2,560
Views
22
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Problems experienced by informal caregivers with older care recipients with and without cognitive impairment

, MSc, , MD, PhD, , PhD, , PhD, , PhD & , MD, PhD

ABSTRACT

The care of older persons can have negative impact on the caregiver. The objective of this population-based observational study is to identify problems experienced by informal caregivers, and the extent of related difficulties, in their care of older care-dependent recipients with and without cognitive impairment. Caregivers (n = 2,704) caring for a home-dwelling person aged ≥ 75 years responded to a questionnaire with 23 questions on problems and related difficulties by mail. Prevalence of self-reported problems and related difficulties was calculated. The impact of the problem was estimated by weighing the percentage of problems reported as being difficult against the prevalence of problems. The median number of problems was 12 (range 0–23), with 5 (range 0–23) reported as difficult. Informal caregivers experience a variety of problems, with the impossibility to engage in joint social activities having the highest impact. The impact of problems increased when the care recipient had a cognitive problem.

Introduction

As in all aging societies, in The Netherlands the number of older and care-dependent adults is growing. Because recent Dutch policy is directed at keeping people in need of care, at home as long as possible, older adults in need of care become increasingly dependent on informal caregivers. In most cases informal caregivers stem from family (Oudijk, De Boer, Woittiez, Timmermans, & De Klerk, Citation2010). For example, in the USA 13% of adults aged ≥ 40 years provide care to their parents and another 73% is likely to do this in the future (Taylor, Parker, Patten, & Motel, Citation2013).

Care giving can be a source of satisfaction but also a source of stress and emotional strain. When informal caregivers feel that the care is not sufficient, this may lead to feelings of burn out, depression, and guilt. (Almberg, Grafström, & Winblad, Citation1997, Collins & Jones, Citation1997; Donaldson, Tarrier, & Burns, Citation1998; Edwards & Scheetz, Citation2002; Ekwall & Hallberg, Citation2007; Gonyea, Paris, & De Saxe Zerden, Citation2008; Madsen & Birkelund, Citation2013; Roach, Laidlaw, Gillanders & Quinn, Citation2013; Springate & Tremont, 2014) In particular, cognitive problems of the care recipient are related to an increased complexity of the care and increased burden of the informal caregiver (Fowler, Fisher, & Pitts, Citation2014; Nordtug & Holen, Citation2011; Sequiera, Citation2013; Wenzel & Poynter, Citation2014). Apart from stress, problems in the social and relational domain occur, (Feinberg & Whitlatch, Citation2002), especially when the care recipient has cognitive problems (Harris, Citation2013; Luchetti et al., Citation2009). Although positive feelings, such as satisfaction and reciprocity play a role in care giving, care givers with a care recipient with cognitive problems experience these feelings to a lesser degree (Sequeira, Citation2013).

To provide appropriate support to informal caregivers, the associated problems need to be identified. Therefore, this study explores the prevalence and self-reported extent of difficulty of the problems faced by informal caregivers and evaluates to what extent these problems depend on the presence of cognitive impairment of the care recipient.

Methods

Study design

This population-based cross-sectional survey was conducted between March and May 2012.

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Leiden University Medical Center, The Netherlands.

Study population

The participants were informal caregivers registered at the Social Insurance Bank (SIB), an organization that implements national insurance schemes in The Netherlands. Eligible for the study were adults who provided informal care (in the last 12 months) to a home-dwelling adult aged ≥ 75 years who was also receiving professional care (e.g., home care, or assistance with practical activities in daily life; professional domestic assistance not included).

In The Netherlands, care-dependent people who are entitled to professional care can endorse their primary informal caregiver with an annual gift of €250 (via the government). Accordingly, 97,057 care-dependent eligible adults aged ≥ 75 years, living at home and entitled to professional care, provided the SIB with the names and addresses of their primary informal caregiver. As we aimed to have a balanced mix on gender, the SIB sent the questionnaire by mail to a random sample of (otherwise anonymous) 3,000 male and 3,000 female informal caregivers. We were unable to balance on relationship to care recipient as the SIB does not register this information.

The questionnaire

To develop a questionnaire addressing the daily problems of caregiving, we employed qualitative research methods based on grounded theory (Corbin & Strauss, Citation1990), conducting interviews with a sample of informal caregivers. Informal caregivers of care recipients with and without cognitive problems, participating in a research project on integrated systematic care for older people living at home (van Houwelingen, den Elzen, le Cessie, Blom, & Gussekloo, Citation2015), were invited to participate in an interview. Cognitive problems were measured by the Minimal Mental State Examination (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, Citation1975). We interviewed 15 males and 24 females; 21 were children, 10 were spouses, and 8 other. Thematic saturation was reached after 24 interviews for caregivers with care recipients without cognitive problems, and after 15 interviews in case of care recipients with cognitive problems. In these interviews participants were invited to speak freely about the following topics: (a) tensions and burdens in the process of caregiving, (b) changes in the relationship with the care recipient, (c) medical decision-making, and (d) cooperation with professional home care services. Items emerging from these interviews were transformed (by authors SvB and DPT using thematic topic analysis) into statements for use in the study questionnaire.

Subsequently, for the questionnaire, these items were thematically categorized into three domains: demand on the caregiver (6 items), safety of the care recipient (6 items), and social-relational issues (11 items).

To estimate prevalences, the respondents could express to what extent they recognized a particular problem on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = never; 4 = always) and to what extent they experienced this particular problem as being difficult (1 = not difficult at all, 4 = really difficult). To ensure that we would measure the degree of difficulty in this specific care situation, rather than merely hypothetical attitudes, for each item we included only those respondents who indicated that this problem did occur (sometimes, often, or always). For each item, the “self-reported difficulty” was dichotomized by recoding score 1 (not difficult at all) and score 2 (not really difficult) to “not difficult,” and recoding score 3 (a bit difficult) and score 4 (very difficult) to “difficult.”

In the questionnaire, several characteristics of the caregiver and care recipient were examined: including gender, age, type of relationship, and characteristics of the support, as well as care needs and assistance provided by professional home care. To assess the cognitive functioning of the care recipient, the first item of the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (Hughes, Berg, Danziger, Coben, & Martin, Citation1982), which aims to detect memory impairment, was used. This question provided us information about the perception of the care giver about the memory of the care recipient.

To synchronize the answer scale of this latter item with the answer scale of most other items, this answer was transformed from a “yes/no” answer to an answer on a 4-point Likert scale. Participants were also asked if the care recipient was suffering from forgetfulness: answers could be “most of the time,” “often,” “sometimes,” or “seldom.” We considered cognitive impairment to be present when respondents filled out “most of the time” or “often.”

Analyses

For each domain we performed the following psychometric analyses before performing principal component analyses. We calculated common variance and eigenvalue per domain. The constructs were tested toward sampling adequacy by the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) index, with values above .70 being considering adequate. Bartlett’s test of sphericity was used to verify whether the correlation matrix differed from an identity matrix. Results are shown in Appendix A. To test the reliability of our domain classification, the respondents who completed all items with respect to experienced difficulty were selected (n = 1,863; only respondents who experienced a certain problem, answered the question about difficulty). The dimensional structure of the items was tested with a principal component factor analysis (PCA) with Varimax rotation. Second, the internal consistency of the domain classification was tested with Cronbach’s alpha. Factor analysis showed that each domain consisted of one single factor. In addition, the internal consistency of the domains was confirmed by Cronbach’s alpha, which was high on all domains (demand: α = .90, safety: α = .71, social-relational: α = .80).

We calculated the prevalence of problems and the experienced difficulty, in total, per domain and per problem. To compare prevalence of problems and the experienced difficulty, in total and per domain between informal caregivers with a care recipient with cognitive impairment (according to the questionnaire) with informal caregivers with a care recipient without cognitive impairment, we calculated the odds ratios for increase in risk of having a higher prevalence or higher experienced difficulty than the median value. We chose medians because in some groups the mean number of problems was different from the mean—i.e., a slightly skewed distribution. Medians were compared with Mann-Whitney U test.

To make a comparison per problem, the calculated impact for each problem was compared in both groups by weighing the percentage of problems reported as being difficult with the prevalence of the problems. Comparisons were tested using chi-square tests.

When a participant indicated that an item was experienced but the perception of difficulty was missing, this record was omitted from the “difficulty-analysis” of this item. When the item was not filled in for prevalence, this record was omitted from the analysis of prevalence of problems and the “difficulty-analysis” of this item.

Participants were also asked if the care recipient had a formal diagnosis of dementia. As a sensitivity analysis, within the group caring for recipients with cognitive impairment, we compared those caring for individuals with a diagnosis of dementia to those caring for individuals without a diagnosis of dementia, regarding the number of experienced problems in total and per domain.

Results

Participants

The questionnaire was returned by 3,341 respondents (56%). However, as 627 of this latter group had missing values on the care recipient’s age or housing condition, or had exclusion criteria (i.e., the care recipient was aged ≤ 75 years and/or living in a care center), and 10 participants had not filled in any answer at the 23 problem-statements, 2,704 respondents were left for further analysis. Of these remaining caregivers: 54% were children of the care recipient and 23% spouses. For the remaining 23% the care recipient was: mother-in-law (n = 136), father-in-law (n = 28), brother (n = 20), sister (n = 38), uncle (n = 19), aunt (n = 65), another male (n = 92), another female (n = 211). For 10 respondents this was unknown. Their median age was 61 (interquartile range 54–73) years and 53% was female (). All 2,704 participants filled in the 23 items, with a small number of missing values per item. On average 95% (range 90–97%) of each item was filled in for prevalence, and out of the positive answers on prevalence, 97% (range 95–98%) of each item was filled in for difficulty.

Table 1. Characteristics of the Informal Caregivers and Care Recipients.

Prevalence of problems

Overall, the median number of self-recognized problems was 12 of 23 (range 0–23). Of these recognized problems, a median number of 5 (range 0–23) was reported as being difficult.

The median number of problems on (a) the demand domain was 4 (range 0–6) with 1 problem (range 0–6) reported as being difficult, (b) on the safety domain 2 (range 0–6) with 1 problem (range 0–6) reported as being difficult, and (c) on the social-relational domain 6 (range 0–11) with 3 problems (range 0–11) reported as being difficult.

Risk of problems in care recipients with cognitive impairment

shows the increase in the median number of problems in case of a care recipient with cognitive impairment. Irrespective of the domain, compared to informal caregivers with a care recipient without cognitive impairment, those caring for someone with cognitive impairment had a 5-times higher risk to report more problems than the median total number of problems, odds ratio (OR) 5.0, 95% confidence interval (CI) [4.1, 6.0]; and a 4-times higher risk, OR 3.9, 95% CI [3.2, 4.7], to perceive more problems as being difficult than the median total number of problems reported as being difficult ().

Figure 1. Median number of problems experienced on the three domains by caregivers with a care recipient without and with cognitive impairment. All p values < .001 (Mann-Whitney U test).

Figure 1. Median number of problems experienced on the three domains by caregivers with a care recipient without and with cognitive impairment. All p values < .001 (Mann-Whitney U test).

Figure 2. The median percentage of items per domain experienced as being difficult, by caregivers with a care recipient without and with cognitive impairment. All p values < .001 (Mann-Whitney U test).

Figure 2. The median percentage of items per domain experienced as being difficult, by caregivers with a care recipient without and with cognitive impairment. All p values < .001 (Mann-Whitney U test).

Within each domain, informal caregivers with a care recipient with cognitive impairment had a higher risk of having more problems than the median ()—“demand domain” OR 2.2, CI [1.8, 2.6]; “safety domain” OR 6.0, CI [5.0, 7.2]; “social-relational domain” OR 4.8, CI [4.0, 5.8]—and more than the median number of problems perceived as being difficult—“demand domain” OR 1.8, CI [1.6, 2.2]; “safety domain” OR 2.9, CI [2.4, 3.4]; “social-relational domain” OR 2.9, CI [2.4, 3.4]—compared to those caring for someone without cognitive impairment ().

In subgroup analyses for type of relationship we found that children experience more problems and perceive these as more difficult (see Appendix B).

Contribution of cognitive impairment to the impact of problems

shows the risk for experiencing a problem, and for experiencing a problem as being difficult, for informal caregivers with a care recipient with cognitive impairment compared to caregivers with a care recipient without cognitive impairment. In the group caring for recipients with cognitive impairment, all problems have a higher impact. For both groups, the impossibility to engage in social activities with the care recipient had the highest impact. In the presence of cognitive impairment, problems on the social-relational domain had the highest impact, whereas in the absence of cognitive impairment problems on the demand domain had the highest impact.

Table 2. Prevalence, Self-Reported Difficulty, and Impact of Problems of Caregivers, Depending on Cognitive Impairment of Care Recipients.

Risk of problems with care recipients formally diagnosed with dementia

The risk of problems for informal caregivers with a care recipient with cognitive impairment according to the questionnaire, plus a formal diagnosis of dementia, compared to caregivers with a care recipient with cognitive impairment but without a formal diagnosis, was similar (ORs for the domains with problems and for self-reported difficulty on the domains were around unity; data not shown).

Discussion

The present study reveals that informal caregivers of care-dependent persons aged ≥ 75 years often experience problems on all three examined domains: demand on the caregiver, safety of the care recipient, and social-relational issues. In this total population of informal caregivers, the impossibility to engage in social activities together had the highest impact. Safety issues were often reported as being difficult, but were not frequently mentioned. When the care recipient was cognitively impaired, informal caregivers had a considerably higher risk of indicating problems (on all three domains) as being difficult. Although we hypothesized that having a formal diagnosis of dementia would make problems easier to deal with, no difference was found in the prevalence or impact of the examined problems.

Participants indicated that most problems occur on the social-relational domain with the care recipient; this confirms a study on caregivers of veterans with dementia showing relational strain as a common phenomenon (Bass et al., Citation2012). Social-relational issues concern the preexisting relationship, shifts in responsibilities within the relationship, and social interaction with others.

Informal care is characterized by the preexisting relationship between the care-receiving older person and his/her informal caregiver: In the present study the majority of informal caregivers was related to the elderly person in some way. This relationship may strengthen the significance of giving care, but may also complicate how to deal with the experienced problems. Many informal caregivers struggle with the changing role and relationship between themselves and the care-receiving older person. Especially in case of a care recipient with cognitive impairment, the roles and relationships are likely to change. In this study, caregivers found it burdensome that the care recipient is often unaware of the burden that the provision of care entails. Although incongruence in the mutual appraisal of caregiving difficulties has been reported (Lyons, Zarit, Sayer & Whitlatch, Citation2002), interventions seldom focus on this item (Carretero, Garcés, Ródenas, & Sanjosé, Citation2009).

The problems faced by caregivers in the care for an older person often arise due to a shift in responsibilities (Walker, Citation2007). The frequency and difficulty of such discomfort increases in the case of diminishing decisional capacity of the older person—e.g., in case of cognitive impairment. Caregivers then have to find a balance between respect for the autonomy of the older person and performing their caring duties.

Furthermore, caregivers may face a negative impact on their own social life, as the older person may no longer want to go out and/or cannot be left at home alone. An interesting result of the present study is that the impossibility to engage in social activities together was perceived as the most important problem among our caregivers.

Psycho-educational programs might alleviate the emotional discomfort of informal caregivers associated with social-relational issues, and provide them with skills and strategies to face the challenge of caregiving (Lyons et al., Citation2002). Such programs may also acknowledge and support informal caregivers in the knowledge that the (former) relationship has changed, and that meaningful joint activities have become impossible; this might stimulate informal caregivers to find new ways to achieve joint activities. Providing insight into the dilemmas inherent to the caregiver/care-recipient relationship and the shift in responsibility might be helpful. However, compared with psycho-educational interventions, interventions offering respite care are reported to be the most effective in reducing caregivers’ burden (Garcés, Carretero, Ródenas, & Alemán, Citation2010). Perhaps interventions aimed at organizing social events for caregiver and recipient together (focusing on their relationship) might be successful, such as the special “memory cafes” available in The Netherlands and other countries (Hope & Pope, Citation2014; Ryan, Citation2014).

Strengths and limitations

We only had information of the respondent and not of the nonrespondents, which makes it difficult to make a judgement about the representativeness of the sample. Therefore we compared our sample to other population-based studies with informal care givers. Another population-based study with informal caregivers caring for frail older people in The Netherlands had a similar response to the questionnaire as in this study (Looman, Fabbricotti, & Huijsman, Citation2014). The age of the caregivers was comparable. However, our study has a relatively small percentage of female care givers (53 vs. 75%). A comparable sample in the United States showed 26% of care givers were partners which is comparable to our study. The percentage of children was 38 whereas our study had 54% (Allen, Lima, Goldscheider & Roy, Citation2012).

Secondly, although we did not use a validated instrument to assess cognitive functioning/impairment of the care recipients, family/close others are reported to adequately estimate cognitive impairment (Teresi & Holmes, Citation1997). Therefore, the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale can be seen as a valid classification tool for use in this study.

Conclusion

Informal caregivers experience a variety of problems in the process of caregiving, many of which have a considerable negative impact.

Cognitive impairment of the care recipient increases the risk of informal caregivers having problems on the domains of demand, safety, and social-relational issues. Addressing and enhancing awareness of these problems may enable informal caregivers to better anticipate these problems and, together with the care recipient, alleviate the complexity of the dilemmas.

Acknowledgments

The study was presented on a poster at the IAGG-meeting 2015 in Dublin, Ireland. The authors thank the national Social Insurance Bank and their representatives A. van den Berg and R. Blommaert, as well as all the respondents for their cooperation during this study. We also thank Jenny Heering for her contribution to the interviews that formed the basis of the study questionnaire. Informed consent has been obtained from those who are mentioned and who are not authors.

Funding

This study was funded by ZonMw, The Netherlands, Organization for Health Research and Development: ZonMw No. 60-61900-98-210. The sponsor had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; and preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Additional information

Funding

This study was funded by ZonMw, The Netherlands, Organization for Health Research and Development: ZonMw No. 60-61900-98-210. The sponsor had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; and preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Notes on contributors

Sytske Van Bruggen

Guarantor: D. P. Touwen had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Acquisition of data: D. P. Touwen, S. van Bruggen.

Analysis and interpretation of data: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, C. Bode

Drafting of the manuscript: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts, C. Bode

Obtained funding: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Jacobijn Gussekloo

Guarantor: D. P. Touwen had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Acquisition of data: D. P. Touwen, S. van Bruggen.

Analysis and interpretation of data: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, C. Bode

Drafting of the manuscript: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts, C. Bode

Obtained funding: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Christina Bode

Guarantor: D. P. Touwen had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Acquisition of data: D. P. Touwen, S. van Bruggen.

Analysis and interpretation of data: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, C. Bode

Drafting of the manuscript: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts, C. Bode

Obtained funding: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Dorothea P. Touwen

Guarantor: D. P. Touwen had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Acquisition of data: D. P. Touwen, S. van Bruggen.

Analysis and interpretation of data: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, C. Bode

Drafting of the manuscript: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts, C. Bode

Obtained funding: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Dick P. Engberts

Guarantor: D. P. Touwen had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Acquisition of data: D. P. Touwen, S. van Bruggen.

Analysis and interpretation of data: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, C. Bode

Drafting of the manuscript: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts, C. Bode

Obtained funding: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Jeanet W. Blom

Guarantor: D. P. Touwen had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

Acquisition of data: D. P. Touwen, S. van Bruggen.

Analysis and interpretation of data: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, C. Bode

Drafting of the manuscript: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: S. van Bruggen, J. W. Blom, D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts, C. Bode

Obtained funding: D. P. Touwen, J. Gussekloo, D. P. Engberts

References

  • Allen, S. M., Lima, J. C., Goldscheider, F. K., & Roy, J. (2012). Primary caregiver characteristics and transitions in community-based care. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67(3), 362–371. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbs032
  • Almberg, B., Grafström, M., & Winblad, B. (1997). Caring for a demented elderly person–Burden and burnout among caregiving relatives. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 25(1), 109–116. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2648.1997.1997025109.x
  • Bass, D. M., Judge, K. S., Snow, A. L., Wilson, N. L., Looman, W. J., McCarthy, C., … Kunik, M. E. (2012). Negative caregiving effects among caregivers of veterans with dementia. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20, 239–247. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e31824108ca
  • Carretero, S., Garcés, J., Ródenas, F., & Sanjosé, V. (2009). The informal caregiver’s burden of dependent people: Theory and empirical review. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 49(1), 74–79. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2008.05.004
  • Collins, C., & Jones, R. (1997). Emotional distress and morbidity in dementia carers: A matched comparison of husbands and wives. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12(12), 1168–1173. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-1166
  • Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (1990). Grounded theory research: Procedures, canons and evaluative criteria. Zeitschrift Für Soziologie, 19(6), 418–427.
  • Donaldson, C., Tarrier, N., & Burns, A. (1998). Determinants of carer stress in Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13(4), 248–256. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-1166
  • Edwards, N. E., & Scheetz, P. S. (2002). Predictors of burden for caregivers of patients with Parkinsonʼs disease. Journal of Neuroscience Nursing, 34(4), 184–190. doi:10.1097/01376517-200208000-00003
  • Ekwall, A. K., & Hallberg, I. R. (2007). The association between caregiving satisfaction, difficulties and coping among older family caregivers. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 16(5), 832–844. doi:10.1111/jcn.2007.16.issue-5
  • Feinberg, L. F., & Whitlatch, J. (2002). Decision-making for persons with cognitive impairment and their family caregivers. American Journal of Alzheimers Disease and Other Dementias, 17(4), 237–244. doi:10.1177/153331750201700406
  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
  • Fowler, C., Fisher, C. L., & Pitts, M. J. (2014). Older adults’ evaluations of middle-aged children’s attempts to initiate discussion of care needs. Health Communication, 29(7), 717–727. doi:10.1080/10410236.2013.786278
  • Garcés, J., Carretero, S., Ródenas, F., & Alemán, C. (2010). A review of programs to alleviate the burden of informal caregivers of dependent persons. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 50(3), 254–259. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2009.04.012
  • Gonyea, J. G., Paris, R., & De Saxe Zerden, L. (2008). Adult daughters and aging mothers: The role of guilt in the experience of caregiver burden. Aging and Mental Health, 12(5), 559–567. doi:10.1080/13607860802343027
  • Harris, P. B. (2013). Dementia and friendship: The quality and nature of the relationships that remain. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 76(2), 141–164. doi:10.2190/AG.76.2.c
  • Hope, K., & Pope, J. (2014). Tea and sympathy. Nursing Standard, 29(14), 26–27. doi:10.7748/ns.29.14.26.s27
  • Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., Danziger, W. L., Coben, L. A., & Martin, R. L. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 566–572. doi:10.1192/bjp.140.6.566
  • Looman, W. M., Fabbricotti, I. N., & Huijsman, R. (2014). The short-term effects of an integrated care model for the frail elderly on health, quality of life, health care use and satisfaction with care. International Journal of Integrated Care, 5(14), e034.
  • Luchetti, L., Uhunmwangho, E., Dordoni, G., Lorido, A., Barbieri, S., Bolognesi, A. G. … Franchi, F. (2009). The subjective feeling of burden in caregivers of elderly with dementia: How to intervene? Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 49(Suppl. 1), 153–161. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2009.09.025
  • Lyons, K. S., Zarit, S. H., Sayer, A. G., & Whitlatch, C. J. (2002). Caregiving as a dyadic process: Perspectives from caregiver and receiver. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57(3), 195–204. doi:10.1093/geronb/57.3.P195
  • Madsen, R., & Birkelund, R. (2013). ‘The path through the unknown’: The experience of being a relative of a dementia-suffering spouse or parent. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 22(21–22), 3024–3031. doi:10.1111/jocn.12131
  • Nordtug, B., & Holen, A. (2011). Similarities and differences in caring burden of home dwellers with partners suffering from chronic obstructive pulmonary disease or dementia. Home Health Care Management & Practice, 23(2), 93–101. doi:10.1177/1084822310368633
  • Oudijk, D., De Boer, A., Woittiez, I., Timmermans, J., & De Klerk, M. (2010). Mantelzorg uit de doeken [An exploration on informal care]. The Hague, The Netherlands: The Netherlands Institute for Social Research.
  • Roach, L., Laidlaw, K., Gillanders, D., & Quinn, K. (2013). Validation of the Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire (CGQ) in a sample of British dementia caregivers. International Psychogeriatrics, 25(12), 2001–2010. doi:10.1017/S1041610213001506
  • Ryan, B. (2014). I hope the memory café initiative takes off in hospitals across the UK. Nursing Standard, 29(15), 34. doi:10.7748/ns.29.5.34.s44
  • Sequeira, C. (2013). Difficulties, coping strategies, satisfaction and burden in informal Portuguese caregivers. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 22(3–4), 491–500. doi:10.1111/jocn.2013.22.issue-3pt4
  • Springate, B. A., & Tremont, G. (2014). Dimensions of caregiver burden in dementia: Impact of demographic, mood, and care recipient variables. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22, 294–300.
  • Taylor, P., Parker, K., Patten, E., & Motel, S. (2013). The sandwich generation. Rising financial burdens for middle-aged Americans. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center, Social and Demographic Trends.
  • Teresi, J. A., & Holmes, D. (1997). Reporting source bias in estimating prevalence of cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 50(2), 175–184. doi:10.1016/S0895-4356(96)00332-0
  • Van Houwelingen, A. H., Den Elzen, W. P., le Cessie, S., Blom, J. W., & Gussekloo, J. (2015). Consequences of interaction of functional, somatic, mental and social problems in community-dwelling older people. PLoS One, 10(4), e0121013. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0121013
  • Walker, M. U. (2007). Moral understandings. A feminist study in ethics (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Wenzel, K. A., & Poynter, D. (2014). ‘I’m mother! I can take care of myself!’ A contrapuntal analysis of older parents’ relational talk with their adult children. Southern Communication Journal, 79, 147–170. doi:10.1080/1041794X.2014.881540

Appendix A. Psychometric characteristics of the questionnaire.

Appendix B. Table subgroup analyses by type of relation

Note. Differences between care recipient with and without cognitive impairment were all significant. Type of relation: (a) Care recipient without cognitive impairment—prevalence in demand and social domain significantly different, with children experiencing more problems; experienced difficulty significantly different in all domains, with children experiencing more difficulty. (b) Care recipient with cognitive impairment—prevalence in social domain significantly different, with partners experiencing more problems; experienced difficulty significantly different in safety and social domains, with children experiencing most difficulty.

Appendix B. Table subgroup analyses by type of relationNote. Differences between care recipient with and without cognitive impairment were all significant. Type of relation: (a) Care recipient without cognitive impairment—prevalence in demand and social domain significantly different, with children experiencing more problems; experienced difficulty significantly different in all domains, with children experiencing more difficulty. (b) Care recipient with cognitive impairment—prevalence in social domain significantly different, with partners experiencing more problems; experienced difficulty significantly different in safety and social domains, with children experiencing most difficulty.