4,048
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Measurement, Statistics, and Research Design

Testing associations between global and specific levels of student academic motivation and engagement in the classroom

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, , & ORCID Icon

Abstract

Using data from 4047 adolescents in three countries, this study was designed to investigate the associations between two important components of the learning process: academic motivation and student engagement. To increase the precision and accuracy of these analyses, preliminary analyses were conducted to identify the optimal measurement structure of both constructs, leading us to retain a bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling representation of academic motivation and of a partial bifactor confirmatory factor analytic representation for student engagement. Our main analyses revealed that academic motivation factors were able to explain almost 66% of the variance in global levels of engagement, and between 5% and 35% of the variance in specific levels of engagement. Finally, mediation analyses supported the role of emotional engagement as a mediator of the association between academic motivation and global and specific behavioral forms of engagement.

ACADEMIC MOTIVATION AND student engagement have both been found to be related to a variety of desirable educational outcomes, such as higher academic achievement (Eccles, Citation2004; Fredricks et al., Citation2004; Roksa & Whitley, Citation2017; Steinmayr & Spinath, Citation2009; Wu, Citation2019) and more positive developmental trajectories (Lazowski & Hulleman, Citation2016; Lerner et al., Citation2011; Reschly & Christenson, Citation2012; Wigfield & Cambria, Citation2010), as well as with lower rates of grade retention and dropout (Allen, Citation1999; Appleton et al., Citation2008; Finn, Citation1989; Finn & Rock, Citation1997; Furlong & Christenson, Citation2008). Whereas motivation is generally assumed, from a theoretical perspective, to predate engagement (Ainley, Citation2012; Froiland & Worrell, Citation2016; Green et al., Citation2012; Martin et al., Citation2017; Reeve, Citation2012; Skinner, Kindermann, Connell, et al., Citation2009), both motivation and engagement are broad multidimensional constructs whose correct operationalization requires the ability to account for this multidimensional structure and their subdimensions. This complexity, and the variety of approaches used to operationalize both constructs, could potentially explain why research results have typically led to inconsistent conclusions regarding their associations and the practical implications of these associations (Christenson et al., Citation2012; Martin et al., Citation2017). This study was designed to test the associations between motivation and engagement by clarifying the multidimensional structure of both constructs and, in doing so, to achieve a more accurate picture of their associations. The study also responds to prior calls for more integrative approaches to motivation and engagement research (Martin, Citation2007; Reschly & Christenson, Citation2012).

Definitions of Academic Motivation and Student Engagement

Anchored in the theoretical underpinnings of self-determination theory ( SDT; Deci, Citation1975; Deci & Ryan, Citation1985 ), Vallerand et al. (Citation1989, Citation1992) have defined academic motivation as a multidimensional construct varying in quantity (different global levels of self-determination) and retaining a specific quality captured by its subdimensions (intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation and amotivation). Based on this perspective, intrinsic motivation refers to engaging in an activity for the pleasure and satisfaction that it procures, either by allowing students to learn something new, to experience stimulation, or to accomplish something. In contrast, extrinsic motivation refers to engaging in an activity for instrumental reasons (i.e., to obtain a reward). Finally, amotivation refers to a lack of desire, reason, or motive for engaging in the activity. Deci and Ryan (Citation1985) note that extrinsic motivation encompasses various forms of motivation differentiated by the extent to which the perceived instrumentality of the activity is internally or externally driven. At its most extreme, external regulation occurs when involvement in an activity is underpinned by a desire to gain rewards, to avoid punishment, or by social pressure. In contrast, introjected regulation refers to the internalization of these various contingencies, so that activity involvement becomes driven by internal pressures, such as by a desire to avoid negative emotions (such as shame or culpability), to preserve one’s self-esteem, or to achieve a sense of pride. Finally, identified regulation refers to involvement in an activity seen as personally relevant and driven by personally-endorsed values and objectives (such as acceptance, value, importance). Despite acknowledging the unique qualities associated with each of these specific subdimensions of motivation, SDT also positions them along a single self-determination continuum (Deci & Ryan, Citation1985; Ryan & Deci, Citation2017), ranging from intrinsic motivation, to identified regulation, to introjected regulation, to external regulation, and finally to amotivation.

Conversely, student engagement refers to students’ active participation in academic, co-curricular, and school-related activities, as well as to their commitment to educational goals and learning (Christenson et al., Citation2012). Just like motivation, student engagement is generally conceptualized as a multidimensional construct encompassing various subdimensions. However, unlike motivation (at least when seen from the perspective of SDT), there is little agreement on the number and types of subdimensions of student engagement. For instance, Fredricks et al. (Citation2004) and Archambault et al. (Citation2009) differentiate behavioral, emotional and cognitive forms of engagement, whereas Finn and Zimmer (Citation2012) propose to consider academic, social, cognitive and affective forms of engagement. Even more comprehensive, Appleton et al. (Citation2006) identified six subdimensions of engagement encompassing teacher-student relationships, control and relevance of school work, peer support for learning, future aspirations and goals, family support for learning, and extrinsic motivation. Many of these previous conceptualizations, however, confound manifestations of student engagement occurring within the classroom context, with additional aspects of student engagement occurring outside of that critical classroom context (Dierendonck et al., Citation2020). Given the wide range of conceptualizations found in the literature on student engagement, concept clarification and dimensionality testing of student engagement thus seems to remain a salient research avenue in this specific field of research. In this regard, the present study adopts a recent comprehensive conceptualization of student engagement (Dierendonck et al., Citation2020) which exclusively focuses on specific subdimensions of behavioral (i.e., effort/attention and boredom/distraction), emotional (i.e., social and learning), and cognitive (i.e., strategies and autoregulation) manifestations of student engagement occurring within the classroom context. Similar to motivation, a global engagement construct can be specified to provide, following Christenson et al.’s (Citation2012) definition, a direct reflective indicator of students’ global levels of commitment and persistence toward their academic activities, educational goals, and learning in general, thus encapsulating students’ global levels of engagement in their academic activities. From this perspective, and similar to other studies in engagement literature (e.g., Gillet et al., Citation2020; Olivier et al., Citation2020; Tóth-Király et al., Citation2021), students are proposed to experience academic engagement in a more holistic manner as a single overarching dimension. In this conception, the specific engagement dimensions are posited to refer to the presence of students’ actions and conduct in class (i.e., behavioral), their affective reactions in class (i.e., emotional), and their non-visible thought processes in class (i.e., cognitive) over and above, as well as independently from, their global engagement tendencies. These specific dimensions also refer to the extent to which behavioral, cognitive and emotional engagement deviates from the global levels of engagement.1

Academic Motivation and Student Engagement: Relations and Mediation

In the empirical and theoretical research literature, a broad agreement exists to the effect that: (1) motivation and engagement are distinct but related constructs (Christenson et al., Citation2012; Skinner et al., Kindermann, & Furrer, Citation2009), (2) motivation is an antecedent to student engagement (Anderman & Patrick, Citation2012; Reeve, Citation2012; Schunk & Mullen, Citation2012; Skinner, Kindermann, Connell et al., Citation2009; Wigfield & Guthrie, Citation2010), and (3) the benefits of motivation in terms of achievement are generally expected to occur via student engagement (Ainley, Citation2012; Green et al., Citation2012; Martin et al., Citation2017; Reeve, Citation2012; Voelkl, Citation2012). However, very little is known regarding how the global and specific facets of these two constructs relate to one another.

Our first objective is to address this limitation by testing the associations between global and specific facets of academic motivation and engagement. Based on previous research (Dierendonck et al., Citation2020; Howard et al., Citation2018; Litalien et al., Citation2017; Stefansson et al., Citation2016; Wang et al., Citation2016), we expect stronger positive associations to occur between global levels of academic motivation and student engagement than among their more specific facets. From a theoretical perspective, students are more likely to display enhanced commitment and persistence toward academic activities (i.e., high global levels of engagement) when they are able to act with a full sense of volition (i.e., high global levels of self-determination) because they find the activity interesting, exciting, and personally valuable, while not feeling obligated to engage in it (Ryan & Deci, Citation2017). Self-determined students are thus expected to engage with academic activities more globally and intensely.

We also expect some significant and meaningful relations to occur at the more specific level. When considering the effects of specific facets of academic motivation, abundant empirical research (e.g., Cox et al., Citation2013; Litalien et al., Citation2017; Liu et al., Citation2009; Ryan & Deci, Citation2017) lead us to expect positive associations between specific levels of intrinsic motivation and identified regulation and the positive facets of school engagement (higher levels of effort/attention, positive emotions related to others and to learning, use of cognitive strategies and autoregulation, and lower levels of boredom/distraction), smaller or negative associations between specific levels of introjected and external regulation and these positive facets of engagement, and negative associations between specific levels of amotivation and these positive facets of engagement. These expectations are in line with SDT’s theoretical propositions (Ryan & Deci, Citation2017) suggesting that students driven by interest and enjoyment (intrinsic) or the accepted value and personal importance of learning (identified) are more likely to display enhanced levels of engagement and persistence in an activity. In contrast, being driven toward learning by internal (introjected) or external pressures is less likely to facilitate the development of persistent engagement because students do not have an inherent desire to learn. In contrast, when students are amotivated, they lack any drive and willingness to perform any learning-related activity, in turn decreasing their engagement.

Expected associations between motivation and engagement become slightly more complex when considering how the effects of motivation would differ as a function of the specific emotional, behavioral, and cognitive components of student engagement. In this regard, taking anchor in a wide range of motivational theories (e.g., Deci & Ryan, Citation1985; Harter, Citation1978; Pekrun et al., Citation2002), Skinner et al. (Citation2008) suggested that emotional forms of engagement (interest, enthusiasm) were likely to play a key role in the emergence of other (cognitive and behavioral) forms of engagement. This proposition thus positions emotional forms of engagement as mediators of the effects of various predictors (including motivation) on other facets of student engagement. Hence, this proposition is important to our understanding of the possible psychological mechanisms underpinning the associations between motivation and engagement, as well as to how motivation carries over to engagement. Likewise, Green et al. (Citation2012) compared three models depicting the associations between academic motivation, emotional engagement (positive attitude toward school), behavioral engagement (class participation, homework completion, absenteeism) and performance. Their results supported the heuristic superiority of a model conceptualizing emotional engagement as a predictor of behavioral engagement rather than as being located at the same level of the predictive sequence. Their results showed that motivation predicted emotional engagement, which in turn predicted behavioral engagement dimensions (i.e., higher levels of class participation and homework completion, and lower levels of absenteeism), which themselves predicted performance. The present study tests this mediation effect.

Dimensionality of Academic Motivation and Student Engagement

In order to avoid measurement imprecision and resulting bias in the estimation of associations with other constructs (Asparouhov et al., Citation2015; Mai et al., Citation2018; Morin, Arens, & Marsh, Citation2016), the present study first seeks to identify the optimal way to represent the multidimensional structure of students’ ratings of their academic motivation and engagement. More precisely, this preliminary investigation of the factor structure of motivation and engagement relies on the overarching bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling (bifactor-ESEM) framework (Morin, Arens, & Marsh, Citation2016; Morin, Arens, Tran, et al., Citation2016). First, this analytic framework makes it possible to account for the multidimensional structure of these two constructs (motivation and engagement) via the extraction of a global component reflecting the globality of each construct (shared across all subdimensions) distinct from a non-redundant estimate of the specificity unique to each subdimension. For instance, this framework makes it possible to obtain a direct and explicit estimate of students’ global levels of self-determination or engagement via the estimation of a global factor (G-factor) underpinning responses to all items include in each specific instrument.2 This G-factor co-exists with specific factors (S-factors) depicting the unique quality associated with each subscale and left unexplained by the G-factor. Furthermore, the bifactor-ESEM framework also makes it possible to account for the fallible nature of the indicators used to assess each construct by allowing cross-loadings to be freely estimated among all factors used to reflect the multidimensional structure of each instrument (Morin et al., Citation2020). This component thus takes into account the fact that items typically present at least some degree of association with other conceptually-related constructs. Statistical research (Asparouhov et al., Citation2015; Mai et al., Citation2018) has shown that this free estimation of cross-loadings tends to result in a more accurate estimate of latent constructs and relations among constructs (relative to CFA), whenever cross-loadings as small as .100 are present in the data, but to remain unbiased in the absence of cross-loadings. Importantly, the reliance on target rotation makes it possible to rely on a theoretically driven definition of the factors, while allowing all cross-loadings to be freely estimated by assigned a target value of zero.

Academic Motivation. SDT researchers have adopted a variety of approaches, each with their own unique pros and cons, in order to test the theoretical structure of the motivation continuum expected to underlie all types of motivation (for a recent review, see Howard et al., Citation2020). The bifactor-ESEM framework was recently proposed as a way to bridge these various methodological approaches in a way that made it possible to obtain direct estimates of the co-existing global (i.e., a G-factor reflecting a continuum of self-determination) and specific (i.e., a series of S-factors reflecting the unique quality associated with students’ ratings of intrinsic motivation, identified regulation, introjected regulation, external regulation, and amotivation) structure of academic (using the Academic Motivation Scale: Litalien et al., Citation2017) and of work (using the Multidimensional Work Motivation Scale: Howard et al., Citation2018) motivations. In these studies, the G-factor was found to perfectly match the SDT continuum hypothesis, being characterized by strong positive loadings from intrinsic motivation items, moderate positive loadings from identified regulation items, smaller positive loadings from introjected regulation items, null or negative loadings from the external regulation items, and stronger negative loadings from the amotivation items. In the present study, we hereafter refer to this G-factor as reflecting students’ global levels of self-determination, defined as students’ global sense of self-directedness and volition. Both studies also revealed that, although the G-factor was the key driver of associations with a variety of covariates (i.e., vitality, ill-being, achievement, dropout intentions, satisfaction with studies, and need satisfaction in Litalien et al., Citation2017, and commitment and need satisfaction in Howard et al., Citation2018), the S-factors were also able to explain additional variance in outcome levels beyond that already explained by the G-factor. However, despite the promising nature of these results, they have yet to be replicated within each of these fields of research. In addition, outcome associations have yet to be examined in relation to other motivational constructs (such as engagement) known to also present a global/specific nature.

Student Engagement. Although it is generally recognized that student engagement, like motivation, can be operationalized using both global and specific components, research into the optimal structure of school engagement still lags behind that of academic motivation, possibly due to the lack of conceptual consensus regarding the optimal conceptualization. However, across conceptualizations, research has often found that a global level of student engagement could be estimated from a series of well-defined specific student engagement dimensions (e.g., Archambault et al., Citation2009; Lam et al., 2014). More recent studies have rather adopted the more flexible bifactor approach, and found support for the idea that students’ ratings of their own engagement typically encompassed co-existing global (i.e., reflecting their global level of engagement across dimensions) and specific (i.e., reflecting the unique quality associated with students’ levels of behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement over and above the global factor) facets (e.g., Dierendonck et al., Citation2020; Stefansson et al., Citation2016; Wang et al., Citation2016). These studies also found that specific engagement dimensions were able to predict additional variance in academic achievement (Stefansson et al., Citation2016; Wang et al., Citation2016), aspirations (Wang et al., Citation2016) and misconduct (Dierendonck et al., Citation2020) over and above that already explained by the global engagement factor. Importantly, Dierendonck et al.’s (Citation2020) study relied on the same comprehensive measure of student engagement used in the present study and found that the global engagement factor was able to co-exist with six specific factors reflecting the a priori facets of student behavioral (i.e., effort/attention and boredom/distraction), emotional (i.e., social and learning), and cognitive (i.e., strategies and autoregulation) engagement.

It is important to note that in two of these studies (Dierendonck et al., Citation2020; Wang et al., Citation2016), the global factor was found to account for a much larger part of the variance in ratings of the cognitive and behavioral engagement items relative to that of emotional engagement items, suggesting that emotional engagement might represent a component of student engagement distinct from that measured globally across behavioral and cognitive engagement items. Moreover, although these three studies supported the role of global levels of student engagement in the prediction of achievement, aspirations and misconduct, they generally led to discrepant results regarding the exact role of specific engagement dimensions in these predictions, thus mirroring the discrepant results obtained with higher-order factor models (Archambault et al., Citation2009; Lam et al., 2014). A possible source of explanation for these discrepant results may come from the lack of consideration of cross-loadings in these models, an exclusion which has been statistically shown not only to potentially result in a lack of clarity in terms of construct definition (Asparouhov et al., Citation2015), but also in the possible inflation of the variance explained by the G-factor in bifactor models (Morin, Arens, & Marsh, Citation2016; Murray & Johnson, Citation2013). When we more specifically consider student engagement, no single study has yet implemented a bifactor-ESEM approach to the measurement of engagement and supported the value of incorporating cross-loadings to the model. In the present study, we seek to bridge this measurement gap by contrasting correlated factors and bifactor CFA and ESEM representations of engagement.

The Present Study

To our knowledge, no study has yet examined the associations between motivation and engagement while relying on models allowing for a proper disaggregation of global and specific levels of motivation and engagement. This is the objective of the present study. Based on the available empirical information, we hypothesize that the bifactor-ESEM solution will provide the most accurate representation of students’ ratings of academic motivation, and that a bifactor approach (bifactor-CFA or bifactor-ESEM) will provide the most accurate representation of students’ ratings of engagement. For ratings of academic motivation, we also expect the G-factor to be associated with a factor loading pattern corresponding to the SDT continuum hypothesis (Howard et al., Citation2018; Litalien et al., Citation2017). As for the predictive model, we expect stronger positive associations to occur at the global levels of motivation and engagement than among more specific facets, but also expect some significant and meaningful relations to occur at the more specific level. Given prior studies (Dierendonck et al., Citation2020; Wang et al., Citation2016) showing that the general factor of engagement accounted for a large part of the variance of cognitive and behavioral engagement items, but not of emotional engagement items, two alternative predictive models, illustrated in , will be contrasted. These models will include, or not, a global factor depending on whether the optimal solution for both constructs follows a bifactor representation. In the first model, no mediation is present, and facets of engagement are all specified as outcomes of motivation facets located at the same position in the a priori motivational dynamic. In the second model, emotional engagement facets are expected to mediate, fully or partially, the effects of motivation facets of behavioral and cognitive engagement facets.

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the potential predictive models of academic motivation predicting engagement in the classroom.

Note. G: Global factor; INT: Intrinsic motivation; EIN: Extrinsic-identified regulation; EID: Extrinsic-introjected regulation. ER: External regulation. A: Amotivation. BE: Behavioral engagement: Effort/attention; BB: Behavioral engagement: Boredom/distraction; ES: Emotional engagement: Social; EL: Emotional engagement: Learning; CS: Cognitive engagement: Strategies; CA: Cognitive engagement: Autoregulation.

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the potential predictive models of academic motivation predicting engagement in the classroom.Note. G: Global factor; INT: Intrinsic motivation; EIN: Extrinsic-identified regulation; EID: Extrinsic-introjected regulation. ER: External regulation. A: Amotivation. BE: Behavioral engagement: Effort/attention; BB: Behavioral engagement: Boredom/distraction; ES: Emotional engagement: Social; EL: Emotional engagement: Learning; CS: Cognitive engagement: Strategies; CA: Cognitive engagement: Autoregulation.

Method

Participants and Procedure

In this study, we rely on data collected in 2017-2018 as part of the FAVAS study funded by the European Union, conducted in three countries (France, Belgium, Luxembourg), among students enrolled in grades 7 to 12 in a total of 19 regular education secondary schools. These schools were recruited based on geographic location in France (Strasbourg district) and Luxembourg (North region). In Belgium, schools from the Wallonia-Brussels Federation were selected with the help of the Center for coordination and management of European programs. This study thus relied on a convenience sample of schools willing to participate in the study. A random selection of classrooms was made within each school to collect data at school level. A total of 4127 students were asked to complete a 50-minute online survey during school hours, in a single testing occasion, within a computer room located in their schools. Standardized instructions were available for administering the questionnaire in each school. The children were briefed on the nature of the questionnaire and on the confidentiality of their answers. Parental and students’ consent was obtained from all participants in this study. describes the analytic sample of 4047 students used in the present study who responded to the academic motivation and student engagement questionnaires. These students came from each of the 19 schools (with 30 to 520 students per school; M = 213; SD = 146) and from a total of 278 classroom (with 1 to 34 students per classroom; M = 15; SD = 8)

Table 1. Sample descriptive statistics (N = 4047).

Measures

Items used in the present study were already available in French for the academic motivation scale and in English for the student engagement scale. All items were administered in French in France and Belgium. In Luxembourg, items were administered either in German or in French. In line with the International Testing Commission (ITC) Guidelines for Translating and Adapting Tests (ITC, Citation2017), adaptation to French (student engagement) or German (both questionnaires) was done using a classical translation-back translation procedure conducted by two of the authors of the present study who are experts in the fields of Psychology, Education, and Sociology, and both native Luxembourgers fluent in German, French and English. The original items were first translated into the target language by one of the authors. A second translator (another coauthor of this article) then independently translated these items back to the original language without having seen the original items. Discrepancies between the original and the back-translated version were then examined by both authors. In the few cases where a discrepancy was found to affect the meaning of the items, the discrepancy was resolved by consensus between the two translators and by the consultation of two other bilingual authors when necessary. To ensure that the resulting items were clear, a convenience sample of several German-Speaking and French-speaking secondary school students and teachers were asked to review the questionnaire while commenting on clarity. This verification confirmed that all items were found to be clear and easy to understand by all pilot participants and did not need to be further adjusted for this study.

Student academic motivation was measured using a shorter version of the AMS (Vallerand et al., Citation1989) considering only one dimension of intrinsic motivation, as it has been suggested in several studies (e.g., Grouzet et al., Citation2006; Ratelle et al., Citation2007). The 20-item instrument covers five dimensions of intrinsic motivation (8 items, α = .882, e.g., For the pleasure I have in discovering new things never seen before), extrinsic-identified regulation (3 items, α = .787, e.g., Because it will allow me to work later on in a field I like), extrinsic-introjected regulation (3 items, α = .776, e.g., To prove to myself that I am capable of succeeding in high school), external regulation (3 items, α = .664, e.g., To be able to find a good job later on), and amotivation (3 items, α = .760, e.g., Honestly, I don't know; I really feel like I'm wasting my time at school). These dimensions are expected to form their own S-factor, and all items are also expected to contribute to the definition of a self-determination G-factor matching SDT continuum hypothesis (Litalien et al., Citation2017). All items were rated using a six-point scale (Totally disagree, Disagree, Rather disagree, Rather agree, Agree, Totally agree). Past research has established the scale score reliability, factor validity, convergent validity, and predictive validity of the short and long forms of the AMS (e.g., Grouzet et al., Citation2006; Guay et al., Citation2015; Litalien et al., Citation2017; Ratelle et al., Citation2007).

Student engagement was assessed using an 18 items measure developed by Dierendonck et al. (Citation2020) in order to provide a comprehensive coverage of engagement dimensions covered by a series of engagement measures (i.e., Appleton et al., Citation2006; Skinner et al., Citation2009; Wang et al., Citation2011). This measure covers the six dimensions (3 items per dimension) of behavioral engagement: Effort/attention (α = .861, e.g., When I’m in class, I listen very carefully), behavioral engagement: boredom/distraction (α = .799, e.g., I do something else during lessons), emotional engagement: Social (α = .817, e.g., I feel well integrated in this class); emotional engagement: Learning (α = .677, e.g., When I can’t answer a question in class, I feel frustrated), cognitive engagement: Strategies (α = .727, e.g., When I am doing schoolwork, I try to understand what these tasks are supposed to teach me), and cognitive engagement: Autoregulation (α = .758, e.g., I try to learn from my mistakes). These dimensions are expected to form their own S-factor, and all items are also expected to contribute to the definition of global engagement G-factor (Dierendonck et al., Citation2020). Each item was rated using a six-point Likert scale (Never, Almost never, Sometimes, Often, Quite often, Always). Dierendonck et al. (Citation2020) demonstrated the scale score reliability, factor validity and convergent validity of this measure.

Analyses

Analyses were conducted with the Robust Maximum Likelihood (MLR) estimator implemented in Mplus 8.4 (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2017). To handle the few missing responses at the item level (from .5 to 5%, M = 2.5%), full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation was used (Enders, Citation2010). The hierarchical nature of the data (students nested in classroom) was taken in account with the Mplus design-based adjustment implemented by the TYPE = COMPLEX function (Asparouhov, Citation2005).

Four alternative measurement models were estimated separately for academic motivation and student engagement (correlated factors CFA and ESEM, bifactor-CFA and ESEM), see for a schematic illustration. A more extensive presentation of the specification of these models is presented in Appendix 1 of the online supplements. When contrasting these models, we considered model fit information and their parameter estimates, following a sequential strategy advocated by Morin et al (Citation2020) and presented in Appendix 1 of the online supplements.

Figure 2. Alternative measurement models of academic motivation and engagement in the classroom.

Note. CFA: Confirmatory Factor Analysis. ESEM: Exploratory Structural Equation Model; G: Global factor; INT: Intrinsic motivation; EIN: Extrinsic-identified regulation; EID: Extrinsic-introjected regulation. ER: External regulation. A: Amotivation. BE: Behavioral engagement: Effort/attention; BB: Behavioral engagement: Boredom/distraction; ES: Emotional engagement: Social; EL: Emotional engagement: Learning; CS: Cognitive engagement: Strategies; CA: Cognitive engagement: Autoregulation.

Figure 2. Alternative measurement models of academic motivation and engagement in the classroom.Note. CFA: Confirmatory Factor Analysis. ESEM: Exploratory Structural Equation Model; G: Global factor; INT: Intrinsic motivation; EIN: Extrinsic-identified regulation; EID: Extrinsic-introjected regulation. ER: External regulation. A: Amotivation. BE: Behavioral engagement: Effort/attention; BB: Behavioral engagement: Boredom/distraction; ES: Emotional engagement: Social; EL: Emotional engagement: Learning; CS: Cognitive engagement: Strategies; CA: Cognitive engagement: Autoregulation.

Once the optimal solution was selected for each measure separately, the final measurement model for academic motivation was combined with the final measurement model for student engagement into three fully latent models designed to assess the associations between these two multidimensional constructs. In Model 1 (see the left-hand side of ), the academic motivation factors were allowed to predict3 all of the student engagement factors. This first model was estimated to obtain a first overview of associations between academic motivation and student engagement. In Model 2 and 3, the associations between the academic motivation factors and the behavioral and cognitive (as well as global if the retained model is bifactorial) engagement factors was assumed to be mediated by the emotional engagement factors. In Model 2, this mediation was assumed to be total (as illustrated by the full arrows on the right-hand side of ). In Model 3, direct paths were added to allow the motivation factors to directly influence the behavioral and cognitive (as well as global if the retained model is bifactorial) engagement factors beyond their effects on the emotional engagement factor (as illustrated by the dotted arrow on the right-hand side of ). Model 2 and 3 were contrasted to assess the mediation hypothesis, and to assess whether this mediation is total (Model 2) or partial (Model 3). The significance of indirect effects was tested via the calculation of bias-corrected bootstrap (1000 bootstrap samples) 95% confidence intervals (CI) (Cheung & Lau, Citation2008) and these effects were considered to be statistically significant when the confidence intervals excluded zero.

Model fit was assessed using fit indices and typical interpretation guidelines (Hu & Bentler, Citation1999; Marsh et al., Citation2004). More precisely, we report the chi-square statistic, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) with its 90% confidence interval, the comparative fit index (CFI) and the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI). Model fit was considered to be excellent when the RMSEA was below .06, and when the CFI and TLI were above .95. Model fit was considered to be acceptable when the RMSEA was below .08 and CFI and TLI were above .90. Although these indices have never been formally advocated for purposes of comparing alternative measurement specification, we also report the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), the Consistent AIC (CAIC), the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), and the Sample Size Adjusted BIC (SSBIC) upon request from a reviewer. For all of those indices, a lower value indicates better fit. Following Morin et al.’s (Citation2020) recommendations, we report classical model-based omega coefficients (ω) of composite reliability (McDonald, Citation1970) for all factors, calculated as: ω = (Σ|λi|)2/([Σ|λi|]2 + Σδii) where λi are the standardized factor loadings of the items on the target factor, and δii are the error variances of the items.

Results

The Multidimensional Structure of Academic Motivation

The fit of the alternative models is provided in the top section of . These results, coupled with a detailed examination of the parameter estimates from all of the alternative solutions, led us to retain the bifactor-ESEM solution. Detailed information about model selection and comparison is provided in Appendix 2 of the online supplements. The parameter estimates from this solution are reported in . The loadings obtained on the G-factor revealed a relatively well-defined global dimension representing participants’ global levels of self-determination (|λ| = .143 to .780; M|λ|= .516; ω = .923). Indeed, these loadings were strong and positive for intrinsic motivation items (λ = .557 to .780, Mλ = .692), positive and moderate for extrinsic-identified regulation (λ = .490 to .534, Mλ = .511) and extrinsic-introjected regulation (λ = .526 to .584, Mλ = .557) items and positive but smaller for external regulation items (λ = .241 to .416, Mλ = .312), and moderately negative for the amotivation items (λ = −.329 to −.143, Mλ = −.217). The extrinsic-introjected regulation (λ = .338 to .576, Mλ = .454; ω = .591), extrinsic-identified regulation (λ = .294 to .651, Mλ = .446; ω = .604), external regulation (λ = .403 to .654, Mλ = .502; ω = .583), and amotivation (λ = .640 to .708, Mλ = .678; ω = .744) S-factors were also reasonably well-defined. However, the intrinsic motivation S-factor was not defined as clearly (|λ| = .004 to .556; M|λ|= .174; ω = .368) due to the item loadings on this S-factor decreasing in magnitude and even becoming non-significant for items 4 and 7 in the presence of the G-factor. However, these results simply reflect the fact that the variance included in these intrinsic motivation items were mainly used in defining participants’ global levels of self-determination (i.e., the G-factor). For all of these reasons, the best-fitting bifactor-ESEM solution was thus retained for further analyses.

Table 2. Alternative measurement models.

Table 3. Standardized factor loadings (λ) and uniquenesses (δ) of the bifactor-CFA and bifactor-ESEM solutions: academic motivation.

The Multidimensional Structure of Student Engagement

The fit of the alternative models is provided in the bottom section of . These results, coupled with a detailed examination of the parameter estimates from all of the alternative solutions, led us to retain a partial bifactor-CFA solution in which the emotional engagement items were not used to define the engagement G-factor but were rather specified to form an independent correlated factor. This partial bifactor-CFA solution (see ) resulted in an excellent level of model fit according to all indicators, displaying an improvement relative to the initial bifactor-CFA solution (ΔCFI = +.005; ΔTLI = +.005; ΔRMSEA = −.002). Detailed information about model selection and comparison is provided in Appendix 2 of the online supplements. The parameter estimates from this solution are reported in and revealed a well-defined G-factor (|λ| = .338 to .697; M|λ|= .551; ω = .848). This G-factor was accompanied by well-defined S-factors for the effort/attention (λ = .311 to .635, Mλ = .488; ω = .707) and boredom/distraction (λ = .558 to .728, Mλ = .623; ω = .739) components of behavioral engagement, as well as for the strategies (λ = .223 to .561, Mλ = .419; ω = .517) and autoregulation (λ = .244 to .662, Mλ = .479; ω = .609) components of cognitive engagement. The S-factors reflecting the social (λ = .695 to .841, Mλ = .776; ω = .821) and learning (λ = .506 to .790, Mλ = .645; ω = .686) components of emotional engagement were also well-defined in this solution, allow us to retain this solution for further analyses.

Table 4. Standardized factor loadings (λ) and uniquenesses (δ) of the partial bifactor-CFA solution: student engagement.

Associations between Academic Motivation and Engagement

The retained bifactor-ESEM (for academic motivation) and partial bifactor-CFA (for student engagement) models were combined into a set of predictive models. The results from Model 1 are reported in the top section of . This model resulted in a satisfactory level of fit to the data (χ2 = 2340.153; df = 515; p ≤ .01; CFI = .962; TLI = .948; RMSEA = .030, 95% CI [.028, .031]). When considering the results from this model, it is first noteworthy that the proportion of variance in engagement explained by motivation (R2) is quite high for participants’ global levels of engagement (65.7%), moderate for their specific levels of the cognitive engagement strategies (35.5%) and autoregulation (33.3%) facets, but lower for their specific levels of behavioral engagement effort/attention (16.9%) and boredom/distraction (9.4%) facets, as well as for their levels of emotional engagement social (10.2%) and learning (5.4%) facets.

Table 5. Latent associations between academic motivation and school engagement.

With respect to individual path coefficients, several results are noteworthy. First, student’s global levels of engagement were significantly and positively predicted by their global levels of self-determination (β = .712) as well as by their specific levels of extrinsic-introjected regulation (β = .278), but were negatively predicted by their specific levels of extrinsic-identified regulation (β = −.133) and amotivation (β = −.232). In other words, these results show that the most engaged students tend to be those who display the highest global levels of self-determination and those who study for more introjected reasons. In contrast, students who study for identified reasons (i.e., ascribing personal importance to school work) over and above their global level of self-determination or who are not motivated for school work, tend to display lower levels of engagement.

Second, specific levels of intrinsic motivation (β = −.167) and of extrinsic-introjected regulation (β = −.335) were both found to predict lower levels of involvement in specific auto-regulatory cognitive engagement. Likewise, specific levels of extrinsic-introjected regulation (β = −.384) also predicted lower levels of involvement in specific cognitive engagement strategies. Thus, students whose academic motivation is influenced by interest and pleasure, or by more introjected reasons, will tend to invest more limited amounts of cognitive efforts in attempts to figure out how to solve learning challenges or difficulties. It is important to note that these effects occur over and above the desirable effects of global levels of self-determination and specific levels of extrinsic-introjected regulation on students’ global levels of engagement. In addition, it is noteworthy that none of these specific facets of cognitive engagement seems to be influenced by students’ global levels of self-determination.

Third, students’ specific levels on the effort/attention facet of behavioral engagement was found to be positively predicted by their levels of extrinsic-identified regulation (β = .144), but negatively predicted by their levels of extrinsic-introjected regulation (β = −.300). In contrast, specific levels on the boredom/distraction facet of behavioral engagement were positively predicted by students’ specific levels of extrinsic-introjected regulation (β = .111) and amotivation (β = .155), but negatively predicted by their specific levels of intrinsic motivation (β = −.125). Thus, it appears that students whose motivation is anchored in introjected reasons will tend to invest less efforts during learning activities, while those whose motivation is rather anchored in the personal importance of their studies (identified reasons) will tend to invest more efforts. In contrast, students lacking motivation, or driven by introjected reasons will display higher levels of boredom, whereas those studying for pleasure then will tend to display less boredom. Once again, these effects occur beyond the desirable effects of global levels of self-determination and specific levels of extrinsic-introjected regulation, as well as the negative effects of specific levels of extrinsic-identified regulation and amotivation on students’ global levels of engagement. As for cognitive engagement, none of these specific facets of behavioral engagement appears to be influenced by students’ global levels of self-determination.

Finally, the social (β = .232) and learning (β = .150) facets of emotional engagement both appeared to be significantly and positively predicted by students’ global levels of self-determination. In addition, students’ specific levels of intrinsic motivation (β = .116) and extrinsic-identified regulation (β = .065) both positively predicted students’ levels of social-emotional engagement, whereas levels of extrinsic-introjected regulation (β = .155) positively predicted their levels of learning-emotional engagement. In contrast, their specific levels of amotivation predicted lower levels of social-emotional engagement (β = −.168), but higher levels of learning-emotional engagement (β = .058). In other words, these students who display the highest global levels of self-determination tend to also be more emotionally engaged in their learning. Moreover, students interested in schoolwork perceived as being personally important (identified regulation) will also be more socially engaged in their learning. In contrast, students studying for mainly introjected reasons tend to be more emotionally concerned by new learnings or learning difficulties. Finally, students lacking motivation tend to display lower levels of social engagement but higher levels of emotional concerns.

Mediation Analyses

Results from the analyses of mediation first revealed that Model 3 (partial mediation: χ2 = 2327.535; df = 516; p ≤ .01; CFI = .962; TLI = .948; RMSEA = .029, 95% CI [.028, .031]) was able to achieve a much-improved level of fit to the data when compared to Model 2 (total mediation: χ2 = 3890.786; df = 546; p ≤ .01; CFI = .930; TLI = .910; RMSEA = .039, 95% CI [.038, .040]). Model 3 was thus retained for interpretation. The results from this model are reported in the bottom section of and summarized in . When we first consider the direct effects of the academic motivation factors on the student engagement factors, the results from Model 3 entirely replicate those from Model 1, attesting to the stability of these associations.

Figure 3. Result summary from the final predictive model (partial mediation).

Note. Only statistically significant paths are shown (p ≤ .05); positive paths are represented by full arrows whereas negative paths are represented by dotted-arrows; G: Global factor; INT: Intrinsic motivation; EIN: Extrinsic-identified regulation; EID: Extrinsic-introjected regulation. ER: External regulation. A: Amotivation. BE: Behavioral engagement: Effort/attention; BB: Behavioral engagement: Boredom/distraction; ES: Emotional engagement: Social; EL: Emotional engagement: Learning; CS: Cognitive engagement: Strategies; CA: Cognitive engagement: Autoregulation.

Figure 3. Result summary from the final predictive model (partial mediation).Note. Only statistically significant paths are shown (p ≤ .05); positive paths are represented by full arrows whereas negative paths are represented by dotted-arrows; G: Global factor; INT: Intrinsic motivation; EIN: Extrinsic-identified regulation; EID: Extrinsic-introjected regulation. ER: External regulation. A: Amotivation. BE: Behavioral engagement: Effort/attention; BB: Behavioral engagement: Boredom/distraction; ES: Emotional engagement: Social; EL: Emotional engagement: Learning; CS: Cognitive engagement: Strategies; CA: Cognitive engagement: Autoregulation.

In addition, these results reveal that both forms of emotional engagement (social and learning) predict higher global levels of student engagement (β = .114 and .210, respectively) as well as higher specific levels on the boredom/distraction facet of behavioral engagement (β = .226 and .172, respectively). Thus, students reporting higher levels of social-emotional engagement and tending to be more concerned about learning new material tended to report the highest global level of engagement but also the highest levels of boredom and distraction behaviors. In addition, higher levels of learning-emotional engagement were also associated with lower levels of cognitive engagement across dimensions (β = −.167 for the strategies facet and β = −.156 for the auto-regulation facet). Thus, students reporting greater levels of concerns about learning new things or learning difficulties tended to report lower levels of specific preventive and corrective cognitive engagement strategies facets. Again, these associations occur beyond the association with global levels of engagement. It is noteworthy that, by allowing emotional engagement facets to also influence the remaining student engagement factors, Model 3 was able to explain more variance in global levels of engagement (+3.2%), specific effort/attention (+4%), specific boredom/distraction (+5.8%) and specific autoregulation (+1.3%).

Altogether, these results suggest 12 indirect (mediated) relations, which were all supported by the estimation of confidence intervals for these indirect effects which excluded the value of 0. First, in addition to its direct positive effect on students’ global levels of engagement, students’ global levels of self-determination also present a positive indirect relation with students global levels of engagement and with their specific levels of boredom/distraction, as mediated by both specific facets of emotional engagement: (a) global self-determination → social-emotional engagement → global engagement: indirect effect = .048, 95% CI [.007, .333]; (b) global self-determination → social-emotional engagement → boredom/distraction: indirect effect = .057, 95% CI [.033, .166]; (c) global self-determination → learning-emotional engagement → global engagement: indirect effect = .056, 95% CI [.023, .093]; (d) global self-determination → learning-emotional engagement → boredom/distraction: indirect effect = .028, 95% CI [.012, .047]. Second, in addition to its direct negative effect on student specific levels of cognitive-autoregulation engagement, students’ global levels of self-determination also present a negative indirect relation with students’ specific levels of cognitive-strategies engagement (indirect effect = −.031, 95% CI [-.080, −.002]) and cognitive-autoregulation engagement (indirect effect = −.029, 95% CI [-.063, −.002]) as mediated by learning-emotional engagement.

Turning our attention to the specific facets of academic motivation, in addition to their various direct effects, the results also reveal a variety of indirect effects involving social-emotional engagement: (a) specific intrinsic motivation → social-emotional engagement → global engagement: indirect effect = .026, 95% CI [.005, .198]; (b) specific intrinsic motivation → social-emotional engagement → boredom/distraction: indirect effect = .031, 95% CI [.014, .099]; (c) specific identified regulation → social-emotional engagement → global engagement: indirect effect = .014, 95% CI [.001, .121]; (d) specific identified regulation → social-emotional engagement → boredom/distraction: indirect effect = .017, 95% CI [.004, .060]; (e) specific amotivation → social-emotional engagement → global engagement: indirect effect = −.035, 95% CI [-.280, −.006]; (f) specific amotivation → social-emotional engagement → boredom/distraction: indirect effect = −.042, 95% CI [-.133, −.023].

Discussion

Dimensionality

The present study relied on the bifactor-ESEM psychometric framework (Morin, Arens, & Marsh, Citation2016; Morin et al., Citation2020) to investigate the multidimensionality of, and associations between, students’ ratings of their academic motivation and school engagement. Our results supported the superiority of a bifactor-ESEM representation of academic motivation, thus highlighting the need to properly disaggregate students’ global levels of self-determined motivation (i.e., students’ global sense of self-directedness and volition) from the specific quality inherent of each type of behavioral regulation. These results add to the accumulating evidence supporting the value of a bifactor-ESEM representation of motivation measures anchored in the SDT framework across a variety of life domains, including education (Howard et al., Citation2018; Litalien et al., Citation2017). Interestingly, factor loadings observed on the global self-determination factor (i.e., students’ global sense of self-directedness and volition) identified as part of this model were found to mainly match the SDT’s hypothesized continuum structure of motivation, thus lending further support to this hypothesis (e.g., Howard et al., Citation2017; Ryan & Deci, Citation2017). Yet, it is important to keep in mind that support for this continuum structure remained imperfect in the present study, as the G-factor loadings obtained for the extrinsic-introjected regulation items were found to be slightly higher than those obtained for the extrinsic-identified regulation items. These findings nevertheless match those previously obtained across two distinct samples of students by Litalien et al. (Citation2017), who also reported lower G-factor loadings for identified regulation (λ = .376 to .493, Mλ = .455) than for introjected regulation (λ = .382 to .599, Mλ = .518). In addition, although most S-factors retained a meaningful level of specificity beyond the variance in item ratings already explained by the G-factor, the intrinsic motivation S-factor retained a more limited amount of specificity. This result, which makes sense from the perspective of SDT (Ryan & Deci, Citation2017), suggests that students’ report of intrinsic motivation mainly serve to define their global levels of self-determined academic motivation (i.e., global sense of self-directedness and volition) and retained only a limited amount of specificity once these global levels are taken into account.

In relation to school engagement, our results rather supported the value of a partial bifactor-CFA representation of school engagement, allowing for a proper disaggregation of students’ global levels of school engagement from specific cognitive and behavioral engagement facets. This result thus supports previous research supporting the value of a bifactor representation of school engagement (Dierendonck et al., Citation2020; Stefansson et al., Citation2016; Wang et al., Citation2016). However, in the present study, students’ levels of emotional engagement were found to tap into something distinct than the other facets and from the global construct captured as part of the G-factor identified in this bifactor solution. In other words, although the behavioral and cognitive facets of school engagement were found to present a dual global and specific nature (just like motivation ratings), facets of emotional engagement did not, and rather formed distinct components to consider when assessing students’ engagement dynamics. This result is aligned with previous reports suggesting that emotional engagement might contribute far less to the definition of global levels of school engagement (the G-factor) relative to the other facets of school engagement (Dierendonck et al., Citation2020; Wang et al., Citation2016), as well as with theoretical perspectives attributing a distinct role to emotional engagement relative to other forms of student engagement (Green et al., Citation2012; Skinner et al., Citation2008). In addition to providing replication evidence supporting the value of a bifactor representation of student engagement (Dierendonck et al., Citation2020; Stefansson et al., Citation2016; Wang et al., Citation2016), a key contribution of this study stems from this provision of empirical evidence supporting the theoretically-distinct nature of emotional forms of engagement relative to behavioral and cognitive engagement (Green et al., Citation2012; Skinner et al., Citation2008).

Furthermore, additional results reported in the online supplements (Appendix 2) supported the robustness of our findings by demonstrating the measurement invariance of responses to both instruments as a function of students’ gender, language, and age. Still, these additional results (revealing some differences related to items’ uniquenesses) also suggested that some additional fine-tuning might be needed to ensure the complete linguistic equivalence of the student engagement measure across German and French language groups when relying on manifest scale scores. Researchers could also rely on fully latent models when possible to control for this difference.

Associations between Academic Motivation and Engagement

In terms of associations between academic motivation and engagement, our results first supported our expectation that the most pronounced associations would occur at the level of the G-factors, but also that meaningful associations would happen at the level of the S-factors.4 Importantly, academic motivation facets were found to explain almost 66% of the variance in the G-factor of engagement, but lower proportions of variance (5% to 35%) in the specific engagement factors.

Turning our attention to more specific effects, global levels of self-determination were also associated with higher specific levels of emotional engagement. Also matching our expectations, once the effects of global levels of self-determination were taken into account, students’ specific levels of external regulation were found to share no association with their levels of engagement. Likewise, and also as expected, specific levels of amotivation appeared to share negative associations with students’ global levels of school engagement and social-emotional engagement and positive associations with boredom/distraction. However, these specific levels of amotivation also presented an unexpected positive association with students’ specific levels of learning-emotional engagement, although this association was relatively small. Similarly, students’ specific levels of external-introjected regulation were found to be associated with, as expected, lower levels of cognitive engagement and effort/attention, and higher levels of boredom/distraction. However, these specific levels of external-introjected regulations were also found to be associated with higher global levels of engagement and specific levels of learning-emotional engagement, suggesting that, contrary to our expectations, there seems to be both risks and benefits to the endorsement of introjected instrumental reasons for studying. This result is generally aligned with SDT (Ryan & Deci, Citation2017) representation of introjected regulation as located close to the middle of the motivation continuum and thus likely to exert both desirable and undesirable effects for students’ engagement. More precisely, this study suggests that, despite specific effects on increasing boredom and decreasing efforts and cognitive engagement, the endorsement of introjected instrumental reasons for learning can still help to increase students’ global and emotional levels of engagement.

The results also showed that specific levels of intrinsic motivation were associated with lower specific levels of boredom/distraction, as well as with higher specific levels of social emotional engagement. As such, when students study for intrinsic reasons, they are more likely to feel happy, safe and well-integrated in their class. They are also less likely to become bored and to do something else during class because they enjoy the study materials (e.g., Pekrun et al., Citation2014). These results are aligned with SDT (Ryan & Deci, Citation2017), which positions intrinsic motivation as emerging from the fulfillment of three basic psychological needs (autonomy, competence, and relatedness), themselves known to be associated with higher levels of engagement (Park et al., Citation2012). These results thus suggest that need fulfillment might exert its positive effect on engagement via the mediating role of motivations. Future studies will be needed to verify these propositions and this particular sequence. In contrast, intrinsically motivated students were less likely to utilize cognitive autoregulation strategies (e.g., bouncing back quickly from bad experiences), possibly because they were less likely to focus on the outcomes associated with their learning (e.g., grades) than on the enjoyment derived from it. This last result replicates prior studies relying on more traditional analytic approaches and highlights the importance of intrinsic motivation for learning (e.g., Cordova & Lepper, Citation1996; Ryan, Connell, & Plant, Citation1990; Ryan & Deci, Citation2013).

In contrast, specific levels of external-identified regulation were associated with higher levels of social emotional engagement and effort/attention, but with lower levels of global school engagement. The positive associations found between specific levels of identified regulation and higher specific levels of social emotional engagement and effort/attention suggest, in line with prior studies (e.g., Jeno et al., Citation2020), that students perceiving academics as a valuable and important activity might be more likely to exert additional effort, work harder, and become more emotionally invested in their studies. In contrast, and contrary to our expectations, our results revealed negative associations between students’ specific levels of identified regulation and their global levels of school engagement. This last result suggests that, once students’ global level of self-determination toward school is taken into account, simply perceiving schooling as a valuable activity without the accompanying self-determined drive to pursue this activity might hinder their global levels of engagement. For instance, this situation would apply to students who know that school activities are important, but fail to feel any volitional drive to pursue these activities. As a result, these students might become less engaged toward these otherwise important activities. Clearly, future studies are needed to verify the replicability of these findings, and to better understand the mechanisms at play in this association.

Mediation

Finally, this study assessed Skinner et al.’s (Citation2008) mediation hypothesis according to which the associations between academic motivation and global, behavioral and cognitive facets of school engagement would be mediated by emotional engagement. Adding to accumulating evidence in support of this hypothesis (Green et al., Citation2012; Skinner et al., Citation2008), our results also found support for a partial mediational role of emotional engagement. Interestingly, previously identified associations remained unchanged in this new mediational model, supporting their robustness. In addition, this model revealed that levels of emotional engagement were directly related to higher levels of global engagement, as suggested by the mediation hypothesis (Green et al., Citation2012; Skinner et al., Citation2008). However, this model surprisingly revealed that, once all direct associations between motivation facets and student engagement are accounted for in the model, emotional engagement levels were also associated with lower levels of cognitive engagement and higher levels of boredom/distraction. This result might also be related to the specific nature of these engagement components as estimated in the partial bifactor model. More precisely, these results suggest that, beyond their positive association with global levels of school engagement, high levels of emotional engagement could still be associated with slightly higher levels of boredom/distraction (possibly when course content does not match this emotional drive) and lower levels of cognitive engagement. This later effect seems aligned with the idea that too much emotionality might limit the ability to engage in school activities in a more cognitively-oriented manner. Once again, this result would need to be more extensively explored in future research. Clearly, additional studies will be needed to better document these associations, their possible replicability, and the mechanism underpinning them.

Implications for Future Research and Limitations

The present study has some limitations. Given the manifest differences observed in the operationalization of engagement measures (see for example Green et al., Citation2012), future research is needed to confirm the replicability of the present results, and their generalizability to alternative representations of academic motivation and student engagement. Importantly, the cross-sectional nature of the present study precludes reaching any conclusion related to the directionality of the observed associations, making it impossible to systematically test our expectation that motivation would predate engagement. Thus, future longitudinal research would be needed to verify that the observed associations really follow the currently hypothesized causal pathways. Longitudinal research would also make it possible to more precisely study continuity and change in longitudinal trajectories of motivation and engagement, and time-structured associations between these two constructs. Perhaps even more importantly, longitudinal research would make it possible to incorporate more contextual measures of the possible family, school, and peer-related determinants of students’ motivation and engagement trajectories, as well as some objective outcomes measures of these trajectories related to students’ achievement, persistence, and attainment.

The sample of schools and students used in the present study cannot be considered to be representative of the school and student population from these three countries. As a consequence, the generalizability of these results to these countries, and even more importantly beyond these countries, remains uncertain. Future research should thus seek to more systematically assess the generalizability of the present results to other countries, cultural groups, and languages. The convergent and divergent validity of the retained measurement models should be better established using other relevant constructs such as academic achievement, school dropout, and vocational choices using a combination of self-reported, informant-reported, and objective measures. Providing evidence of factor validity and linguistic equivalence for our measures was not a main objective of the present research. For this reason, future research should be designed to more closely investigate the potential use and interpretation of motivation and engagement tests scores for research and practice. These future studies should be guided by a more comprehensive validation framework (i.e., domain description, scoring, generalization, extrapolation, and implications), such as that proposed by Kane (Citation2006, Citation2013; for an application see Gotch & French, Citation2020).

Declarations of Interest

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (74.8 KB)

Additional information

Funding

The present study was supported by the European Union (Erasmus+, grant number 2016-1-LU01-KA201-013826). Erasmus + is the EU’s program to support education, training, youth and sport in Europe. The second author was supported by a Horizon Postdoctoral Fellowship by Concordia University. The third author was supported by a grant from the Social Science and Humanity Research Council of Canada (435-2018-0368) in the preparation of this article.

Notes

1 As an example, readers could imagine a group of students who actively participate in class (i.e., behavioral), react to the learning process with interest and enjoyment (i.e., emotional) and try different problem-solving strategies (i.e., cognitive). On a global level, these students might be considered to be globally engaged with their academic activities. However, they might also uniquely manifest additional signs of behavioral engagement (e.g., they might try to answer all questions asked by the teacher or perform additional assignments tasks) over and above their global engagement levels.

2 While higher-order and bifactor models both assume the presence of a global construct underlying all indicators, higher-order models rely on an extremely restrictive (and rarely verified) assumption that the ratio of variance explained by the first-order relative to the variance explained by the higher-order factor is constant for all items associated with a single first-order dimension (Gignac, Citation2016; Morin et al., Citation2020; Part et al., Citation2020; Perera et al., Citation2018). Bifactor models provide a more flexible alternative, not limited by this assumption, and are able to recover true higher-order factor structures (i.e., are mathematically equivalent) when this assumption is met by the data (Jennrich & Bentler, Citation2011). Moreover, whereas bifactor models allow one to obtain non-redundant G-factors and S-factors, higher-order factors and first-order factors share a conceptual redundancy due to the presence of variance explained by the higher-order factors within the first-order factors, creating confusion when both are simultaneously used in prediction (Morin, Boudrias, et al., Citation2016; Morin et al., Citation2017).

3 Here, as well as in the results section, we use the verb “predict” to depict regressions associations whereby scores on one latent variable are used to statistically predict scores on another variable, without assuming causality and directionality.

4 When considering the results pertaining to the specific factors, it is important to note that their interpretation differs from the typical interpretation of correlated factors representations. While factors taken from a correlated factors model reflect the covariance between the items forming a subscale, the specific factors from a bifactor model rather reflect the residual covariance between these items once the covariance between all items has been absorbed by the G-factor. Thus, rather than reflecting the desire to pursue an activity for the pleasure that it procures (intrinsic), or because it matches one’s personal value (external-identified), these S-factors might reflect more a form of quest for pure pleasure (specific intrinsic) or the impression of a match between one’s values and those conveyed by the activity (specific external-identified), without also capturing the drive component (i.e., the desire to get involved) of the factor taken from a correlated factors model.

References

  • Allen, D. (1999). Desire to finish college: An empirical link between motivation and persistence. Research in Higher Education, 40(4), 461–485. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018740226006
  • Ainley, M. D. (2012). Students’ interest and engagement in classroom activities. In S. L. Christenson, L. A. Reschly & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 283–302). Springer. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/101007/978-1-4614-2018-7_13
  • Anderman, E. M., & Patrick, H. (2012). Achievement goal theory, conceptualization of ability/intelligence, and classroom climate. In S. L. Christenson, L. A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 173–191). Springer.
  • Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. (2008). Student engagement with school: Critical conceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Psychology in the Schools, 45(5), 369–386. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20303
  • Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological engagement: Validation of the student engagement instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44(5), 427–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2006.04.002
  • Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Fallu, J.-S., & Pagani, L. S. (2009). Student engagement and its relationship with early high school dropout. Journal of Adolescence, 32(3), 651–667. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.06.007
  • Asparouhov, T. (2005). Sampling weights in latent variable modeling. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 12(3), 411–434. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1203_4
  • Asparouhov, T., Muthén, B., & Morin, A. J. S. (2015). Bayesian Structural equation modeling with cross-loadings and residual covariances. Journal of Management, 41(6), 1561–1577. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206315591075
  • Cheung, G. W., & Lau, R. S. (2008). Testing mediation and suppression effects of latent variables: Bootstrapping with structural equation models. Organizational Research Methods, 11(2), 296–325. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428107300343
  • Christenson, S. L., Reschly, L. A., & Wylie, C. (2012). Handbook of research on student engagement. Springer.
  • Cordova, D. I., & Lepper, M. R. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: Beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(4), 715–730. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.88.4.715
  • Cox, A. E., Ullrich-French, S., & Sabiston, C. (2013). Using motivation regulations in a person-centered approach to examine the link between social physique anxiety in physical education and physical activity-related outcomes in adolescents. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14(4), 461–467. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.01.005
  • Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. Plenum.
  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. Plenum.
  • Dierendonck, C., Milmeister, P., Kerger, S., & Poncelet, D. (2020). Examining the measure of student engagement in the classroom using the bifactor model: Increased validity when predicting misconduct at school. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 44(3), 279–286. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025419876360
  • Eccles, J. S. (2004). Schools, academic motivation, and stage-environment fit. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed., pp. 125–153). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470479193.adlpsy001013
  • Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. Guilford.
  • Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Educational Research, 59(2), 117–142. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543059002117
  • Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success among students at risk for school failure. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(2), 221–234. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.82.2.221
  • Finn, J. D., & Zimmer, K. (2012). Student engagement: What is it? Why does it matter? In S. L. Christenson, L. A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 97–131). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_5
  • Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74(1), 59–109. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543074001059
  • Froiland, J., & Worrell, F. (2016). Intrinsic motivation, learning goals, engagement, and achievement in a diverse high school. Psychology in the Schools, 53(3), 321–336. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.21901
  • Furlong, M. J., & Christenson, S. L. (2008). Engaging students at school and with learning: A relevant construct for all students. Psychology in the Schools, 45(5), 365–368. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20302
  • Gignac, G. E. (2016). The higher-order model imposes a proportionality constraint: That is why the bifactor model tends to fit better. Intelligence, 55, 57–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.01.006
  • Gillet, N., Morin, A. J. S., Jeoffrion, C., & Fouquereau, E. (2020). A person-centered perspective on the combined effects of global and specific levels of job engagement. Group & Organization Management, 45(4), 556–594. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059601119899182
  • Gotch, C. M., & French, B. F. (2020). A validation trajectory for the Washington assessment of risks and needs of students. Educational Assessment, 25(1), 65–82. https://doi.org/10.1080/10627197.2019.1702462
  • Green, J., Liem, G. A. D., Martin, A. J., Colmar, S., Marsh, H. W., & McInerney, D. (2012). Academic motivation, self-concept, engagement, and performance in high school: Key processes from a longitudinal perspective. Journal of Adolescence, 35(5), 1111–1122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.016
  • Grouzet, F. M. E., Otis, N., & Pelletier, L. G. (2006). Longitudinal cross-gender factorial invariance of the Academic Motivation Scale. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 13(1), 73–98. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1301_4
  • Guay, F., Morin, A. J. S., Litalien, D., Valois, P., & Vallerand, R. J. (2015). Application of exploratory structural equation modeling to evaluate the academic motivation scale. The Journal of Experimental Education, 83(1), 51–82. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2013.876231
  • Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered. Toward a developmental model. Human Development, 21(1), 34–64. https://doi.org/10.1159/000271574
  • Howard, J. L., Gagné, M., & Bureau, J. S. (2017). Testing a continuum structure of self-determined motivation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 1346–1377. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000125
  • Howard, J. L., Gagné, M., & Morin, A. J. S. (2020). Putting the pieces together: Reviewing the structural conceptualization of motivation within SDT. Motivation & Emotion, 44, 846–861. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-020-09838-2
  • Howard, J. L., Gagné, M., Morin, A. J. S., & Forest, J. (2018). Using bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling to test for a continuum structure of motivation. Journal of Management, 44(7), 2638–2664. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206316645653
  • Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
  • International Test Commission. (2017). The ITC guidelines for translating and adapting tests (2nd ed.). www.InTestCom.org
  • Jennrich, R. I., & Bentler, P. M. (2011). Exploratory bi-factor analysis. Psychometrika, 76(4), 537–549. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-011-9218-4
  • Jeno, L. M., Dettweiler, U., & Grytnes, J. A. (2020). The effects of a goal-framing and need-supportive app on undergraduates' intentions, effort, and achievement in mobile science learning. Computers & Education, 159, 104022. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2020.104022
  • Kane, M. T. (2006). Validation in educational measurement (4th ed., pp. 17–64). Praeger.
  • Kane, M. T. (2013). Validating the interpretations and uses of test scores. Journal of Educational Measurement, 50(1), 1–73. https://doi.org/10.1111/jedm.12000
  • Lam, S-f., Jimerson, S., Wong, B. P. H., Kikas, E., Shin, H., Veiga, F. H., Hatzichristou, C., Polychroni, F., Cefai, C., Negovan, V., Stanculescu, E., Yang, H., Liu, Y., Basnett, J., Duck, R., Farrell, P., Nelson, B., & Zollneritsch, J. (2014). Understanding and measuring student engagement in school: The results of an international study from 12 countries. School Psychology Quarterly, 29(2), 213–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000057
  • Lazowski, R. A., & Hulleman, C. S. (2016). Motivation interventions in education: A meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research, 86(2), 602–664. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654315617832
  • Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., & Benson, J. B. (2011). Positive youth development: Research and applications for promoting thriving in adolescence. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 41, 1–17.
  • Litalien, D., Morin, A. J. S., Gagné, M., Vallerand, R., Losier, G., & Ryan, R. (2017). Evidence of a continuum structure of academic self-determination: A two-study test using a bifactor-ESEM representation of academic motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51, 67–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.06.010
  • Liu, W., Wang, J., Tan, O., Koh, C., & Ee, J. (2009). A self-determination approach to understanding students’ motivation in project work. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(1), 139–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2008.07.002
  • Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu & Bentler (1999). Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 11(3), 320–341. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2
  • Mai, Y., Zhang, Z., & Wen, Z. (2018). Comparing exploratory structural equation modeling and existing approaches for multiple regression with latent variables. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 25(5), 737–749. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2018.1444993
  • Martin, A. J. (2007). Examining a multidimensional model of student motivation and engagement using a construct validation approach. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(Pt 2), 413–440. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709906X118036
  • Martin, A. J., Ginns, P., & Papworth, B. (2017). Motivation and engagement: Same or different? Learning and Individual Differences, 55, 150–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.013
  • McDonald, R. P. (1970). Theoretical foundations of principal factor analysis and alpha factor analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 23(1), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8317.1970.tb00432.x
  • Morin, A. J. S., Arens, A. K., & Marsh, H. W. (2016). A bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling framework for the identification of distinct sources of construct-relevant psychometric multidimensionality. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 23(1), 116–139. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2014.961800
  • Morin, A. J. S., Arens, A. K., Tran, A., & Caci, H. (2016). Exploring sources of construct-relevant multidimensionality in psychiatric measurement: A tutorial and illustration using the Composite Scale of Morningness. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 25(4), 277–288. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1485
  • Morin, A. J. S., Boudrias, J. S., Marsh, H. W., Madore, I., & Desrumaux, P. (2016). Further reflections on disentangling shape and level effects in person-centered analyses: An illustration aimed at exploring the dimensionality of psychological health. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 23(3), 438–454. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2015.1116077
  • Morin, A. J. S., Boudrias, J., Marsh, H. W., McInerney, D. M., Dagenais-Desmarais, V., Madore, I., & Litalien, D. (2017). Complementary variable- and person-centered approaches to the dimensionality of psychometric constructs: Application to psychological wellbeing at work. Journal of Business and Psychology, 32(4), 395–419. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-016-9448-7
  • Morin, A. J. S., Myers, N. D., & Lee, S. (2020). Modern factor analytic techniques: Bifactor models, exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) and bifactor-ESEM. In G. Tenenbaum & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (4th ed., pp. 1044–1073). Wiley.
  • Murray, A. L., & Johnson, W. (2013). The limitations of model fit in comparing the bi-factor versus higher-order models of human cognitive ability structure. Intelligence, 41(5), 407–422. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.004
  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. (2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.
  • Olivier, E., Morin, A. J. S., Langlois, J., Tardif-Grenier, K., & Archambault, I. (2020). Internalizing and externalizing behavior problems and student engagement in elementary and secondary school students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49(11), 2327–2346. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-020-01295-x
  • Park, S., Holloway, S. D., Arendtsz, A., Bempechat, J., & Li, J. (2012). What makes students engaged in learning? A time-use study of within-and between-individual predictors of emotional engagement in low-performing high schools. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(3), 390–401. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-011-9738-3
  • Part, R., Perera, H. N., Marchand, G. C., & Bernacki, M. L. (2020). Revisiting the dimensionality of subjective task value: Towards clarification of competing perspectives. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 62, 101875. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101875
  • Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002). Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learning and achievement: A program of qualitative and quantitative research. Educational Psychologist, 37(2), 91–105. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3702_4
  • Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., Goetz, T., & Perry, R. P. (2014). Boredom and academic achievement: Testing a model of reciprocal causation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(3), 696–710. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036006
  • Perera, H. N., Vosicka, L., Granziera, H., & McIlveen, P. (2018). Towards an integrative perspective on the structure of teacher work engagement. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 108, 28–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2018.05.006
  • Ratelle, C. F., Guay, F., Vallerand, R. J., Larose, S., & Senécal, C. (2007). Autonomous, controlled, and amotivated types of academic motivation: A person-oriented analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(4), 734–746. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.4.734
  • Reeve, J. (2012). A Self-determination Theory perspective on student engagement. In S. L. Christenson, L. A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 149–172). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_7
  • Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2012). Jingle, jangle, and conceptual haziness: Evolution and future directions of the engagement construct. In S. L. Christenson, L. A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 149–172). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_1
  • Roksa, J., & Whitley, S. E. (2017). Fostering academic success of first-year students: Exploring the roles of motivation, race, and faculty. Journal of College Student Development, 58(3), 333–348. https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.2017.0026
  • Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P., & Plant, R. W. (1990). Emotions in nondirected text learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 2(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/1041-6080(90)90014-8
  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2013). Toward a social psychology of assimilation: Self-determination theory in cognitive development and education. In B. W. Sokol, F. M. E. Grouzet, & U. Muller (Eds.), Self-regulation and autonomy: Social and developmental dimensions of human conduct (pp. 191–207). Cambridge University Press.
  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. Guilford.
  • Schunk, D. H., & Mullen, C. A. (2012). Self-efficacy as an engaged learner. In S. L. Christenson, L. A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 219–235). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_10
  • Skinner, E. A., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kindermann, T. (2008). Engagement and disaffection in the classroom: Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(4), 765–781. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012840
  • Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (2009). Engagement and disaffection as organizational constructs in the dynamics of motivational development. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 223–245). Routledge.
  • Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., & Furrer, C. J. (2009). A motivational perspective on engagement and disaffection: Conceptualization and assessment of children’s behavioral and emotional participation in academic activities in the classroom. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 69(3), 493–525. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164408323233
  • Stefansson, K. K., Gestsdottir, S., Geldhof, G. J., Skulason, S., & Lerner, R. M. (2016). A bifactor model of school engagement: Assessing general and specific aspects of behavioral, emotional and cognitive engagement among adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(5), 471–480. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415604056
  • Steinmayr, R., & Spinath, B. (2009). The importance of motivation as a predictor of school achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(1), 80–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2008.05.004
  • Tóth-Király, I., Morin, A. J. S., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2021). A longitudinal perspective on the associations between work engagement and workaholism. Work & Stress, 35(1), 27–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/02678373.2020.1801888
  • Vallerand, R. J., Blais, M. R., Briere, N. M., & Pelletier, L. G. (1989). Construction et validation de l’échelle de motivation en éducation (EME) [Development and validation of the Academic Motivation Scale (AMS)]. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences du Comportement, 21(3), 323–349. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0079855
  • Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., Blais, M. R., Brière, N. M., Senécal, C. B., & Vallières, E. F. (1992). The Academic Motivation Scale: A measure of intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in education. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52(4), 1003–1017. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164492052004025
  • Voelkl, K. E. (2012). School identification. In S. L. Christenson, L. A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 193–218). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_9
  • Wang, M. T., Fredricks, J. A., Ye, F., Hofkens, T. L., & Linn, J. S. (2016). The math and science engagement scales: Scale development, validation, and psychometric properties. Learning and Instruction, 43, 16–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.01.008
  • Wang, M. T., Willett, J. B., & Eccles, J. S. (2011). The assessment of school engagement: Examining dimensionality and measurement invariance by gender and race/ethnicity. Journal of School Psychology, 49(4), 465–480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2011.04.001
  • Wigfield, A., & Cambria, J. (2010). Students’ achievement values, goal orientations, and interest. Developmental Review, 30(1), 1–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.12.001
  • Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (2010). The impact of concept-oriented reading instruction on students’ reading motivation, reading engagement, and reading comprehension. In J. L. Meece & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Handbook of research on schools, schooling, and human development (pp. 463–477). Routledge.
  • Wu, Z. (2019). Academic motivation, engagement, and achievement among college students. College Student Journal, 53, 99–112.