102
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Articles

The Effects of Construal Level on Predictive Heuristics: Disentangling Representativeness From Availability

References

  • Anderson, N. H. (1991). Contributions to information integration theory (Vol. 1). Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315807331
  • Bless, H., & Schwarz, N. (2010). Mental construal and the emergence of assimilation and contrast effects: The inclusion/exclusion model. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 319–373. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(10)42006-7
  • Bodenhausen, G. V., Gabriel, S., & Lineberger, M. (2000). Sadness and susceptibility to judgmental bias: The case of anchoring. Psychological Science, 11(4), 320–323. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00263
  • Braga, J. N., Ferreira, M. B., & Sherman, S. J. (2015). The effects of construal level on heuristic reasoning: The case of representativeness and availability. Decision, 2(3), 216–227. https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000021
  • Braga, J. N., Ferreira, M. B., Sherman, S. J., Mata, A., Jacinto, S., & Ferreira, M. (2018). What’s next? Disentangling availability from representativeness using binary decision tasks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 76, 307–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.03.006
  • Caruso, E. M., Waytz, A., & Epley, N. (2010). The intentional mind and the hot hand: Perceiving intentions makes streaks seem likely to continue. Cognition, 116(1), 149–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.04.006
  • Chandran, S., & Menon, G. (2004). When a day means more than a year: Effects of temporal framing on judgments of health risk. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(2), 375–389. https://doi.org/10.1086/422116
  • Cheek, N. N., & Norem, J. K. (2017). Holistic thinkers anchor less: Exploring the roles of self-construal and thinking styles in anchoring susceptibility. Personality and Individual Differences, 115, 174–176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.01.034
  • Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2001). Putting adjustment back in the anchoring and adjustment heuristic: Differential processing of self-generated and experimenter-provided anchors. Psychological Science, 12(5), 391–396. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.0037
  • Eyal, T., Hoover, G. M., Fujita, K., & Nussbaum, S. (2011). The effect of distance-dependent construals on schema-driven impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(1), 278–281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.007
  • Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Kuschel, S. (2008). The effect of global versus local processing styles on assimilation versus contrast in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(4), 579–599. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.579
  • Freitas, A. L., Gollwitzer, P., & Trope, Y. (2004). The influence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding others’ self-regulatory efforts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(6), 739–752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.04.003
  • Friedman, R., Fishbach, A., Forster, J., & Werth, L. (2003). Attentional priming effects on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 15(2), 277–286. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2003.9651420
  • Fujita, K., Eyal, T., Chaiken, S., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2008). Influencing attitudes toward near and distant objects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 227(21), 9044–9062. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.005
  • Gabrielcik, A., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). Priming and frequency estimation: A strict test of the availability heuristic. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10(1), 85–89. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167284101009
  • Gigerenzer, G. (1991). From tools to theories: A heuristic of discovery in cognitive psychology. Psychological Review, 98(2), 254–267. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.254
  • Gigerenzer, G. (1996). On narrow norms and vague heuristics: A reply to Kahneman and Tversky (1996). Psychological Review, 103(3), 592–596. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.592
  • Gigerenzer, G., Hell, W., & Blank, H. (1988). Presentation and content: The use of base rates as a continuous variable. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), 513–525. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.14.3.513
  • Gigerenzer, G., & Todd, P. M. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford University Press.
  • Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., & Tversky, A. (1985). The Hot Hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17(3), 295–314. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(85)90010-6
  • Hadar, B., Glickman, M., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Usher, M. (2022). Abstract thinking facilitates aggregation of information. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 151(7), 1733–1743. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001126
  • Hansen, J. (2019). Construal level and cross-sensory influences: High-level construal increases the effect of color on drink perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 148(5), 890–904. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000548
  • Herr, P. M., Sherman, S. J., & Fazio, R. H. (1983). On the consequences of priming: Assimilation and contrast effects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(4), 323–340. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(83)90026-4
  • Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases (pp. 49–81)Cambridge University Press.
  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 430–454. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(72)90016-3
  • Kalkstein, D. A., Hubbard, A. D., & Trope, Y. (2018). Beyond direct reference: Comparing the present to the past promotes abstract processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 147(6), 933–938. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000448
  • Kubovy, M. (1977). Response availability and the apparent spontaneity of numerical choices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3(2), 359–364. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.3.2.359
  • Liberman, N., Sagristano, M., & Trope, Y. (2002). The effect of temporal distance on level of construal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(6), 523–534. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00013-6
  • Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2008). The psychology of transcending the here and now. Science, 322(5905), 1201–1205. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1161958
  • McCrea, S. M., Wieber, F., & Myers, A. L. (2012). Construal level mindsets moderate self- and social stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(1), 51–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026108
  • Milkman, K. L., Akinola, M., & Chugh, D. (2012). Temporal distance and discrimination: An audit study in academia. Psychological Science, 23(7), 710–717. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611434539
  • Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (1999). Comparing is believing: A selective accessibility model of judgmental anchoring. European Review of Social Psychology, 10(1), 135–167. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779943000044
  • Nussbaum, S., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Creeping dispositionism: The temporal dynamics of behavior prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(3), 485–497. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.84.3.485
  • Oppenheimer, D. (2004). Spontaneous discounting of availability in frequency judgment tasks. Psychological Science, 15(2), 100–105. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502005.x
  • Rim, S., Uleman, J. S., & Trope, Y. (2009). Spontaneous trait inference and construal level theory: Psychological distance increases nonconscious trait thinking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(5), 1088–1097. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.015
  • Schwarz, N. (1998). Accessible content and accessibility experiences: The interplay of declarative and experiential information in judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Review: An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc, 2(2), 87–99. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0202_2
  • Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 195–202. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.195
  • Sherman, S. J., & Corty, E. (1984). Cognitive heuristics. In R. S. Wyer, & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 189–286). LEA. 15
  • Smith, P. K., & Trope, Y. (2006). You focus on the forest when you’re in charge of the trees: Power priming and abstract information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(4), 578–596. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.578
  • Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440–463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963
  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1971). Belief in the law of small numbers. Psychological Bulletin, 76(2), 105–110. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031322
  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(73)90033-9
  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science (New York, N.Y.), 185(4157), 1124–1131. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.185.4157.1124
  • Wakslak, C., & Trope, Y. (2009). The effect of construal level on subjective probability estimates. Psychological Science, 20(1), 52–58. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02250.x
  • Wakslak, C., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Alony, R. (2006). Seeing the forest when entry is unlikely: Probability and the mental representation of events. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 135(4), 641–653. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.641
  • Wänke, M., & Bless, H. (2000). The effects of subjective ease of retrieval on attitudinal judgments: The moderating role of processing motivation. In H. Bless & J. P. Forgas (Eds.), The message within: The role of subjective experience in social cognition and behavior (pp. 143–161). Psychology Press.
  • Wänke, M., Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1995). The availability heuristic revisited: Experienced ease of retrieval in mundane frequency estimates. Acta Psychologica, 89(1), 83–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(93)E0072-A
  • Wilson, T. D., Houston, C. E., Etling, K. M., & Brekke, N. (1996). A new look at anchoring effects: Basic anchoring and its antecedents. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 125(4), 387–402. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.125.4.387

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.