11,230
Views
91
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Modeling of food intake: a meta-analytic review

, , &
Pages 119-136 | Received 14 Sep 2014, Accepted 09 Jan 2015, Published online: 24 Feb 2015

Abstract

This meta-analysis provides a comprehensive quantitative assessment of research on modeling of food intake. Thirty-eight articles met inclusion criteria. Overall, there was a large modeling effect (r = .39) such that participants ate more when their companion ate more, and ate less when their companion ate less. Furthermore, social models appear to have stronger inhibitory effects than augmenting effects. Moderator analyses indicated that there were larger effects for correlational versus experimental studies, and for women versus men. There was no difference in effect sizes for studies using a live versus remote confederate, or for participants who were high or low in concern with eating appropriately. Together, these findings point to modeling as a robust and powerful influence on food intake.

Imagine that you are meeting a friend for lunch at a restaurant. You both order the daily special and, when the food arrives, your friend eats almost everything on her plate. The next day, you go for lunch at the same restaurant with another friend who eats almost nothing. How much would you eat in each of those situations? Although factors such as how hungry you are and how much you like the taste of the food will almost certainly play a role, considerable evidence suggests that how much your companion eats will also play an important role in determining how much you eat. In social situations, one's eating behavior can be influenced by the behavior of others in a variety of ways (see Herman, Roth, & Polivy, Citation2003, for a review). For example, social-facilitation research finds that people tend to eat more in larger groups than when eating alone (de Castro & Brewer, Citation1992; Herman, Citation2015), whereas the impression-management literature indicates that people can use their eating behavior to convey a particular impression of themselves to others (Vartanian, Citation2015; Vartanian, Herman, & Polivy, Citation2007). One of the most powerful social influences on food intake is modeling: people adjust their food intake to that of their eating companion, eating a little when their companion eats a little, and eating more when their companion eats more.

According to the normative account of food intake, modeling occurs because other people provide information about the appropriate amount of food to consume in a given situation (Herman et al., Citation2003). This account follows from the fact that, in social situations, the appropriate amount to eat is often ambiguous, and internal signals (i.e., hunger and satiety) that one would expect to help guide food intake are often unreliable (Herman & Polivy, Citation2005). Thus, in these situations, people may look to the example of others to help them decide how much food is appropriate to consume. More specifically, Herman et al. (Citation2003) argued that people are often motivated to maximize their intake of palatable foods without appearing to eat excessively, with “excess” defined as eating more than what others are eating. Thus, a companion who eats very little can inhibit one's own food intake whereas a companion who eats a large amount can augment one's own food intake (or at least give one permission to eat an equally large amount). Support for the normative account of modeling comes from recent research showing that perceived norms of appropriate food intake mediate the influence of social models on food intake (Vartanian, Sokol, Herman, & Polivy, Citation2013). Such a normative account has also been used by some researchers to explain the spread of obesity in social networks (e.g., Christakis & Fowler, Citation2007).

One of the most notable features of the modeling of food intake is how robust the effect appears to be. Modeling is observed with unhealthy snack foods (Vartanian et al., Citation2013) and healthy snack foods (Hermans, Larsen, Herman & Engels, Citation2009), and also during meals (Hermans, Larsen, Herman, & Engels, Citation2012). Modeling occurs among people who have been food deprived for up to 24 hours (Goldman, Herman, & Polivy, Citation1991), among children (Bevelander, Anschütz, & Engels, Citation2012), and independent of individual differences in body weight (Rosenthal & McSweeney, Citation1979) and dietary restraint (Roth, Herman, Polivy, & Pliner, Citation2001). Modeling persists even when the other person is not physically present and participants are exposed only to a written indication of the amount of food eaten by supposed prior participants (a “remote” confederate; Roth et al., Citation2001; Vartanian et al., Citation2013); indeed, Feeney, Polivy, Pliner, and Sullivan (Citation2011) found no difference in the strength of modeling whether the model was a live confederate or a remote confederate.

Researchers have also examined a variety of individual difference and contextual factors that should enhance or limit the extent to which people model the food intake of others. For example, individuals who are high in trait empathy (Robinson, Tobias, Shaw, Freeman, & Higgs, Citation2011) appear to model the behavior of their eating companion to a greater extent, as do individuals high in expressiveness (Brunner, Citation2012), but individuals high in extraversion or high in self-monitoring do not differ in the extent to which they model another person's food intake (Herman, Koenig-Nobert, Peterson, & Polivy, Citation2005). Furthermore, modeling tends to be stronger when the person's eating companion is an in-group member (Cruwys et al., Citation2012) and when the experimental confederate is lean (as opposed to obese; e.g., McFerran, Dahl, Fitzsimons, & Morales, Citation2010). Examining moderators of modeling is important in order to elucidate any possible boundary conditions of the effect.

The purpose of the present meta-analysis is to quantify the effects of social models on people's food intake. Herman et al. (Citation2003) provided a qualitative review of social influences on food intake, but the literature was in its relative infancy at the time. Since then, there has been a proliferation of research on the modeling of food intake, and there is thus a critical mass of research allowing for a quantitative analysis of modeling effects. One recent meta-analysis (Robinson, Thomas, Aveyard, & Higgs, Citation2014) examined the impact of social norms on food choice and eating behavior and found that social norms exerted a moderate effect on the eating outcomes. However, that review was limited by the inclusion of only a small number of experimental studies (only eight studies that examined actual food intake), and the authors were unable to quantitatively assess any moderators of the modeling effect. Thus, we sought to provide a more comprehensive assessment of the effects of social models on the amount of food that people eat. Furthermore, we sought to develop a better understanding of the moderators of modeling effects by examining variation as a function of experimental design (correlational vs. experimental studies), as well as characteristics of the participant, characteristics of the model, characteristics of the eating context, and factors that might influence the extent of people's concern with eating appropriately. Doing so will not only provide a clearer picture of how, when, and why social models influence people's food intake, but should also provide direction for future research aimed at filling the gaps in the existing knowledge base.

Method

Literature search strategy

The literature search involved a multi-step process and was completed in August 2014. First, we searched three electronic databases (Scopus, PsycInfo, and Web of Science) for articles published through the end of 2013 to identify studies containing the terms eating, food intake, consumption, ingestion, and food choice in combination with modeling, social influence, peer influence, matching, and confederate. Second, the reference section of each retrieved article was searched to identify potentially relevant articles that were missed in the initial searches. Third, citations to the retrieved articles were searched to identify any recent relevant articles. Fourth, the electronic databases indicated above were further searched using the names of the first author, last author, and corresponding author of each eligible study. Fifth, the table of contents of key journals in the area (Appetite, Eating Behaviors, Physiology & Behavior, Health Psychology) were searched for any relevant articles that might have been missed through the other search steps.

The search process identified 110 articles which were then retrieved and reviewed in detail by two independent judges (the first and second author) to determine their eligibility for inclusion in this study. Note that, although the terms “modeling” and “matching” are often used interchangeably in the literature, we suggest that the term “matching” should refer specifically to the extent to which Person A eats an identical amount to Person B, whereas the term “modeling” should refer more generally to the process of adjusting one's intake upward or downward in line with the intake of one's eating companion (see also Spanos, Vartanian, Herman, & Polivy, Citation2014). In this article, we focus on modeling (i.e., studies that assess the correlation between the amounts eaten by individuals, or studies that examine how a confederate's intake affects participants' intake). Inclusion criteria for the current meta-analysis were as follows: (1) the article must have been published in English (1 article excluded); (2) the study must have measured the amount of food consumed by participants (as opposed to food choice, self-reported food intake, behavioral intentions, or other such measures; 28 articles excluded); and (3) for experimental studies, the study must involve at least two norm conditions (i.e., at least two of the following: low-intake model, high-intake model, no-intake model, or eat-alone/no-norm condition) and the norm conditions must specify the amount eaten by the confederates (10 articles excluded). Other reasons for excluding articles were that they were not modeling studies (e.g., social facilitation studies; 15 articles excluded), were only abstracts without sufficient information to code (9 articles excluded), were theoretical review papers (8 articles excluded), or duplicated a sample from another study (1 article excluded). A total of 38 articles (representing 44 separate studies and 67 independent effects) met the inclusion criteria and were included in the meta-analysis.

Effect size coding

We calculated effect sizes as r-values from the information available in the published reports, including means and standard deviations, correlation coefficients or intraclass correlations, F-values, and p-values. For experimental studies in which means for more than two groups were reported (e.g., low-intake norm, high-intake norm, and a control condition), we used the formulas for one-way contrasts described by Rosenthal, Rosnow, and Rubin (Citation2000) to calculate an overall effect size. In each case, we ordered the norm conditions as follows (from lowest to highest): no-intake confederate, low-intake confederate, eat-alone control, and high-intake confederate. For studies that did not provide sufficient information to calculate precise effect sizes, we contacted the authors to obtain additional information (when possible), or used a conservative estimate of effect sizes. For example, effects described as “significant” were assigned a p-value of .05, and effects described as “non-significant” were assigned a p-value of .99. This conservative effect-size estimate was used in only two cases and allowed us to include as many studies as possible in the meta-analysis without potentially exaggerating any observed effects. Effect sizes were weighted by sample size. We interpreted an effect size of .10 as small, .25 as medium, and .40 or above as large (Lipsey & Wilson, Citation2001).

Meta-analytic procedures

To determine the magnitude and significance of the overall effect size, we fit a random-effects model. In contrast to a fixed-effects approach, which assumes that individual effect sizes differ from the population mean through sampling error alone, a random-effects approach assumes that variability among effect sizes is due to sampling error as well as unsystematic, random sources of error that vary across studies. Random-effects models are also more conservative and have substantially reduced Type 1 error rates compared to fixed-effects models (Field, Citation2003; Lipsey & Wilson, Citation2001), and allow for inferences to be drawn beyond the studies included in the meta-analysis (Field & Gillett, Citation2010).

The primary analysis was an examination of the overall modeling effect across all studies. Because there is considerable variability in the research design used in modeling studies, we also tested whether mean effect sizes differed for experimental studies (those in which the amount eaten by the model is determined by the experimenter) and correlational studies (in which the researchers simply calculated the degree of correspondence between the amounts eaten by two members of a dyad). For this analysis, we excluded no-norm (eat alone) control conditions from the experimental studies because there is no equivalent in correlational studies (the results are identical if the eat-alone condition is included). Furthermore, past theory and research suggests that social models might have an inhibiting effect more than they have an augmenting effect (Herman et al., Citation2003; Vartanian et al., Citation2013); we therefore computed the mean effect size separately for low-norm conditions versus no-norm/eat-alone control conditions, and for no-norm/eat-alone control conditions versus high-intake conditions.

The potential for publication bias was assessed by calculating Orwin's fail-safe N (the number of studies with a correlation of r = .00 that would need to be added to the meta-analysis to bring the overall effect size to a negligible level, defined as r = .05 in this case; Orwin, Citation1983). The fail-safe N was 451 studies, suggesting that these effects are robust to the so-called file-drawer problem.

Moderators

In addition to determining the overall effect size, we also evaluated the degree of heterogeneity in effect-size distribution by calculating the Q statistic, and followed up a significant Q statistic with a series of moderator analyses. Moderators were coded by two independent coders with any discrepancies resolved through discussion with the first author. The moderator coding for each study is displayed in Table .

Table 1 Study characteristics for each moderator included in the meta-analysis.

Characteristics of the participant

Participant gender (only male, only female, or male and female combined) and participant age group (children vs. adults) were examined as potential moderators. Although the weight classification of participants would be of interest, only three studies (Bevelander et al., Citation2012; Nisbett & Storms, Citation1974; Salvy, Howard, Read, & Mele, Citation2009) provided data separately for overweight and normal-weight participants, and thus participant weight status was not included as a moderator.

Characteristics of the model

We compared effect sizes for studies involving live confederates versus remote confederates. Remote-confederate studies included those that provided participants with a list of the amount eaten by supposed previous participants (usually the 10 previous participants; e.g., Roth et al., Citation2001), as well as studies using a video or social-media presentation of the confederate (e.g., Bevelander, Anschütz, Creemers, Kleinjan, & Engels, Citation2013; Hermans, Salvy, Larsen, & Engels, Citation2012). We also examined the confederate's weight status as a moderator of the overall effect size in those studies that directly manipulated the confederate's weight. Hermans, Larsen, Herman, and Engels (Citation2008) included a “slim” confederate (body mass index [BMI; kg/m2] = 20.9) and a “normal-weight” confederate (BMI unspecified); McFerran et al. (Citation2010) included a “thin” confederate (BMI = 19.2) and an obese confederate (apparent BMI = 33); and Johnston (Citation2002) included a normal-weight confederate (BMI = 24) and an obese confederate (BMI = 35). Because of the differences in how these weight groups were defined, we compared effect sizes for thin (BMI < 21), normal-weight (BMI = 21–24), and obese confederates (BMI>30). Finally, we examined whether familiarity with one's eating companion moderated the modeling effect.

Characteristics of the eating context

Studies were coded in terms of whether participants were served a meal or a snack. This designation was established by considering the food provided to participants and the description of the study context provided by the authors. Studies were also coded in terms of whether the study was framed for participants as an eating task (e.g., a taste test, a meal) or as a non-eating task in which they were given incidental access to food.

Concern with eating appropriately

Social models are thought to influence people's eating behavior by providing a norm of appropriate intake (Herman et al., Citation2003; Vartanian et al., Citation2013). A number of studies have examined factors that might make participants more or less concerned about eating in an appropriate manner and therefore more or less likely to model the intake of an eating companion. These studies were coded, based on the description provided by the authors, in terms of whether participants should experience high or low concern with eating appropriately (see Table ). Two other relevant characteristics (sociotropy and self-esteem) were not included in the analysis because the specific study did not meet our inclusion criteria (Exline, Zell, Bratslavsky, Hamilton, & Swenson, Citation2012), the data duplicated another variable already included in the analysis (Robinson et al., Citation2011), or the study did not provide sufficient data to calculate relevant effect sizes (Bevelander, Anschütz, et al., Citation2013).

Results

Overall effect of modeling on food intake

As expected, social models had a significant effect on participants' food intake: Participants ate more food when the model ate a lot than when the model ate very little. The overall effect size across all studies was r = .39 (p < .001, 95% CI = .33 to .44), indicating that social models had what could be considered a “large” effect on the amount of food that participants ate (Figure ).

Figure 1 Forest plot of effect sizes for all studies included in the meta-analysis. For ease of presentation, an average effect size is provided for each individual study.
Figure 1 Forest plot of effect sizes for all studies included in the meta-analysis. For ease of presentation, an average effect size is provided for each individual study.

A comparison of correlational and experimental studies showed that correlational studies (r = .56) produced larger effect sizes than did experimental studies (r = .31), Q(1) = 12.94, p < .001 (see Table ). Note, however, that the effect size for experimental studies would still be considered moderate-to-large in magnitude.

Table 2 Moderators of modeling effects.

Inhibiting versus augmenting models

We next examined the inhibiting and augmenting effects of a social model by comparing, separately, the effects of a low-intake confederate versus an eat-alone (no norm) control condition, and the effects of a high-intake confederate versus an eat-alone (no norm) control condition. These analyses were conducted separately because some studies included both low-intake and high-intake conditions along with the control condition, whereas others included only a low-intake or high-intake condition with the control condition. Thus, statistical comparison between the two mean effect sizes was not possible. The mean effect size for low-intake versus control was r = − .23 (p < .001, 95% CI = − .11 to − .35), indicating that low-intake models produce a moderate inhibiting effect on participants' food intake (see Figure ). Consistent with previous theory and research, high-intake models did augment participants' food intake, but the mean effect size for high intake versus control was small (r = .14, p = .001, 95% CI = .05 to .22; see Figure ).

Figure 2 Forest plot of effect sizes for low-intake confederate condition versus control condition. For ease of presentation, an average effect size is provided for each individual study.
Figure 2 Forest plot of effect sizes for low-intake confederate condition versus control condition. For ease of presentation, an average effect size is provided for each individual study.

Figure 3 Forest plot of effect sizes for control condition versus high-intake confederate condition. For ease of presentation, an average effect size is provided for each individual study.
Figure 3 Forest plot of effect sizes for control condition versus high-intake confederate condition. For ease of presentation, an average effect size is provided for each individual study.

Moderators of the modeling effect

There was significant heterogeneity of effect size distribution for the overall modeling effect, Q(66) = 253.47, p < .001, with effect sizes ranging from − .18 to .93. Therefore, we proceeded to examine the moderators of the overall modeling effect (see Table ).

Characteristics of the participants

Among studies that provided data separately for female participants and for male participants, the mean effect size was larger among female participants (r = .39) than among male participants (r = .17), Q(1) = 4.70, p = .03. The mean effect size for studies that included both male and female participants (r = .45) was significantly larger than the mean effect size for studies with only male participants (p = .02) but did not differ from studies with only female participants (p = .42). Studies examining modeling effects among children (r = .47) and among adults (r = .37) did not differ in their mean effect size, Q(1) = 1.56, p = .21. Interestingly, a follow-up meta-regression for studies involving children showed that children's mean age was positively associated with the degree of modeling (slope = .06, SE = .02, p < .001).

Characteristics of the model

Studies involving a remote confederate produced a mean effect size (r = .30) that was virtually identical to that of studies using a live confederate (r = .31), Q(1) = 0.03, p = .85. As for the confederate's body size, thin confederates (r = .41) and normal-weight confederates (r = .43) produced equivalent effect sizes (p = .88). Furthermore, although thin confederates and normal-weight confederates produced larger mean effect sizes than did obese confederates (r = .14), these differences were not statistically significant (ps>.05), which is probably due to the small number of studies involved (k = 2 per group). Finally, the mean effect sizes for eating with a familiar companion (r = .53) and an unfamiliar companion (r = .37) did not differ significantly, Q(1) = 2.49, p = .12.

Characteristics of the eating context

There was no significant difference in mean effect size for meals (r = .46) compared to snacks (r = .38), Q(1) = 2.17, p = .14. There was also no significant difference in mean effect size when the task was presented as an eating task (e.g., a taste test; r = .35) or a non-eating task (e.g., incidental access to snack foods as part of a separate task; r = .42), Q(1) = 2.04, p = .15.

Concern with eating appropriately

Conditions that would be expected to elicit higher concern with eating appropriately (r = .51) did not differ from conditions that should elicit lower concern with eating appropriately (r = .36), Q(1) = 0.92, p = .34.

Discussion

Social factors play an important role in dictating how much food people will eat in a given situation. Our meta-analysis summarized the effect sizes from studies on the modeling of food intake, and found an overall mean effect size of r = .39, which corresponds to a large effect (Lipsey & Wilson, Citation2001). We also found that inhibiting models tend to have a greater effect on people's food intake than do augmenting models. Herman et al. (Citation2003) argued that people are motivated to maximize their intake of palatable foods without appearing to eat excessively, and “excess” in social settings is defined as eating more than other people are eating. When the model eats very little, this sets a relatively low ceiling for acceptable food intake, leading people to suppress their food intake relative to how much they would eat if they were alone. In contrast, when the model eats a great deal, people essentially have the freedom to eat as much as they typically would and may even have permission to eat somewhat more than they typically would (as indicated by the relatively small difference between the high-intake conditions and the eat-alone control condition).

In addition to examining the overall effect size, we also examined a range of potential moderators of modeling effects. With respect to research design, we found that effect sizes were larger for correlational studies than for experimental studies. Of course, correlational studies are limited by the fact that there is no clear “model” in those studies and thus nothing can be said about causal effects on eating behavior; indeed, in these contexts, there is the potential for mutual influence by both members of the dyad. It is also the case, however, that correlational studies more closely approximate “natural” eating contexts because one rarely (if ever) eats with a confederate who has been instructed to eat a particular amount. Furthermore, correlational studies have the advantage of increasing variability in food intake because intake is not constrained by the experimenters' instructions, allowing researchers to examine the relationship between co-eaters' food intake (Salvy, Jarrin, Paluch, Irfan, & Pliner, Citation2007). It may be that the increased variability in participants' intake in correlational studies, as well as the reciprocal influence that each member of the dyad can have on the other person's food intake, accounts for the larger effect sizes observed in those studies compared to experimental studies. Capitalizing on the unique features of correlational designs, it would be interesting for future research to examine which factors determine who will lead and who will follow in naturally occurring social eating situations.

Very few other factors that we examined were significant moderators of the modeling effect. In terms of characteristics of the participant, we found that women showed larger effects than did men. Although this sex difference is consistent with research suggesting that women might be more concerned with how they are viewed by others while they are eating (Vartanian et al., Citation2007), other work suggests that men are more influenced by external eating cues, including portion size (Zlatevska, Dubelaar, & Holden, Citation2014), and that men show stronger social facilitation effects (Bellisle, Dalix, & de Castro, Citation1999). Thus, whether the observed sex difference in modeling effects reflects a genuine difference in the extent to which women's (relative to men's) food intake is influenced by social cues, or reflects something more mundane such as differences in how undergraduate male participants respond to free food, remains an open question. We also found that, among children, participants' mean age was positively correlated with the magnitude of the modeling effect. This finding is consistent with other research showing, for example, that older children are more likely to be influenced by portion size than are younger children (Rolls, Engell, & Birch, Citation2000), and suggests that a tendency to rely on external cues (and perhaps a tendency to want to eat “appropriately”) is learned over time. Determining at what age children begin relying less on internal signals and more on normative signals with respect to their food intake would be an important direction for future research.

With respect to characteristics of the model, leaner models produced somewhat larger effects than did heavier models but this difference was not statistically significant, probably because of the small number of studies represented. It may be that obese models elicit less modeling because they are not seen as being relevant guides to appropriate behavior or that people somehow adjust for assumed baseline differences in how much obese people eat. We might also expect that participants' own weight status would interact with the confederate's weight status in predicting the degree of modeling. Future research is needed to clarify the effect of participants' and models' weight status on food intake, as well as the mechanisms underlying those effects.

An important finding from our meta-analysis is the fact that we observed identical effect sizes for studies that used a live model versus a remote confederate. How can modeling effects occur when the “model” is not actually present? Vartanian et al. (Citation2013) found that both live models and remote confederates create and convey a norm of appropriate food intake, which in turn affects how much participants eat. However, it may be that some of the mechanisms underlying modeling effects differ between live models and remote confederates. For example, Robinson et al. (Citation2011) and Robinson, Benwell, and Higgs (Citation2013) have argued that empathy plays an important role in modeling the food intake of another person, but not in modeling of remote confederates. It might also be that some components of modeling (e.g., behavioral mimicry; Hermans, Lichtwarck-Aschoff, et al., Citation2012) are observed only with live models. Future research examining the different processes underlying modeling effects in live and remote confederate designs would help elucidate the mechanisms underlying the modeling of food intake.

Although we were able to examine a number of moderators of effect sizes, there are other factors that need to be considered in future research that would broaden our understanding of the scope and limits of modeling effects. For example, little is known about the modeling of healthy foods, such as fruits and vegetables. Most modeling studies employ highly palatable but unhealthy foods, such as pizza or cookies. Only one study has examined modeling of healthy snacks (Hermans, Larsen, et al., Citation2009) and, although modeling of vegetable intake was observed, it is not known how consistent that effect is, what the mechanisms are, or whether there are any moderators of the effect. Given that very few people meet their target recommendations for fruit and vegetable consumption (e.g., Guenther, Dodd, Reedy, & Krebs-Smith, Citation2006), research examining the modeling of healthy food intake would be an important direction for future research.

Why do people model other people's food intake? Herman et al. (Citation2003) argued that the appropriate amount to eat is often ambiguous and that people therefore rely on the example of others to determine how much they themselves should eat. Furthermore, Vartanian et al. (Citation2013) showed that perceived norms of appropriate intake mediated the link between a model's behavior and participants' own food intake. Surprisingly, our meta-analysis found no difference in the mean effect size for modeling between conditions that should elicit relatively high or relatively low concern with eating appropriately. Indeed, even conditions that are thought to “minimize” modeling effects (e.g., low appropriateness-concern conditions) still produce sizable effects (average r = .36). It should be noted, however, that those studies used experimental manipulations or self-report measures that only indirectly tapped into people's concern with eating appropriately (e.g., trait empathy or eating with an unsociable confederate). It may be that some of the methods used to either manipulate or measure appropriateness concerns do not adequately capture that construct. It is also possible that most of the participants in these studies—even many of those who were categorized as low in concern for appropriateness on the basis of a median split on some measure—were relatively high in concern for appropriateness. A more compelling test of the possible role of concern for appropriateness awaits direct manipulation of people's level of concern with eating appropriately, the identification of measures that adequately capture concern with behaving appropriately, and/or the identification of samples that are demonstrably high and low in this attribute.

Situating the findings of the current meta-analysis in the context of other external influences on food intake, a recent meta-analysis of the effect of portion size on people's food intake found that the mean effect size was r = .22 (a moderate effect; Zlatevska et al., Citation2014). Although direct comparisons between the two literatures are difficult due to various methodological differences, it is notable that the average modeling effect in our meta-analysis (r = .39) is considerably larger than the average portion-size effect reported by Zlatevska et al. (Citation2014). The effect of larger portions has justly received a great deal of attention in the research literature and in the popular media, but the findings of the present meta-analysis suggest that modeling effects deserve just as much attention.

Conclusion

Social models provide a powerful influence on people's food intake, with implications for their ability to appropriately regulate food intake. First, eating with a low-intake model can lead people to restrict their food intake. Although in some cases restriction (or reduced “overconsumption”) might be seen as healthy and desirable, in other cases it might exacerbate unhealthy restrictive eating patterns, particularly among individuals at risk for disordered eating. Second, eating with a high-intake model can lead to overindulgence and excess energy intake. The potential for overindulgence in social situations might be heightened by the contemporary food environment, characterized by widespread availability of high-calorie foods and oversized portions (Brownell & Horgen, Citation2004). Efforts to help people eat a healthy diet might potentially include using social models to promote the consumption of healthy foods, as well as helping people discern when a model's food intake is an appropriate guide to behavior and when it is not.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Suzanna Azevedo, Alana Eyles, and Robyn Winwood-Smith for their assistance retrieving and coding the articles. We also wish to thank the authors who generously responded to our request for additional data.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Additional information

Funding

This research was supported under Australian Research Council's Discovery Projects funding scheme (project number DP110101124).

References

References marked with an asterisk (*) indicate studies included in the meta-analysis

  • *Addessi, E., Galloway, A. T., Visalberghi, E., & Birch, L. L. (2005). Specific social influences on the acceptance of novel foods in 2-5-year-old children. Appetite, 45, 264–271. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2005.07.007.
  • Bellisle, F., Dalix, A. -M., & de Castro, J. M. (1999). Eating patterns in French subjects studied by the “weekly food diary” method. Appetite, 32, 46–52. doi:10.1006/appe.1998.0195.
  • *Bevelander, K. E., Anschütz, D. J., Creemers, D. H. M., Kleinjan, M., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2013). The role of explicit and implicit self-esteem in peer modeling of palatable food intake: A study on social media interaction among youngsters. PLoS ONE, 8, e72481. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0072481.
  • *Bevelander, K. E., Anschütz, D. J., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2012). Social norms in food intake among normal weight and overweight children. Appetite, 58, 864–872. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.02.003.
  • *Bevelander, K. E., Meiselman, H. L., Anschütz, D. J., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2013). Television watching and the emotional impact on social modeling of food intake among children. Appetite, 63, 70–76. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.12.015.
  • Brownell, K. D., & Horgen, K. B. (2004). Food fight: The inside story of the food industry, America's obesity crisis, and what we can do about it. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill, Contemporary Books.
  • *Brunner, T. A. (2010). How weight-related cues affect food intake in a modeling situation. Appetite, 55, 507–511. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2010.08.018.
  • *Brunner, T. A. (2012). Matching effects on eating. Individual differences do make a difference!. Appetite, 58, 429–431. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.12.003.
  • Christakis, N. A., & Fowler, J. H. (2007). The spread of obesity in a large social network over 32 years. New England Journal of Medicine, 357, 370–379. doi:10.1056/NEJMsa066082.
  • *Conger, J. C., Conger, A. J., Costanzo, P. R., Wright, K. L., & Matter, J. A. (1980). The effect of social cues on the eating behavior of obese and normal subjects. Journal of Personality, 48, 258–271. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1980.tb00832.x.
  • *Cruwys, T., Platow, M. J., Angullia, S. A., Chang, J. M., Diler, S. E., Kirchner, J. L., … Wadley, A. L. (2012). Modeling of food intake is moderated by salient psychological group membership. Appetite, 58, 754–757. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.12.002.
  • de Castro, J. M., & Brewer, E. M. (1992). The amount eaten in meals by humans is a power function of the number of people present. Physiology & Behavior, 51, 121–125. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(92)90212-K.
  • Exline, J. J., Zell, A. L., Bratslavsky, E., Hamilton, M., & Swenson, A. (2012). People-pleasing through eating: Sociotropy predicts greater eating in response to perceived social pressure. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 31, 169–193. doi:10.1521/jscp.2012.31.2.169.
  • *Feeney, J. R., Polivy, J., Pliner, P., & Sullivan, M. D. (2011). Comparing live and remote models in eating conformity research. Eating Behaviors, 12, 75–77. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2010.09.007.
  • Field, A. P. (2003). The problems in using fixed-effects models of meta-analysis on real-world data. Understanding Statistics, 2, 105–124. doi:10.1207/S15328031US0202_02.
  • Field, A. P., & Gillett, R. (2010). How to do a meta-analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 63, 665–694, .doi:10.1348/000711010X502733.
  • *Florack, A., Palcu, J., & Friese, M. (2013). The moderating role of regulatory focus on the social modeling of food intake. Appetite, 69, 114–122. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2013.05.012.
  • *Goldman, S. J., Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1991). Is the effect of a social model on eating attenuated by hunger?Appetite, 17, 129–140. doi:10.1016/0195-6663(91)90068-4.
  • Guenther, P. M., Dodd, K. W., Reedy, J., & Krebs-Smith, S. M. (2006). Most Americans eat much less than recommended amounts of fruits and vegetables. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 106, 1371–1379. doi:10.1016/j.jada.2006.06.002.
  • Herman, C. P. (2015). The social facilitation of eating: A review. Appetite, 86, 61–73. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2014.09.016.
  • *Herman, C. P., Koenig-Nobert, S., Peterson, J. B., & Polivy, J. (2005). Matching effects on eating: Do individual differences make a difference?Appetite, 45, 108–109. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2005.03.013.
  • Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (2005). Normative influences on food intake. Physiology & Behavior, 86, 762–772. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.08.064.
  • Herman, C. P., Roth, D. A., & Polivy, J. (2003). Effects of the presence of others on food intake: A normative interpretation. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 873–886. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.873.
  • *Hermans, R. C. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., Larsen, J. K., & Herman, C. P. (2009). Modeling of palatable food intake. The influence of quality of social interaction. Appetite, 52, 801–804. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2009.03.008.
  • *Hermans, R. C. J., Herman, C. P., Larsen, J. K., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2010a). Social modeling effects on snack intake among young men. The role of hunger. Appetite, 54, 378–383. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2010.01.006.
  • *Hermans, R. C. J., Herman, C. P., Larsen, J. K., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2010b). Social modeling effects on young women's breakfast intake. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 110, 1901–1905. doi:10.1016/j.jada.2010.09.007.
  • *Hermans, R. C. J., Larsen, J. K., Herman, C. P., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2008). Modeling of palatable food intake in female young adults. Effects of perceived body size. Appetite, 51, 512–518. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2008.03.016.
  • *Hermans, R. C. J., Larsen, J. K., Herman, C. P., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2009). Effects of social modeling on young women's nutrient-dense food intake. Appetite, 53, 135–138. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2009.05.004.
  • *Hermans, R. C. J., Larsen, J. K., Lochbuehler, K., Nederkoorn, C., Herman, C. P., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2013). The power of social influence over food intake: Examining the effects of attentional bias and impulsivity. British Journal of Nutrition, 109, 572–580. doi:10.1017/S0007114512001390.
  • *Hermans, R. C. J., Larsen, J. K., Herman, C. P., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2012). How much should I eat? Situational norms affect young women's food intake during meal time. British Journal of Nutrition, 107, 588–594. doi:10.1017/S0007114511003278.
  • Hermans, R. C. J., Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A., Bevelander, K. E., Herman, C. P., Larsen, J. K., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2012). Mimicry of food intake: The dynamic interplay between eating companions. PLoS ONE, 7, e31027. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0031027.
  • *Hermans, R. C. J., Salvy, S.-J., Larsen, J. K., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2012). Examining the effects of remote-video confederates on young women's food intake. Eating Behaviors, 13, 246–251. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.03.008.
  • *Howland, M., Hunger, J. M., & Mann, T. (2012). Friends don't let friends eat cookies: Effects of restrictive eating norms on consumption among friends. Appetite, 59, 505–509. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.06.020.
  • *Johnston, L. (2002). Behavioral mimicry and stigmatization. Social Cognition, 20, 18–35. doi:10.1521/soco.20.1.18.20944.
  • Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • *McFerran, B., Dahl, D. W., Fitzsimons, G. J., & Morales, A. C. (2010). I'll have what she's having: Effects of social influence and body type on the food choices of others. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 915–929. doi:10.1086/644611.
  • *Nisbett, R. E., & Storms, M. D. (1974). Cognitive and social determinants of food intake. In H.London & R. E.Nisbett (Eds.), Thought and feeling: Cognitive alteration of feeling states (pp. 190–208). Chicago, IL: Aldine.
  • Orwin, R. G. (1983). A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Statistics, 8, 157–159. doi:10.2307/1164923.
  • *Pliner, P., & Mann, N. (2004). Influence of social norms and palatability on amount consumed and food choice. Appetite, 42, 227–237. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2003.12.001.
  • *Polivy, J., Herman, C. P., Younger, J. C., & Erskine, B. (1979). Effects of a model on eating behavior: The induction of a restrained eating style. Journal of Personality, 47, 100–117. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1979.tb00617.x.
  • *Robinson, E., Benwell, H., & Higgs, S. (2013). Food intake norms increase and decrease snack food intake in a remote confederate study. Appetite, 65, 20–24. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2013.01.010.
  • Robinson, E., Thomas, J., Aveyard, P., & Higgs, S. (2014). What everyone else is eating: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of informational eating norms on eating behavior. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 114, 414–429. doi:10.1016/j.jand.2013.11.009.
  • *Robinson, E., Tobias, T., Shaw, L., Freeman, E., & Higgs, S. (2011). Social matching of food intake and the need for social acceptance. Appetite, 56, 747–752. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.03.001.
  • Rolls, B. J., Engell, D., & Birch, L. L. (2000). Serving portion size influences 5-year-old but not 3-year-old children's food intakes. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 100, 232–234. doi:10.1016/S0002-8223(00)00070-5.
  • *Romero, N. D., Epstein, L. H., & Salvy, S.-J. (2009). Peer modeling influences girls' snack intake. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 109, 133–136. doi:10.1016/j.jada.2008.10.005.
  • *Rosenthal, B., & Marx, R. B. (1979). Modeling influences on the eating behavior of successful and unsuccessful dieters and untreated normal weight individuals. Addictive Behaviors, 4, 215–221. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(79)90030-3.
  • *Rosenthal, B., & McSweeney, F. K. (1979). Modeling influences on eating behavior. Addictive Behavior, 4, 205–214. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(79)90029-7.
  • Rosenthal, R., Rosnow, R. L., & Rubin, D. B. (2000). Contrasts and effect sizes in behavioral research: A correlational approach. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • *Roth, D. A., Herman, C. P., Polivy, J., & Pliner, P. (2001). Self-presentational conflict in social eating situations: A normative perspective. Appetite, 36, 165–171. doi:10.1006/appe.2000.0388.
  • *Salvy, S. -J., Howard, M., Read, M., & Mele, E. (2009). The presence of friends increases food intake in youth. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 90, 282–287. doi:10.3945/ajcn.2009.27658.
  • *Salvy, S.-J., Jarrin, D., Paluch, R., Irfan, N., & Pliner, P. (2007). Effects of social influence on eating in couples, friends and strangers. Appetite, 49, 92–99. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.12.004.
  • *Salvy, S. -J., Kieffer, E., & Epstein, L. H. (2008). Effects of social context on overweight and normal-weight children's food selection. Eating Behaviors, 9, 190–196. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2007.08.001.
  • *Salvy, S. -J., Romero, N., Paluch, R., & Epstein, L. H. (2007). Peer influence on pre-adolescent girls' snack intake: Effects of weight status. Appetite, 49, 177–182. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2007.01.011.
  • *Salvy, S. -J., Vartanian, L. R., Coelho, J. S., Jarrin, D., & Pliner, P. P. (2008). The role of familiarity on modeling of eating and food consumption in children. Appetite, 50, 514–518. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2007.10.009.
  • Spanos, S., Vartanian, L. R., Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (2014). Failure to report social influences on food intake: Lack of awareness or motivated denial?Health Psychology, 33, 1487–1494. doi:10.1037/hea0000008.
  • Vartanian, L. R. (2015). Impression management and food intake: Current directions in research. Appetite, 86, 74–80. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2014.08.021.
  • Vartanian, L. R., Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (2007). Consumption stereotypes and impression management: How you are what you eat. Appetite, 48, 265–277. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.10.008.
  • *Vartanian, L. R., Sokol, N., Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (2013). Social models provide a norm of appropriate food intake for young women. PLoS One, 8, e79268. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0079268.
  • Zlatevska, N., Dubelaar, C., & Holden, S. S. (2014). Sizing up the effect of portion size on consumption: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Marketing, 78, 140–154. doi:10.1509/jm.12.0303.