1,015
Views
20
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Article

Iconicity in the Development of Picture skills: Typical Development and Implications for Individuals with Severe Intellectual Disabilities

Pages 187-201 | Published online: 09 Sep 2009

References

  • Alant E, Bornman J, Lloyd L. L. Issues in AAC research: How much do we really understand. Disability and Rehabilitation 2006; 28: 143–150
  • Beilin H. Understanding the photographic image. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 1999; 20: 1–30
  • Beukelman D. R., Mirenda P. Augmentative and alternative communication: Supporting children and adults with complex communication needs3rd ed. Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, MD 2005
  • Bloomberg K, Karlan G, Lloyd L. L. The comparative translucency of initial lexical items represented in five graphic symbol systems and sets. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 1990; 33: 717–725
  • Bovet D, Vauclair J. Picture recognition in animals and humans. Behavioural Brain Research 2000; 109: 143–165
  • Brady N. C., McLean L. K. Simultaneous and delayed matching to sample in gesture users and speakers with mental retardation. Research in Developmental Disabilities 1998; 19: 409–421
  • Callaghan T. C. Early understanding and production of graphic symbols. Child Development 1999; 70: 1314–1324
  • Callaghan T. C. Factors affecting children's graphic symbol use in the third year: Language, similarity and iconicity. Cognitive Development 2000; 15: 185–214
  • Callaghan T. C. The origins and development of pictorial symbol functioning. Children's understanding and production of pictures, drawings and art, C. Milbraith, H. M. Trauter. Hogrefe & Huber, Cambridge, MA 2008
  • Callaghan T. C., Rankin M. P. Emergence of graphic symbol functioning and the question of domain specificity: A longitudinal study. Child Development 2002; 73: 359–376
  • Callaghan T. C., Rochat P, MacGillivray T, MacLellan C. Modeling referential actions in 6 to 18-month-old infants: A precursor to symbolic understanding. Child Development 2004; 75: 1733–1744
  • Carr D, Wilkinson K. M., Blackman D, McIlvane W. J. Equivalence classes in individuals with minimal verbal repertoires. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 2000; 74: 101–114
  • DeLoache J. Symbolic functioning in very young children: Understanding of pictures and models. Child Development 1991; 62: 736–752
  • DeLoache J. Early understanding and use of symbols: The model model. Current Directions in Psychological Science 1995; 4: 109–113
  • DeLoache J. Dual representation and young children's use of scale models. Child Development 2000; 71: 329–338
  • DeLoache J. Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Science 2004; 8: 66–70
  • DeLoache J. The Pygmalion problem in early symbol use. Symbol use and symbolic representation: Developmental and comparative perspectives, L. L. Namy. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ 2005; 47–67
  • DeLoache J, Burns N. M. Symbolic development in young children: Understanding models and pictures. Systems of representation in children, C. Pratt, A. F. Garton. John Wiley & Sons, London 1993
  • DeLoache J, Burns N. M. Early understanding of the representational function of pictures. Cognition 1994; 52: 83–110
  • DeLoache J, Kolstad V, Anderson K. N. Physical similarity and young children's understanding of scale models. Child Development 1991; 62: 111–126
  • DeLoache J. S., Peralta de Mendoza O. A., Anderson K. N. Multiple factors in early symbol use: Instructions, similarity, and age in understanding a symbol-referent relation. Cognitive Development 1999; 14: 299–312
  • DeLoache J, Pierroutsakos S. L., Uttal D. H. The origins of pictorial competence. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2003; 12: 114–118
  • DeLoache J. S., Pierroutsakos S. L., Uttal D. H., Rosengren K. S., Gottlieb A. Grasping the nature of pictures. Psychological Science 1998; 9: 205–210
  • DeLoache J, Sharon T. Symbols and similarity: You can get too much of a good thing. Journal of Cognition and Development 2005; 6: 33–49
  • DeLoache J, Strauss M. S., Maynard J. Picture perception in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development 1979; 2: 77–89
  • Deregowski J. B. Illusions, patterns and pictures. Academic Press, LondonUK 1980a
  • Deregowski J. B. Perception. Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (vol. 3): Basic processes, H. C. Triandes, W. Lonner. Allyn & Bacon, Boston, MA 1980b; 21–115
  • Dixon L. S. A functional analysis of photo-object matching skills of severely retarded adolescents. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 1981; 14: 465–478
  • Evans D. G., Bowick L, Johnson M, Blenkhorn P. Using iconicity to evaluate symbol use. Lecture Notes in Computer Science 2006; 4061: 874–881
  • Franklin N. K., Mirenda P, Phillips G. Comparisons of five symbol assessment protocols with nondisabled preschoolers and learners with severe intellectual disabilities. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 1996; 12: 73–77
  • Fuller D. R., Lloyd L. L. Toward a common usage of iconicity terminology. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 1991; 7: 215–219
  • Ganea P. A., Pickard M. B., DeLoache J. S. Transfer between picture books and the real world by very young children. Journal of Cognition and Development 2008; 8: 46–66
  • Huer M. B. Examining perceptions of graphic symbols across cultures: Preliminary study of the impact of culture/ethnicity. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 2000; 16: 180–185
  • Huer M. B. Individuals from diverse cultural and ethnic backgrounds may perceive graphic symbols differently: Response to Nigam. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 2003; 19: 137–140
  • Hurlbut B. I., Iwata B. A., Green J. D. Nonvocal language acquisition in adolescents with severe physical disabilities: Blissymbol versus iconic stimulus formats. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 1982; 15: 241–258
  • Ittelson W. H. Visual perception of markings. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 1996; 3: 171–187
  • Koppenhaver D. A., Erickson K. A., Harris B, McLellan J, Skotko B. G., Newton R. A. Storybook-based communication intervention for girls with Rett syndrome and their mothers. Disability and Rehabilitation 2001; 23: 149–159
  • Light J. C., Binger C, Kelford Smith A. Story reading interactions between preschoolers who use AAC and their mothers. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 1994; 10: 255–268
  • Lloyd L. L., Fuller D. R. The role of iconicity in augmentative and alternative communication-symbol learning. Key issues in mental retardation research, W. I. Fraser. Routledge, LondonUK 1990; 295–306
  • Loncke F. T., Campbell J, England A, Haley T. Multimodality: A basis for augmentative and alternative communication – psycholinguistic, cognitive, and clinical/educational aspects. Disability and Rehabilitation 2006; 28: 169–174
  • Mineo Mollica B. Representational competence. Communicative competence for individuals who use AAC: From research to effective practice, J. C. Light, D. R. Beukelman, J. Reichle. Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, MD 2003; 107–146
  • Mirenda P, Locke P. A comparison of symbol transparency in nonspeaking persons with intellectual disability. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 1989; 54: 131–140
  • Mizuko M. Transparency and ease of learning of symbols represented by Blissymbols, PCS, and Picsyms. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 1987; 3: 129–136
  • Mizuko M, Reichle J. Transparency and recall of symbols among intellectual handicapped adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 1989; 54: 627–633
  • Musselwhite C, Ruscello D. Transparency of three symbol communication systems. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 1984; 27: 436–443
  • Namy L. L. What's in a name when it isn't a word? 17-months-old's mapping of nonverbal symbols to object categories. Infancy 2001; 2: 73–86
  • Namy L. L., Campbell A. L., Tomasello M. The changing role of iconicity on non-verbal symbol learning: A U-shaped trajectory in the acquisition of arbitrary gestures. Journal of Cognition and Development 2004; 5: 37–57
  • Namy L. L., Waxman S. R. Words and gestures: Infant's interpretations of different forms of symbolic reference. Child Development 1998; 69: 295–308
  • Namy L. L., Waxman S. R. Symbols redefined. Symbol use and symbolic representation: developmental and comparative perspectives, L. L. Namy. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ 2005; 269–277
  • Nigam R. Do individuals from diverse cultural and ethnic backgrounds perceive graphic symbols differently. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 2003; 19: 135–136
  • Pierroutsakos S. L., DeLoache J. Infant's manual exploration of pictorial objects varying in realism. Infancy 2003; 4: 141–156
  • Pierroutsakos S. L., DeLoache J, Gound M, Bernard E. N. Very young children are insensitive to picture- but not object-orientation. Developmental Science 2005; 8: 326–332
  • Romski M. A., Sevcik R. A. Breaking the speech barrier. Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, MD 1996
  • Romski M. A., Sevcik R. A., Pate J. L. Establishment of symbolic communication in persons with severe retardation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 1988; 53: 94–107
  • Romski M. A., Sevcik R. A. Augmentative communication and early intervention: Myths and realities. Infants and Young Children 2005; 18: 174–185
  • Rowland C, Schweigert P. Tangible symbols: Symbolic communication for individuals with multisensory impairments. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 1989; 5: 226–234
  • Rowland C, Schweigert P. Cognitive skills and AAC. Communicative competence for individuals who use AAC, J. C. Light, D. R. Beukelman, J. Reichle. Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, MD 2003; 241–276
  • Salsa M. A., Peralta de Mendoza O. P. Routes to symbolization: Intentionality and correspondence in early understanding of pictures. Journal of Cognition and Development 2007; 8: 79–92
  • R. W. Schlosser, 2003a. The efficacy of augmentative and alternative communication. San Diego, CA, Academic Press
  • Schlosser R. W. Selecting graphic symbols for an initial requesting lexicon. The efficacy of augmentative and alternative communication: Towards evidence-based practice, R. W. Schlosser. Academic Press, New York 2003b; 347–401
  • Schlosser R. W. Effects of AAC on natural speech development. The efficacy of augmentative and alternative communication: Toward evidence-based practice, R. W. Schlosser. Academic Press, Boston 2003c; 403–425
  • Schlosser R. W., Lee D. Evidence-based strategies for promoting generalization and maintenance. The efficacy of augmentative and alternative communication: Toward evidence-based practice, R. W. Schlosser. Academic Press, New York 2003; 533–552
  • Schlosser R. W., Sigafoos J. Selecting graphic symbols for an initial request lexicon: Integrative review. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 2002; 18: 102–123
  • Sevcik R. A. Comprehension: An overlooked component in augmented language development. Disability and Rehabilitation 2006; 28: 159–167
  • Sevcik R. A., Romski M. A. Representational skills of persons with severe retardation. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 1986; 2: 160–164
  • Sevcik R. A., Romski M. A., Wilkinson K. M. Roles of graphic symbols in the language acquisition process for persons with severe cognitive disabilities. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 1991; 7: 161–170
  • Siegel E, Cress C. J. Overview of the emergence of early AAC behavior: Progression from communicative to symbolic skills. Exemplary practices for beginning communicators: Implications for AAC, J. Reichle, D. R. Beukelman, J. C. Light. Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, MD 2002; 25–58
  • Sigel I. E. The development of pictorial comprehension. Visual learning, thinking and communication, B. S. Rhandhawa, W. E. Coffman. Academic Press, New York 1978; 95–106
  • Simcock G, DeLoache J. Get the picture? The effects of iconicity on toddlers’ reenactment from picture books. Developmental Psychology 2006; 42: 1352–1357
  • Simcock G, Dooley M. Generalization of learning from picture books to novel test conditions by 180 and 24-month-old children. Developmental Psychology 2007; 43: 1568–1578
  • Snell M, Chen L, Hoover K. Teaching augmentative and alternative communication to students with severe disabilities: A review of the intervention research 1997–2003. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities 2006; 31: 203–214
  • Stephenson J, Linfoot K. Pictures as communication symbols for students with severe intellectual disability. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 1996; 12: 244–256
  • Suddendorf T. Early representational insight: Twenty-four-month-olds can use a photo to find an object in the world. Child Development 2003; 74: 896–904
  • Tolar T. D., Lederberg A. R., Gokhale S, Tomasello M. The development of the ability to recognize the meaning of iconic signs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 2008; 13: 225–240
  • Tomasello M, Striano T, Rochat P. Do young children use objects as symbols. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 1999; 17: 563–584
  • Troseth G. L. Getting a clear picture: Young children's understanding of a televised image. Developmental Science 2003; 6: 247–253
  • Troseth G. L., DeLoache J. The medium can obscure the message: Young children's understanding of video. Child Development 1998; 69: 950–965
  • von Tetzchner S, Grove N. The development of alternative language forms. Augmentative and alternative communication: Developmental issues, S. von Tetzchner, N. Grove. Whurr, LondonUK 2003; 1–27
  • von Tetzchner S, Overeeide K. D., Jorgensen K. K., Ormhaug B. M., Oxholm B, Warme R. Acquisition of graphic communication by a young girl without comprehension of spoken language. Disability and Rehabilitation 2004; 26: 1335–1346
  • von Tetzchner S, Martinsen H. Augmentative and alternative communication. Whurr, LondonUK 2000
  • Werner H, Kaplan B. Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to the psychology of language. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ 1984
  • Wetherby A. M., Reichle J, Pierce P. L. The transition to symbolic communication. Transitions in prelinguistic communication, A. M. Wetherby, S. F. Warren, J. Reichle. Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, MD 1998; 197–230
  • Wilkinson K. M., Hennig S. The state of research and practice in augmentative and alternative communication for children with developmental/intellectual disabilities. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 2007; 13: 58–69
  • Wilkinson K. M., McIlvane W. J. Considerations in teaching graphic symbols to beginning communicators. Exemplary practices for beginning communicators: Implications for AAC, J. Reichle, D. R. Beukelman, J. C. Light. Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, MD 2002; 273–322

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.