623
Views
39
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Organizational Justice and Managerial Trust as Predictors of Antisocial Employee Responses

&
Pages 357-375 | Published online: 15 Nov 2008
 

Abstract

This study examined the relationships between employee perceptions of organizational justice and trust and employee antisocial organizational behavior and communication. Participants from organizations representing two geographic regions were surveyed regarding their most recent performance appraisal. Results indicated that perceptions of justice and trust negatively predicted indirect interpersonal aggression, hostility, obstructionism, and deception. When justice and trust were entered simultaneously into the regression model, perceptions of trust predicted hostility, while perceptions of distributive justice predicted deception. Post hoc analyses demonstrated that justice and trust interacted to predict antisocial responses, and trust mediated the relationships between justice and antisocial responses.

Notes

Note: All correlations are statistically significant at p < .001.

Note: Sex: male = 1, female = 2. Region: southwest = 1, mid-Atlantic = 2. Give evaluation: no = 1, yes = 2.

p < .10.

Note: Sex: male = 1, female = 2. Region: southwest = 1, mid-Atlantic = 2. Give evaluation: no = 1, yes = 2

p < .05; p = .079.

Participants recruited from the mid-Atlantic were older [(M = 42.31, SD = 12.66 vs. M = 34.68, SD = 11.37), t (142) = − 3.18, p < .05] and earned more [(M = $37,900, SD = $25,300 vs. M = $29,000, SD = $19,400), t (142) = − 2.40, p < .05] than those from the Southwest; however, the mid-Atlantic participants perceived less distributive justice after their performance evaluations than did the Southwestern participants [(M = 6.18, SD = 1.11 vs. M = 5.71, SD = 1.40), t (142) = 2.26, p < .05]. The form of the evaluation also differed according to the participants' geographic region, F (1, 128) = 7.19, p < .05.

For the justice variables, tests of homogeneity (internal consistency) indicated that for distributive and procedural justice, no error calculated between observed and expected correlations was greater than sampling error. For interactional justice, only one error calculated between observed and expected correlations was greater than sampling error (error was .01 beyond sampling error). Tests of heterogeneity (parallelism) indicated that fewer than 3% of errors calculated between observed and expected correlations was greater than sampling error (on average, errors were .13 beyond sampling error).

Tests of homogeneity indicated that for deception and obstructionism no error calculated between observed and expected correlations was greater than sampling error. For indirect interpersonal aggressiveness, only one error calculated between observed and expected correlations was greater than sampling error (error was .01 beyond sampling error). For hostility, less than 10% of the errors calculated between observed and expected correlations was greater than sampling error (on average, errors were .06 beyond sampling error). Tests of heterogeneity indicated that less than 3% of errors calculated between observed and expected correlations was greater than sampling error (on average, errors were .13 beyond sampling error).

Perceptions of all three justice types differed by participant sex, with women perceiving higher levels of justice. Participants responsible for giving feedback perceived higher levels of distributive justice and managerial trust than did participants not responsible for giving feedback. Participant age was negatively correlated with hostility, obstruction, and deception. Participant salary was negatively correlated with perceptions of procedural justice. As mentioned in Note 1, participant perceptions of distributive justice also differed according to the region from which the participants were recruited.

Because the distributions for the predictor and criterion variables were quite skewed and exhibited non-normal kurtosis, the data were normalized using the logarithm transformation. The results for the hypotheses only became stronger after transforming the variables and the results for the post hoc analyses only changed slightly.

Results after transforming the variables are available from the first author.

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Rebecca M. Chory

Rebecca M. Chory (PhD, Michigan State University, 2000) is an Associate Professor in the Department of Communication Studies, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV, USA.

Anne P. Hubbell

Anne P. Hubbell (PhD, Michigan State University, 2000) is an Associate Professor in the Department of Communication Studies, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM, USA.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.