr 2013, Published online: 02 Jul 2013

References

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412
  • Bernicot, J., Salazar Orvig, A., & Veneziano, E. (2006). Les reprises: Dialogue, formes, fonctions et ontogenèse. La linguistique, 42, 29–49
  • Biber, D. (1991). Variation across speech and writing. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press
  • Biber, D., & Conrad, S. (2009). Register, genre, and style. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press
  • Bishop, D. V. M., Chan, J., Adams, C., Hartley, J., & Weir, F. (2000). Conversational responsiveness in specific language impairment: Evidence of disproportionate pragmatic difficulties in a subset of children. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 177–199
  • Bohannon, J. N. III, Padgett, R. J., Nelson, K. E., & Mark, M. (1996). Useful evidence on negative evidence. Developmental Psychology, 32, 551–555
  • Brinton, B., Fujiki, M., & Sonnenberg, E. A. (1988). Responses to requests for clarification by linguistically normal and language-impaired children in conversation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53, 383–391
  • Bruner, J. S. (1983). Children’s talk: Learning how to use language. New York, NY: Norton
  • Chevrie-Muller, C., & Plaza, M. (2001). Tests de logopédie: Nouvelles épreuves pour l'examen du langage (N-EEL). Paris, France: Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée
  • Chouinard, M. M., & Clark, E. V. (2003). Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language, 30, 637–669
  • Clark, E. V., & Bernicot, J. (2008). Repetition as ratification: How parents and children place information in common ground. Journal of Child Language, 35, 349–371
  • Conti-Ramsden, G. (1990). Maternal recasts and other contingent replies to language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 262–274
  • Conti-Ramsden, G., Hutcheson, G. D., & Grove, J. (1995). Contingency and breakdown: Children with SLI and their conversations with mothers and fathers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 1290–1302
  • de Weck, G. (2001). Stratégies d'étayage d'adultes en interaction avec des enfants normaux et dysphasiques. In M. Almgren, A. Barrena, M.-J. Ezeizabarrena, I. Idiazabal, & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Research on child language acquisition (pp. 352–386). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press
  • de Weck, G. (2006). Les reprises dans les interactions adulte-enfant: Comparaison d'enfants dysphasiques et tout-venant. La linguistique, 42, 115–134
  • Demetras, M., Post, K. N., & Snow, C. E. (1986). Feedback to first language learners: The role of repetitions and clarification questions. Journal of Child Language, 13, 275–292
  • Farrar, M. J. (1992). Negative evidence and grammatical morpheme acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 28, 90–98
  • Fey, M. E., Krulik, T. E., Loeb, D. F., & Proctor-Williams, K. (1999). Sentence recast use by parents of children with typical language and children with specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 273–286
  • Gallaway, C., & Richards, B. J. (1994). Input and interaction in language acquisition. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press
  • Ingold, J., Gendre, S., Rezzonico, S., Corlateanu, C., & Da Silva, C. (2008). Diversité des étayages des mères d'enfants tout-venant et dysphasiques dans deux situations d'interaction. TRANEL, 49, 69–82
  • Kelman, E., & Schneider, C. (1994). Parent--child interaction: An alternative approach to the management of children’s language difficulties. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 10, 81–96
  • Morgenstern, A., Leroy-Collombel, M., & Caët, S. (2012). Corrective self- and other-repairs in child--adult interaction at the intersection of pragmatic abilities and language acquisition. Journal of Pragmatics, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2012.06.017
  • Nelson, K. E. (2007). Young minds in social worlds: Experience, meaning, and memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
  • Nelson, K. E. (2000). Methods for stimulating and measuring lexical and syntactic advances: Why fiffins and lobsters can tag along with other recast friends. In L. Menn & N. R. Bernstein (Eds.), Methods for studying language production (pp. 115–148). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
  • Nelson, K. E., Camarata, S. M., Welsh, J., Butkovsky, L., & Camarata, M. (1996). Effects of imitative and conversational recasting treatment on the acquisition of grammar in children with specific language impairment and younger language-normal children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39, 850–859
  • Nelson, K., Welsh, J., Camarata, S., Butkovsky, L., & Camarata, M. (1995). Available input for language-impaired children and younger children of matched language levels. First Language, 15, 001–017
  • Proctor-Williams, K., & Fey, M. E. (2007). Recast density and acquisition of novel irregular past tense verbs. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1029–1047
  • Proctor-Williams, K. (2009). Dosage and distribution in morphosyntax intervention: Current evidence and future needs. Topics in Language Disorders, 29, 294–311
  • Proctor-Williams, K., Fey, M. E., & Loeb, D. F. (2001). Parental recasts and production in copulas and articles by children with specific language impairment and typical language. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, 155–168
  • Saxton, M. (2000). Negative evidence and negative feedback: Immediate effects on the grammaticality of child speech. First Language, 20, 221–252
  • Saxton, M. (2005). “Recast” in a new light: Insights for practice from typical language studies. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 21, 23–38
  • Snow, C. (1995). Issues in the study of input: Fine tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences and necessary causes. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 180–193). Oxford, England: Blackwell
  • Snow, C. E., & Ferguson, C. A. (1977). Talking to Children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press
  • Snow, C. E., Perlmann, R., & Nathan, D. (1987). Why routines are different: Towards a multiple-factors model of the relation between input and language acquisition. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language (pp. 65–97). Hillsdale: NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
  • Sorsby, A., & Martlew, M. (1991). Representation demands in mothers’ talk to preschool children in two contexts: Picture book reading and a modelling task. Journal of Child Language, 18, 373–395
  • Strapp, C. M., & Federico, A. (2000). Imitations and repetitions: What do children say following recasts? First Language, 20, 273–290
  • Strapp, C. M., Bleakney, D. M., Helmick, A. L., & Tonkovich, H. M. (2008). Developmental differences in the effects of negative and positive evidence. First Language, 28, 35–53
  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
  • Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260
  • Veneziano, E. (2005). Effects of conversational functioning on early language acquisition: When both caregivers and children matter. In B. Bokus (Ed.), Studies in the psychology of child language (pp. 47–69). Warsaw, Poland: Matrix
  • Vigil, D. C., Hodges, J., & Klee, T. (2005). Quantity and quality of parental language input to late-talking toddlers during play. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 21, 107–122
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
  • Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.