2,987
Views
68
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Article

An Evolutionary Theory of Human Motivation

, , &
Pages 129-184 | Published online: 19 Aug 2010

  • American Psychological Society. (2003, March). "Unforgettable" classics: Classic psychology texts stand the test of time. Observer, 16, 1-26.
  • Aston-Jones, G., Ennis, M., Pieribone, V. A., Nickell, W. T., & Shipley, M. T. (1986). The brain nucleus locus coeruleus: Restricted afferent control of a broad efferent network. Science, 234, 734-737.
  • Aunger, R. (2000). Introduction. In R. Aunger (Ed.), Darwinizing culture: The status of memetics as a science (pp. 1-24). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Avital, E., & Jablonka, E. (1994). Social learning and the evolution of behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 48, 1195-1199.
  • Baaré, W. F. C., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Boomsma, D. I., Posthuma, D., de Geus, E. J. C., Schnack, H. G., et al. (2001). Quantitative genetic modeling of variation in human brain morphology. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 816-824.
  • Baars, B. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Baars, B. J. (2001). There are no known differences in brain mechanisms of consciousness between humans and other mammals. Animal Welfare, 10, S31-S40.
  • Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556-559.
  • Ballard, D. H. (1997). An introduction to natural computation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Bandler, R., & Shipley, M. T. (1994). Columnar organization in the midbrain periaqueductal gray: Modules for emotional expression? Trends in Neurosciences, 17, 379-389.
  • Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Barber, T. X. (1996). Restrictive versus open paradigms in comparative psychology [Comment]. American Psychologist, 51, 58-59.
  • Bartley, A. J., Jones, D. W., & Weinberger, D. R. (1997). Genetic variability of human brain size and cortical gyral patterns. Brain, 120, 257-269.
  • Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: An overview. Psychological Inquiry, 7, 1-15.
  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7-15.
  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295-307.
  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293-1295.
  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 215-225.
  • Beggs, J. M., Brown, T. H., Byrne, J. H., Crow, T., LeDoux, J. E., LeBar, K., et al. (1999). Learning and memory: Basic mechanisms. In M. J. Zigmond, F. E. Bloom, S. C. Landis, J. L. Roberts, & L. R. Squire (Eds.), Fundamentals of neuroscience (pp. 1411-1454). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Bekoff, M. (2002). Animal reflections. Nature, 419, 255.
  • Bem, D. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative explanation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183-200.
  • Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Perception and Psychophysics, 8, 279-286.
  • Bermond, B. (2001). A neuropsychological and evolutionary approach to animal consciousness and animal suffering. Animal Welfare, 10, S47-S62.
  • Bernard, J. F., & Bandler, R. (1998). Parallel circuits for emotional coping behaviour: New pieces in the puzzle. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 401, 429-436.
  • Bernard, L. C., Walsh, R. P., & Mills, M. (2005). The Motivation Analysis Test: A contemporary empirical evaluation. Manuscript submitted for publication.
  • Blackmore, S. (1999). The meme machine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Blumberg, M. S., & Sokoloff, G. (2003). Hard heads and open minds: A reply to Panksepp (2003) [Comment]. Psychological Review, 110, 389-394.
  • Blumberg, M. S., & Wasserman, E. A. (1995). Animal mind and the argument from design. American Psychologist, 50, 133-144.
  • Bogen, J. E. (1995). On the neurophysiology of consciousness: I. An overview. Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 52-62.
  • Bookstein, F., Schäefer, K., Prossinger, H., Seidler, H., Fieder, M., Stringer, C., et al. (1999). Comparing frontal cranial profiles in archaic and modern Homo by morphometric analysis. Anatomical Record (New Anatomist), 257, 217-224.
  • Boring, E. G. (1950). A history of experimental psychology (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Bower, G. H., & Clark, M. C. (1969). Narrative stories as mediators of serial learning. Psychonomic Science, 14, 181-182.
  • Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2003). Explaining altruistic behavior in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 153-172.
  • Braver, T. S., Cohen, J. D., & Barch, D. M. (2002). The role of the prefrontal cortex in normal and disordered cognitive control: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 428-448). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Bugental, D. B. (2000). Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: A domain-based approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 187-219.
  • Buss, D. (2004, May). Dyadic antagonistic coevolution: Reciprocal evolutionary changes within a single species between the sexes. In H. L. Roediger III (Chair), The new biological bases of behavior. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the American Psychological Society, Chicago, IL.
  • Buss, D. M., Haselton, M. G., Shackelford, T. K., Bleske, A. L., & Wakefield, J. C. (1995). Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels. American Psychologist, 53, 533-548.
  • Cacioppo, J. T., Klein, D. J., Berntson, G. C., & Hartfield, E. (1993). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 119-142). New York: Guilford Press.
  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Cattell, R. B. (1957). Personality and motivation structure and measurement. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
  • Cattell, R. B., Horn, J. L., Sweney, A. B., & Radcliffe, J. A. (1964). Handbook for the Motivation Analysis Test. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.
  • Cattell, R. B., Radcliffe, J. A., & Sweney, A. B. (1963). The nature and measurement of components of motivation. Genetic Psychological Monographs, 68, 49-211.
  • Chulef, A. S., Read, S. J., & Walsh, D. A. (2001). A hierarchical taxonomy of human goals. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 191-230.
  • Clément, F., & Malerstein, A. J. (2003). What is it like to be conscious? The ontogenesis of consciousness. Philosophical Psychology, 16, 67-85.
  • Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407-428.
  • Corkin, S., Amaral, D. G., Gonzalez, R. G., Johnson, K. A., & Hyman, B. T. (1997). H. M.'s medial temporal lobe lesion: Findings from magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 3664-3979.
  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Beyond intuition and instinct blindness: Toward an evolutionarily rigorous cognitive science. Cognition, 50, 41-77.
  • Craik, F. I. M., Moroz, T. M., Moscovitch, M., Stuss, D. T., Winocur, G., Tulving, E., et al. (1999). In search of the self: A positron emission tomography study. Psychological Science, 10, 26-34.
  • Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (2001). Risk-taking, intrasexual competition, and homicide. In J. A. French, A. C. Kamil, & D. W. Leger (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 47. Evolutionary psychology and motivation (pp. 1-36). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
  • Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' error. New York: Avon Books.
  • Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and mind in the making of consciousness. San Diego, CA: Harcourt.
  • Damasio, A. (2001, October 25). Fundamental feelings. Nature, 781.
  • Damasio, A. (2004, May). Antonio Damasio's theory of thinking faster and faster: Are the brain's emotional circuits hardwired for speed? Discover, 24, 44-49.
  • Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species. London: J. Murray.
  • Davidson, R. J. (2000a). Affective style, psychopathology, and resilience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity. American Psychologist, 55, 1196-1214.
  • Davidson, R. J. (2000b). The functional neuroanatomy of affective style. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 371-388). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Dawkins, R. (1989). The selfish gene. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Deci, E. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.
  • Deci, E., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627-668.
  • Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1987). The support of autonomy and the control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1024-1037.
  • Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston: Little, Brown.
  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Evolutionary psychology: The wheat and the chaff. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 187-191.
  • Donald, M. (1997). Precis of origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 737-791.
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 681-735.
  • Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. Oxford, England: Academic Press.
  • Evans, D. (2002). The search hypothesis of emotion. British Journal of the Philosophy of Science, 53, 497-509.
  • Fairchild, H. H. (1991). Scientific racism: The cloak of objectivity. Journal of Social Issues, 47, 101-115.
  • Fell, J., Klaver, P., Elger, C. E., & Fernandez, G. (2002). The interaction of rhinal cortex and hippocampus in human declarative memory formation. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 13, 299-312.
  • Filskov, S. B., & Boll, T. J. (1981). Handbook of clinical neuropsychology. New York: Wiley-Interscience.
  • Frijda, N. H. (1994). Emotions are functional, most of the time. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. Series in affective science (pp. 112-122). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Gallup, G. G. (1979). Self-awareness in primates. American Scientist, 67, 417-421.
  • Gangestad, S. W. (2001). Adaptive design, selective history, and women's sexual motivations. In J. A. French, A. C. Kamil, & D. W. Leger (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 47. Evolutionary psychology and motivation (pp. 37-74). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
  • Gärdenfors, P. (1996). Cued and detached representations in animal cognition. Behavioural Processes, 35, 263-273.
  • Gil-White, F. J. (2004). Common misunderstandings of memes (and genes): The promise and the limits of the genetic analogy to cultural transmission processes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. Retrieved March 10, 2004, from http://www.bbsonline.org/documents/a/00/00/12/44/bbs00001244-00/Memes2.htm
  • Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., & Fehr, E. (2003). Explaining altruistic behavior in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 153-172.
  • Goldman-Rakic, P. S., Scalaidhe, S., & Chafee, M. (2000). Domain specificity in cognitive systems. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (pp. 733-742). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Gray, J. R. (2001). Emotional modulation of cognitive control: Approach-withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 436-452.
  • Gray, J. R. (2004). Integration of emotion and cognitive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 46-48.
  • Gray, J. R., Braver, T. S., & Raichle, M. E. (2002). Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 99, 4115-4120.
  • Haier, R. J., White, N. S., & Alkire, M. T. (2003). Individual differences in general intelligence correlate with brain function during nonreasoning tasks. Intelligence, 31, 429-441.
  • Hameroff, S. R., Kaszniak, A. W., & Scott, A. C. (Eds.). (1996). Towards a science of consciousness: The first Tucson discussions and debates. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1-52.
  • Hamilton, W. D. (1996). Narrow roads of gene land: Vol. 1. Evolution of social behavior. Oxford, England: W. H. Freeman.
  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.
  • Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 101-134.
  • Hirshfeld, L. W., & Gelman, S. A. (1994). Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Holt, E. B. (1931). Animal drive and the learning process. New York: Holt.
  • Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Plenum Press.
  • Jacobs, L. F., & Schenk, F. (2003). Unpacking the cognitive map: The parallel map theory of hippocampal function. Psychological Review, 110, 285-315.
  • James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt.
  • Jaynes, J. (1976). The origins of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187-245.
  • Jeffries, K. J., Fritz, J. B., & Braun, A. R. (2003, April). Words in melody: An H-sub2-sup 1-sup-5 O PET study of brain activation during singing and speaking. Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 14, 749-754.
  • John, O. P., Angleitner, A., & Ostendorf, F. (1988). The lexical approach to personality: A historical review of trait taxonomic research. European Journal of Personality, 2, 171-203.
  • Kandel, E. R., & Squire, L. R. (2000, November 10), Neuroscience: Breaking down scientific barriers to the study of brain and mind. Science, 290, 1113-1120.
  • Keenan, J. P., Nelson, A., O'Connor, M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Self-recognition and the right hemisphere. Nature, 409, 305.
  • Kelley, W. T., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C., Caglar, S., Inati, S., & Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 785-794.
  • Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265-284.
  • Kenrick, D. T., Li, N. P., & Butner, J. (2003). Dynamical evolutionary psychology: Individual decision rules and emergent social norms. Psychological Review, 110, 3-28.
  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237, 1445-1452.
  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
  • Leger, D. W., Kamil, A. C., & French, J. A. (2001). Introduction: Fear and loathing of evolutionary psychology in the social sciences. In J. A. French, A. C. Kamil, & D. W. Leger (Eds.), The Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 47: Evolutionary psychology and motivation, (pp. ix-xxiii). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
  • Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the "overjustification" hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129-137.
  • Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., Heider, K., & Friesen, W. V. (1992). Emotion and autonomic nervous system activity in the Minangkabau of West Sumatra. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 972-988.
  • Lewicki, P., Hill, T., & Czyzewska, M. (1992). Nonconscious acquisition of information. American Psychologist, 47, 796-801.
  • Lewis, M. (1995). Self-conscious emotions. American Scientist, 83, 68-78.
  • Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self. New York: Plenum.
  • Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 529-566.
  • Lloyd, D. (2002). Functional MRI and the study of human consciousness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 818-831.
  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • MacEachern, D. (2004, April 25). The essence of courage: A special man who didn't want to be anyone special. Arizona Republic, pp. V1-V2.
  • Markus, H. R. (1977). Self-schema and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63-78.
  • Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being. New York: Van Nostrand.
  • Maslow, A. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.
  • Mather, M., Canli, T., English, T., Whitfield, S., Wais, P., Ochsner, K., et al. (2004). Amygdala responses to emotionally valenced stimuli in older and younger adults. Psychological Science, 15, 259-263.
  • McCain, J. (2004). Why courage matters: The way to a braver life. New York: Random House.
  • McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. W., & Lowell, E. L. (1976). The achievement motive (2nd ed.). New York: Irvington.
  • McClelland, J. L. (2000). Connectionist models of memory. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 583-596). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1985). Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 159-188.
  • Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratifications: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3-19.
  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt.
  • Mills, M. (2004, April). Evolution and motivation. In L. C. Bernard (Chair), Integrating perspectives: Toward a new theory of human motivation. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the Western Psychological Association, Phoenix, AZ.
  • Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933-938.
  • Monterosso, J., & Ainslie, G. (1999). Beyond discounting: Possible experimental models of impulse control. Psychopharmacology, 146, 339-347.
  • Mook, D. G. (1996). Motivation: The organization of action. New York: W. W. Norton.
  • Morris, J. S., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393, 467-470.
  • Mott, M. (2004, March). Cat cloning offered to pet owners. National Geographic News. Retrieved June 5, 2004, from http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2004/03/0324_040324_catclones.html
  • Munk, M. H. J., Roelfsema, P. R., König, P., Engel, A. K., & Singer, W. (1996). Role of reticular activation in the modulation of intracortical synchronization. Science, 272, 271-274.
  • Nahm, F. K. D., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1993). Cross-modal associations and the human amygdala. Neuropsychologia, 31, 727-744.
  • Neilsen, M., & Day, R. H. (2000). William James and the evolution of consciousness. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 19, 90-113.
  • Oettingen, G., Bulgarella, C., Henderson, M., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2004). The self-regulation of goal pursuit. In R. A. Wright, J. Greenberg, & S. S. Brehm (Eds.), Motivational analyses of social behavior: Building on Jack Brehm's contributions to psychology (pp. 225-244). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Oyama, S. (1991). Bodies and minds: Dualism in evolutionary theory. Journal of Social Issues, 47, 27-42.
  • Oyama, S. (1993). How shall I name thee? The construction of natural selves. Theory and Psychology, 3, 471-496.
  • Paller, K. A. (2004). Electrical signals of memory and of the awareness of remembering. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 49-55.
  • Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Perlman, B., & McCann, L. I. (1999). The most frequently listed courses in the undergraduate psychology curriculum. Teaching of Psychology, 26, 177-182.
  • Plous, S. (1993). The psychology of judgment and decision making. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  • Plutchik, R. (2003). Emotions and life: Perspectives from psychology, biology, and evolution. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  • Plutchik, R., & Kellerman, H. (1974). Manual of the Emotions Profile Index. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
  • Povinelli, D. J., Gallup, G. G., Jr., Eddy, T. J., Bierschwale, D. T., Engstrom, M. C., Perilloux, H. K., et al. (1997). Chimpanzees recognize themselves in mirrors. Animal Behavior, 53, 1083-1088.
  • Pribram, K. (1977). Human consciousness and the functions of the brain. Somatics, 1, 5-7.
  • Quartz, S. R., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). The neural basis of cognitive development: A constructivist manifesto. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 537-596.
  • Rainville, P., Carrier, B., Hofbauer, R. K., Bushnell, M. C., & Duncan, G. H. (1999). Dissociation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain using hypnotic modulation. Pain, 82, 159-171.
  • Rainville, P., Duncan, G. H., Price, D. D., Carrier, B., & Bushnell, M. C. (1997). Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science, 277, 968-971.
  • Ramachandran, V. S. (1991). 2-D or not 2-D? That is the question. In R. L. Gregory, J. Harris, P. Heard, D. Rose, & C. Cronly-Dillon (Eds.), The artful brain (pp. 249-267). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 261-270.
  • Reiss, S. (2000). Who am I: The 16 basic desires that motivate our actions and define our personality. New York: Tarcher/Putnam.
  • Reiss, S., & Havercamp, S. M. (1996). Sensitivity theory and mental retardation: Why functional analysis is not enough. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 101, 553-566.
  • Reiss, S., & Havercamp, S. M. (1997). The sensitivity theory of motivation: Implications for psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 621-632.
  • Reiss, S., & Havercamp, S. M. (1998). Toward a comprehensive assessment of fundamental motivation: Factor structure of the Reiss Profile. Psychological Assessment, 10, 97-106.
  • Roser, M., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2004). Automatic brains—Interpretive minds. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 56-59.
  • Rozin, P. (1976). The evolution of intelligence and access to the cognitive unconscious. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology, 6, 245-280.
  • Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotion. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 426-450.
  • Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145-172.
  • Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805-819.
  • Salisbury, D. F. (2003, October 3). Neurons that play truth or consequences. Evolutionary Psychology. Retrieved October 4, 2003, from http://www.vanderbilt.edu/exploration/news/news_intentions.htm
  • Salzen, E. A. (1998). Emotion and self-awareness. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 57, 299-313.
  • Schachter, S. (1971). Some extraordinary facts about obese humans and rats. American Psychologist, 26, 129-144.
  • Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinates of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379-399.
  • Schall, J. D. (2001). Neural basis of deciding, choosing and acting. Nature Reviews, 2, 33-42.
  • Schall, J. D., Hanes, D. P., & Taylor, T. L. (1999). Neural control of behavior: Countermanding eye movements. Psychological Research, 63, 299-307.
  • Schall, J. D., & Thompson, K. G. (1999). Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 241-259.
  • Schmitt, D. P., & Pilcher, J. J. (2004). Evaluating evidence of psychological adaptation: How do we know one when we see one? Psychological Science, 15, 643-649.
  • Semendeferi, K., & Damasio, H. (2000). The brain and its main anatomical subdivisions in living hominoids using magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Human Evolution, 38, 317-332.
  • Semendeferi, K., Lu, A., Schenker, N., & Damasio, H. (2002). Humans and great apes share a large frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 272-276.
  • Shermer, M. (2004). The science of good and evil. New York: Times Books.
  • Silvia, P. J., & Duval, T. S. (2004). Self-awareness, self-motives, and self-motivation. In R. A. Wright, J. Greenberg, & S. S. Brehm (Eds.), Motivational analyses of social behavior: Building on Jack Brehm's contributions to psychology (pp. 57-75). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Simon, H. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69, 99-118.
  • Simpson, J. A. (1999). Attachment theory in modern evolutionary perspective. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 115-161). New York: Guilford Press.
  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). Behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf.
  • Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. (2002). The affect heuristic. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Intuitive judgment: Heuristics and biases (pp. 397-420). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., & Koeppe, R. A. (1996). Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 11-20.
  • Smolensky, P. (1995). Connectionism, constituency and the language of thought. In C. Macdonald (Ed.), Connectionism: Debates on psychological explanation, 2, 164-198.
  • Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (2004). Willful determinism: Exploring the possibilities of freedom. In R. A. Wright, J. Greenberg, & S. S. Brehm (Eds.), Motivational analyses of social behavior: Building on Jack Brehm's contributions to psychology (pp. 77-96). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Steriade, M. (1996). Arousal: Revisiting the reticular activating system. Science, 272, 225-226.
  • Strehler, B. L. (1991). Where is the self? A neuroanatomical theory of consciousness. Synapse, 7, 44-91.
  • Sun, R. (1997). Learning, action and consciousness: A hybrid approach toward modelling consciousness. Neural Networks, 10, 1317-1331.
  • Symons, D. (1992). On the use and misuse of Darwinism in the study of human behavior. The psychological foundation of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 137-159). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence. New York: Macmillan.
  • Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Century.
  • Tononi, G., & Edelman, G. M. (1998). Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282, 1846-1851.
  • Tononi, G., Sporns, O., & Edelman, G. M. (1992). Reentry and the problem of integrating multiple cortical areas: Simulation of dynamic integration in the visual system. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 310-335.
  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundation of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 19-136). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1993). The covert learning of affective valence does not require structures in hippocampal system or amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 79-88.
  • Travis, C. B., & Yeager, C. P. (1991). Sexual selection, parental investment, and sexism. Journal of Social Issues, 47, 117-129.
  • Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35-57.
  • Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871-1971 (pp. 136-179). Chicago: Aldine.
  • Tulving, E. T., Kapur, S., Craik, F. I. M., Moscovitch, M., & Houle, S. (1994). Hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry in episodic memory: Positron emission tomography findings. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 91, 2016-2020.
  • Tyler, L. K., Bright, P., Dick, E., Tavares, P., Pilgrim, L., Fletcher, P., et al. (2003). Do semantic categories activate distinct cortical regions? Evidence for a distributed neural semantic system. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 541-559.
  • Van Bockstaele, E. J., & Aston-Jones, G. (1995). Integration in the ventral medulla and coordination of sympathetic, pain and arousal functions. Clinical and Experimental Hypertension, 17, 153-165.
  • Vogeley, K., & Fink, G. R. (2003). Neural correlates of the first-person perspective. TRENDS. Cognitive Sciences, 7, 38-42.
  • Wallbott, H. C., & Scherer, K. R. (1989). Assessing emotion by questionnaire. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience: Vol. 4. The measurement of emotions. New York: Academic Press.
  • Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820-838.
  • Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158-177.
  • Weiner, B. (1989). Human motivation. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  • Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2000). Memory's echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 11125-11129.
  • Wierzbicka, A. (1992). Defining emotion concepts. Cognitive Science, 16, 539-581.
  • Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton, NJ: University Press.
  • Winkielman, P., & Berridge, K. C. (2004). Unconscious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 120-123.
  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151-175.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.