41
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Article

Visual Control of Manual Aiming Movements in 6- to 10-Year-Old Children and Adults

&
Pages 161-172 | Published online: 07 Aug 2010

References

  • Woodworth, R. S. (1899). The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychological Review, 3(2, Whole No. 13), 1-119.
  • Winer, B. J. (1971). Statistical principles in experimental design (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
  • Khan, M. A., Franks, I. M., & Goodman, D. (1998). The effect of practice on the control of rapid aiming movements: Evidence for an interdependency between programming and feedback processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 425-444.
  • Lhuisset, L., & Proteau, L. (2002). Developmental aspects of the control of manual aiming movements in aligned and non-aligned visual displays. Experimental Brain Research, 146, 293-306.
  • Messier, J., & Kalaska, J. F. (1997). Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements. Experimental Brain Research, 115, 469-478.
  • Messier, J., & Kalaska, J. F. (1999). Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements. Experimental Brain Research, 125, 139-52.
  • Meyer, D. E., Abrams, R. A., Kornblum, S., Wright, C. E., & Smith, J. E. K. (1988). Optimality in human performance: Ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychological Reviews, 95, 340-370.
  • Pellizzer, G., & Hauert, C. A. (1996) Visuo-manual aiming movements in 6- to 10-year-old children: Evidence for an asymmetric and asynchronous development of information processes. Brain and Cognition, 30, 175-193.
  • Pratt, J., & Abrams, R. A. (1996). Practice and component submovements: The roles of programming and feedback in rapid aimed limb movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 28, 149-156.
  • Proteau, L., & Isabelle, G. (2002). On the role of visual afferent information for the control of aiming movements toward targets of different sizes. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 367-384.
  • Proteau, L., & Tinjust, D. (2003). On-line modulation of an aiming movement initial impulse based on visual afferent information. Manuscript submitted for publication.
  • Schmidt, R. A., Zelaznik, H. N., Hawkins, B., Frank, J. S., & Quinn, J. T. (1979). Motor-output variability: A theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychological Review, 86, 415-451.
  • Sokal, S. S., & Rohlf, F. J. (1981). Biometry. San Francisco: Freeman.
  • Thomas, J. R., Yan, J. H., & Stelmach, G. E. (2000). Movement substructures change as a function of practice in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 228-244.
  • Vindras, P., & Viviani, P. (1998). Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 569-591.
  • Yan, J. H., & Thomas, J. R., Stelmach, G. E., & Thomas, K. T. (2000). Developmental features of rapid aiming arm movements across the lifespan. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 121-140.
  • Bard, C., Hay, L., & Fleury, M. (1990). Timing and accuracy of visually directed movements in children: Control of direction and amplitude component. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 102-118.
  • Bédard, P., & Proteau, L. (2001). On the role of static and dynamic visual afferent information in goal-directed aiming movements. Experimental Brain Research, 138, 419-431.
  • Charvin, M., & Proteau, L. (1996). Developmental differences in the processing of afferent information for motor control. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12, 387-407.
  • Chicoine, A. M., Lassonde, M., & Proteau, L. (1992). Developmental aspects of sensorimotor integration. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 381-394.
  • Fayt, C., Schepens, N., & Minet, M. (1992). Children's development of reaching: Temporal and spatial aspects of aimed wholearm movements. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 375-384.
  • Fisher, R. A. (1954). Statistical methods for research workers (12th ed.). Edinburgh, Scotland: Oliver & Boyd.
  • Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381-391.
  • Elliott, D., Binsted, G., & Heath, M. (1999). The control of goal-directed movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory. Human Movement Science, 18, 121-136.
  • Elliott, D., Carson, R. G., Goodman, D., & Chua, R. (1991). Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming. Human Movement Science, 10, 393-418.
  • Elliott, D., Chua, R., Pollock, B. J., & Lyons, J. (1995). Optimizing the use of vision in manual aiming: The role of practice. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 72-83.
  • Goodale, M. A., Pelisson, D., & Prablanc, C. (1986). Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature, 320, 748-750.
  • Gordon, J., & Ghez, C. (1987). Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. III. Compensatory adjustments for initial errors. Experimental Brain Research, 67, 253-269.
  • Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M. F., & Ghez, C. (1992). Parallel processing of direction and extent in reaching movements. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology, 11, 92-93.
  • Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M. F., & Ghez, C. (1994). Accuracy of planar reaching movements. I. Independence of direction and extent variability. Experimental Brain Research, 99, 97-111.
  • Greenhouse, S. W., & Geisser, S. (1959). On the methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika, 24, 95-112.
  • Hay, L. (1978). Accuracy of children on an open-loop pointing task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 1079-1082.
  • Hay, L. (1979). Spatial-temporal analysis of movements in children: Motor programs versus feedback in the development of reaching. Journal of Motor Behavior, 11, 189-200.
  • Khan, M., Elliott, D., Coull, J., Chua, R., & Lyons, J. (2002). Optimal control strategies under different feedback schedules: Kinematic evidence. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 45-57.
  • Khan, M. A., & Franks, I. M. (2000). The effect of practice on component submovements is dependent on the availability of visual feedback. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 227-240.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.