74
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Registered Report

Time course of attentional guidance by visual and verbal working memory representations

&
Pages 464-473 | Received 03 Sep 2021, Accepted 09 Apr 2024, Published online: 22 Apr 2024

References

  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829–839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201
  • Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Current Biology, 20(4), R136–R140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014
  • Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Baier, D., & Ansorge, U. (2019). Investigating the role of verbal templates in contingent capture by color. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(6), 1846–1879. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01701-y
  • Brown, A. M., Lindsey, D. T., & Guckes, K. M. (2011). Color names, color categories, and color-cued visual search: Sometimes, color perception is not categorical. Journal of Vision, 11(2), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.12.2
  • Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention. Psychological Review, 97(4), 523–547. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.4.523
  • Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2011). Automatic and strategic effects in the guidance of attention by working memory representations. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 217–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.06.012
  • Cochrane, B. A., Siddhpuria, S., & Milliken, B. (2019). Cueing color imagery: A critical analysis of imagery-perception congruency effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(8), 1410–1421. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000653
  • Cochrane, B. A., Townsend, B., Lee, R. C., Legere, J. K., Milliken, B., & Shedden, J. M. (2021). Visual imagery influences attentional guidance during visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 83(1), 58–66. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02208-7
  • Collie, A., Maruff, P., Yucel, M., Danckert, J., & Currie, J. (2000). Spatiotemporal distribution of facilitation and inhibition of return arising from the reflexive orienting of covert attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(6), 1733–1745. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.26.6.1733
  • Cong, S. H., & Kerzel, D. (2021). Allocation of resources in working memory: Theoretical and empirical implications for visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(4), 1093–1111. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-021-01881-5
  • Cronin, D. A., Peacock, C. E., & Henderson, J. M. (2020). Visual and verbal working memory loads interfere with scene-viewing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(6), 2814–2820. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02076-1
  • Cui, Y., Jing, J., Dong, C., Qi, M., & Gao, H. (2021). The influence of SOA on working memory guidance of attention. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 33(8), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2021.1950735
  • de Fockert, J. W. (2013). Beyond perceptual load and dilution: A review of the role of working memory in selective attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00287
  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193–222. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
  • Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 148–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00511-9
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
  • Gilbert, A. L., Regier, T., Kay, P., & Ivry, R. B. (2006). Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(2), 489–494. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0509868103
  • Grecucci, A., Soto, D., Rumiati, R. I., Humphreys, G. W., & Rotshtein, P. (2010). The interrelations between verbal working memory and visual selection of emotional faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 1189–1200. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21276
  • Hollingworth, A., Richard, A. M., & Luck, S. J. (2008). Understanding the function of visual short-term memory: Transsaccadic memory, object correspondence, and gaze correction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(1), 163–181. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.1.163
  • Hunt, A. R., & Kingstone, A. (2003). Inhibition of return: Dissociating attentional and oculomotor components. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 1068–1074. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1068
  • Jia, L., Wang, J., Zhang, K., Ma, H., & Sun, H. J. (2019). Do emotional faces affect inhibition of return? An ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 721. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00721
  • Kawashima, T., & Matsumoto, E. (2017). Cognitive control of attentional guidance by visual and verbal working memory representations. Japanese Psychological Research, 59(1), 49–57. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12141
  • Kiyonaga, A., Egner, T., & Soto, D. (2012). Cognitive control over working memory biases of selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(4), 639–646. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0253-7
  • Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 138–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01452-2
  • Knapp, W. H., & Abrams, R. A. (2012). Fundamental differences in visual search with verbal and pictorial cues. Vision Research, 71, 28–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2012.08.015
  • Lange, W.-G., Heuer, K., Reinecke, A., Becker, E. S., & Rinck, M. (2008). Inhibition of return is unimpressed by emotional cues. Cognition & Emotion, 22(8), 1433–1456. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701809451
  • Maruff, P., Yucel, M., Danckert, J., Stuart, G., & Currie, J. (1999). Facilitation and inhibition arising from the exogenous orienting of covert attention depends on the temporal properties of spatial cues and targets. Neuropsychologia, 37(6), 731–744. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00067-0
  • Moriya, J. (2018). Visual mental imagery influences attentional guidance in a visual-search task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80(5), 1127–1142. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-018-1520-0
  • Peirce, J. W., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M. R., Höchenberger, R., Sogo, H., Kastman, E., & Lindeløv, J. (2019). Psychopy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 195–203. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y
  • Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (pp. 531–556). Erlbaum.
  • Redden, R. S., MacInnes, W. J., & Klein, R. M. (2021). Inhibition of return: An information processing theory of its natures and significance. Cortex, 135, 30–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.11.009
  • Schmidt, J., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2009). Search guidance is proportional to the categorical specificity of a target cue. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10), 1904–1914. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210902853530
  • Shen, Z., Ding, Y., Satel, J., & Wang, Z. (2021). A concurrent working memory load does not necessarily impair spatial attention: Evidence from inhibition of return. Visual Cognition, 29(1), 38–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2020.1858215
  • Soto, D., Heinke, D., Humphreys, G. W., & Blanco, M. J. (2005). Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(2), 248–261. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.248
  • Soto, D., Hodsoll, J., Rotshtein, P., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Automatic guidance of attention from working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(9), 342–348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.007
  • Soto, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2007). Automatic guidance of visual attention from verbal working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 730–737. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.3.730
  • Soto, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Stressing the mind: The effect of cognitive load and articulatory suppression on attentional guidance from working memory. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(5), 924–934. https://doi.org/10.3758/PP.70.5.924
  • Soto, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2014). Working memory biases in human vision. In A. C. Nobre & S. Kastner (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of attention (pp. 753–776). Oxford Library of Psychology.
  • Stoyanova, R. S., Pratt, J., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Inhibition of return to social signals of fear. Emotion, 7(1), 49–56. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.49
  • van Doorn, J., van den Bergh, D., Böhm, U., Dablander, F., Derks, K., Draws, T., Etz, A., Evans, N. J., Gronau, Q. F., Haaf, J. M., Hinne, M., Kucharský, Š, Ly, A., Marsman, M., Matzke, D., Gupta, A. R. K. N., Sarafoglou, A., Stefan, A., Voelkel, J. G., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2021). The JASP guidelines for conducting and reporting a Bayesian analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(3), 813–826. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01798-5
  • van ‘t Wout, F., & Jarrold, C. (2022). Articulatory suppression during instruction encoding impedes performance in choice reaction time tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29(5), 1960–1968. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02100-5
  • Wolfe, J. M., Horowitz, T. S., Kenner, N., Hyle, M., & Vasan, N. (2004). How fast can you change your mind? The speed of top-down guidance in visual search. Vision Research, 44(12), 1411–1426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2003.11.024
  • Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2007). Do the contents of visual working memory automatically influence attentional selection during visual search? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(2), 363–377. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.363
  • Yu, X., Zhou, Z., Becker, S. I., Boettcher, S. E. P., & Geng, J. J. (2023). Good-enough attentional guidance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 27(4), 391–403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2023.01.007
  • Zhang, Y., & Zhang, M. (2011). Spatial working memory load impairs manual but not saccadic inhibition of return. Vision Research, 51(1), 147–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.022

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.