36
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
BRIEF REPORTS

Seven-Month-Old Infants Selectively Reproduce the Goals of Animate But Not Inanimate Agents

&
Pages 667-679 | Received 07 Sep 2008, Accepted 26 Feb 2009, Published online: 19 Nov 2009

REFERENCES

  • Baldwin , D. A. and Moses , L. J. 1996 . The ontogeny of social information gathering. . Child Development , 67 : 1915 – 1939 .
  • Bates , E. , Benigni , L. , Bretherton , I. , Camaioni , L. and Volterra , V. 1979 . The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy New York : Academic .
  • Behne , T. , Carpenter , M. , Call , J. and Tomasello , M. 2005 . Unwilling versus unable: Infants- understanding of intentional action. . Developmental Psychology , 41 : 328 – 337 .
  • Biro , S. and Leslie , A. 2007 . Infants- perception of goal-directed actions: Development through cue-based bootstrapping. . Developmental Science , 10 : 379 – 398 .
  • Bretherton , I. 1991 . “ Intentional communication and the development of an understanding of mind. ” . In Children-s theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding Edited by: Frye , D. and Moore , C. 49 – 75 . Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. .
  • Brooks , R. and Meltzoff , A. N. 2002 . The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. . Developmental Psychology , 38 : 958 – 966 .
  • Csibra , G. 2008 . Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants. . Cognition , 107 : 705 – 717 .
  • Falck-Yttr , T. , Gredebäck , G. and von Hofsten , C. 2006 . Infants predict other people-s action goals. . Nature Neuroscience , 9 : 878 – 879 .
  • Gergely , G. , Bekkering , H. and Kiraly , I. 2002 . Rational imitation in preverbal infants. . Nature , 415 : 755
  • Gergely , G. and Csibra , G. 2003 . Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naive theory of rational action. . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 7 : 287 – 292 .
  • Gergely , G. , Nadasdy , Z. , Csibra , G. and Biro , S. 1995 . Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. . Cognition , 56 : 165 – 193 .
  • Guajardo , J. J. and Woodward , A. L. 2004 . Is agency skin-deep? Surface attributes influence infants- sensitivity to goal-directed action. . Infancy , 6 : 361 – 384 .
  • Hamlin , J. K. , Hallinan , E. V. and Woodward , A. L. 2008 . Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others- goals. . Developmental Science , 11 : 487 – 494 .
  • Hofer , T. , Hauf , P. and Aschersleben , G. 2005 . Infant-s perception of goal-directed actions performed by a mechanical device. . Infant Behavior and Development , 28 : 466 – 480 .
  • Johnson , S. C. , Booth , A. and O-Hearn , K. 2001 . Inferring the goals of a nonhuman agent. . Cognitive Development , 16 : 637 – 656 .
  • Kuhlmeier , V. , Wynn , K. and Bloom , P. 2003 . Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. . Psychological Science , 14 : 402 – 408 .
  • Legerstee , M. 1991 . The role of person and object in eliciting early imitation. . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 51 : 423 – 433 .
  • Luo , Y. and Baillargeon , R. 2005 . Can a self-propelled box have a goal? . Psychological Science , 16 : 601 – 608 .
  • Mangold , P. 1998 . Interact [computer software] Arnstorf, , Germany : Mangold International .
  • Meltzoff , A. N. 1995 . Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. . Developmental Psychology , 31 : 1 – 16 .
  • Meltzoff , A. N. 2007 . The “like me” framework for recognizing and becoming an intentional agent. . Acta Psychologica , 124 : 26 – 43 .
  • Moll , H. and Tomasello , M. 2007 . How 14- and 18-month-olds know what others have experienced. . Developmental Psychology , 43 : 309 – 317 .
  • Shimizu , Y. A. and Johnson , S. C. 2004 . Infants- attribution of a goal to a morphologically unfamiliar agent. . Developmental Science , 7 : 425 – 430 .
  • Shipley , T. F. and Zacks , J. M. , eds. 2008 . Understanding events: From perception to action. New York : Oxford University Press. .
  • Sodian , B. and Thoermer , C. 2004 . Infants- understanding of looking, pointing, and reaching as cues to goal-directed action. . Journal of Cognition & Development , 5 : 289 – 316 .
  • Sommerville , J. A. , Hildebrand , E. A. and Crane , C. C. 2008 . Experience matters: The impact of doing versus watching on infants- subsequent perception of tool-use events. . Developmental Psychology , 44 : 1249 – 1256 .
  • Sommerville , J. A. and Woodward , A. L. 2005 . Pulling out the intentional structure of action: The relation between action processing and action production in infancy. . Cognition , 95 : 1 – 30 .
  • Tomasello , M. , Carpenter , M. , Call , J. , Behne , T. and Moll , H. 2005 . Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. . Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 28 : 675 – 735 .
  • Warneken , F. and Tomasello , M. 2006 . Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. . Science , 311 : 1301 – 1303 .
  • Woodward , A. L. 1998 . Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor-s reach. . Cognition , 69 : 1 – 34 .
  • Woodward , A. L. 2005 . “ The infant origins of intentional understanding. ” . In Advances in child development and behavior Edited by: Kail , R. V. Vol. 33 , 229 – 262 . Oxford, , UK : Elsevier .

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.