5,278
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Oral language at school entry: dimensionality of speaking and listening skills

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon

ABSTRACT

There has been a resurgence in concern about the levels of pupils’ oral language skills at school entry. To support and develop these skills effectively an understanding of the key components of oral language is required. We examined the oral language skills of monolingual children in Reception (MAge = 57.9 months; n = 126) and Year 1 (MAge = 69.07; n = 124) classes in England. Children were recruited from schools that were representative of London primary schools and were assessed on measures designed to tap phonology, core language skills (vocabulary and grammar) and discourse skills, both in the receptive and expressive modalities. Using confirmatory factor analyses, we examined the associations between oral language skills by component and modality for each age group. Oral language was best represented by four dimensions in Reception (receptive core language skills, receptive discourse, expressive core language skills, and expressive discourse). By contrast in Year 1, three dimensions were identified, irrespective of modality: phonology, core language skills and discourse. Our data speak to the importance of capturing these dimensions in assessments and teaching and monitoring their development at school entry. The results also highlighted the foundational role of discourse skills at the start of school.

Graphical Abstract

Oral language skills have been described as a condition for learning; conceptualised as a tool for gaining knowledge and for social engagement (Alexander, Citation2013; Wilkinson, Citation1968). Oral language underpins text decoding, reading comprehension (Nation et al., Citation2004) and written text production (Dockrell et al., Citation2019). Yet schools provide pupils with variable language learning opportunities (Pelatti et al., Citation2014) and professionals in education contexts are challenged in how to meet children’s language learning needs (Dockrell et al., Citation2017).

The importance of oral language in the school curriculum has varied over the years. The publication of the Bullock report in 1975 was a key step in raising public awareness about the spoken language experience of children in school (Bullock, Citation1975). Oral language is now included as a primary area in the English curriculum for the early years (Department for Education, Citation2021) and primary school years (Department for Education, Citation2014). However, the importance of focussing on oral language in schools has been debated and the implementation of oral language instruction in the curriculum has not always been consistent (Jones, Citation2017). Recently, the UK Oracy All-Party Parliamentary Group (Oracy All-Party Parliamentary Group, Citation2021) has supported calls to embed explicit teaching of oral language across the curriculum and has argued that there is a need to prioritise oral language in education (see also Kaldahl, Citation2019). A necessary prerequisite in meeting these objectives is to capture the key components of the language system.

Children who enter school with small vocabularies and limited grammatical skills (Levine et al., Citation2020) experience difficulties in accessing the curriculum and have lower levels of attainments. Poorer language skills are associated with family socio-economic background (Ginsborg, Citation2006; Hart & Risley, Citation2003; Hoff, Citation2013) and are evidenced in children who do not speak the language of instruction (Hoff, Citation2013) and those with developmental difficulties (Conti‐Ramsden et al., Citation2009). Although there is variability in development within these populations, different aspects of oral language are compromised.

There are three strands of research that have focused on the language skills of young children of school age: (1) studies which focus on developmental disorders (Conti-Ramsden et al., Citation2001; Snowling & Hulme, Citation2011) (2) interventions for children at risk (Dockrell et al., Citation2010; Fricke et al., Citation2013) and (3) attempts to capture the dimensionality of oral language through development (Bornstein et al., Citation2016). Studies that have examined children with developmental disorders speak to the importance of distinguishing between difficulties with phonology and other aspects of oral language (Bishop & Snowling, Citation2004), while intervention studies have emphasised the potential of enhancing poor language skills in young children (Hulme et al., Citation2020). However, intervention studies designed to ameliorate language delays and difficulties are not necessarily based on current conceptualisations of the oral language system and have variable efficacy (Dobinson & Dockrell, Citation2021; Hulme et al., Citation2020). Monitoring and supporting the oral language skills of children in classrooms requires a description of which language skills are important at which points in development. Studies which have explored the dimensionality of oral language have focussed on four foundational dimensions: phonology, vocabulary, grammar and discourse skills.

Oral language skills

Phonological skills – the perception of and ability to manipulate the sound units that are part of a given language – are key to mastering word decoding (Ehri et al., Citation2001). Phonological skills are a key component of the early years curriculum in England (Department for Education, Citation2021; Rose, Citation2006). In children from four to six years of age phonological skills have consistently been identified as a language dimension separate from vocabulary, grammar and discourse skills when these measures have been included in English-speaking (Anthony et al., Citation2014; Foorman et al., Citation2015) and also Greek populations (Mouzaki et al., Citation2020). However, the correlation between phonology and the other components of oral language varies from moderate (Mouzaki et al., Citation2020) to high (Foorman et al., Citation2015).

Growth in oral language during early childhood reflects a continuous development of lexical representations (vocabulary) and the development of an implicit understanding of the rules of grammar: skills which have been described as core language components (Karlsen et al., Citation2021). For both vocabulary and grammar, comprehension is generally more advanced than production, although the drivers of this difference remain underspecified (Dockrell & Marshall, Citation2015). Whether vocabulary and grammar represent different language systems with their own developmental rules and mechanisms of acquisition is a matter of debate (Bates & Goodman, Citation1999; Pinker, Citation1997). Recent research has suggested that vocabulary and grammar skills are strongly correlated in the early years of language development (Karlsen et al., Citation2021; Language and Reading Research Consortium; Language and Reading Research Consortium, Citation2015, Citation2017; Tomblin & Zhang, Citation2006), but are more differentiated in later primary school (at around 10 years of age) (Brinchmann et al., Citation2019; Tomblin & Zhang, Citation2006). Vocabulary and grammar contrast with discourse skills, which require the integration of multiple sentences into a coherent mental representation.

Connected discourse, such as conversations or storytelling, is underpinned by vocabulary and grammar, but also relies on cognitive skills related to comprehension monitoring and inference-making (Silva & Cain, Citation2015). Core language skills and discourse skills are highly correlated in the first few years of instruction but a clearer differentiation has been reported from 6.5 years of age (Massonnié et al., Citation2019; Language and Reading Research Consortium, Citation2015, Citation2017). However, recent work with Norwegian children has identified that receptive core language skills can be differentiated from expressive language skills in children as young as four years of age (Karlsen et al., Citation2021). Comparable factors have also been identified in Greek-speaking children where a full differentiation between phonology, semantics, morphology (a component of grammar) and discourse skills appeared as early as four years of age (Mouzaki et al., Citation2020). There is thus emerging evidence in languages other than English that discourse skills can be identified as a separate component of the language system earlier than previously reported. However, generalising from these studies to English is challenging on both methodological and conceptual grounds. Firstly, the Norwegian data only examined core language skills in the receptive domain, not expressive language, and did not include a measure of phonology (Karlsen et al., Citation2021). Secondly, the languages differ in terms of their grammatical and orthographic features potentially impacting on developmental pathways. Greek is a language with a transparent orthography and a rich morphological structure (Protopapas, Citation2017) whereas Norwegian has a different suffixation process which affects inflexional morphology (Ragnarsdóttir et al., Citation1999). Differences across languages and measures used limit the ability to generalise to children starting school in England.

Current models with English-speaking children have not incorporated phonology, vocabulary, grammar and discourse skills in a single model (Language and Reading Research Consortium, Citation2015, Citation2017; Lonigan & Milburn, Citation2017; Tomblin & Zhang, Citation2006). In addition, models with English-speaking children have not always consistently included both a measure of receptive and expressive skills, and no study has included a measure of expressive discourse skills. The distinction between comprehension (receptive) and production (expressive) is important and captured in the English National Literacy Strategy as ‘speaking’ and ‘listening’ (Department for Education and Skills, Citation2004) and in many standardised assessments of oral language (Dockrell et al., Citation2017), but the distinction between the two modalities has not been directly examined in models investigating the dimensionality of oral language. Previous models suggest that classifications based on language components are a better representation of the structure of oral language than classifications based on modality, but these models have not included measures of phonology or expressive discourse skills (Lonigan & Milburn, Citation2017; Tomblin & Zhang, Citation2006), nor have they consistently measured both expressive and receptive skills across components. Furthermore, studies with English-speaking children have included American children, who enter primary school a year later than in England. Schooling experience impacts on the structure of oral language by placing differential demands on the language components (Language and Reading Research Consortium, Citation2015). To summarise, there is a need to systematically explore the dimensionality of oral language skills across components and modalities in English-speaking children.

The current study

To further inform our understanding of language development in the early years of formal education and to capture the components and modalities which should be monitored at this point in development, the dimensionality of oral language was explored in children attending Reception and Year 1 classes in the United Kingdom. A comprehensive battery of reliable and valid measures assessing phonology, vocabulary, grammar and discourse skills in both the receptive and expressive modalities was used.

We conducted confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to explore dimensionality. Based on the findings discussed above, we examined whether the structure of language abilities is captured by a single oral language factor for children in Reception and Year 1 classes, or whether the assessed oral language skills are better classified by component (phonology, vocabulary, grammar, discourse) and/or modality (receptive and expressive). We predicted that classifications based on language components would better represent the structure of oral language than classifications based on modality (Lonigan & Milburn, Citation2017; Tomblin & Zhang, Citation2006). We further predicted that phonology, core language skills and discourse skills would represent three dimensions of oral language. Given that the differentiation between language skills becomes more salient over development, we anticipated lower correlations between these three constructs in Year 1 compared to Reception.

Methods

Participants

Children were recruited from nine schools in London. Participating schools were broadly representative of primary schools in London where, on average, 17.8% of children are eligible for Free School Meals (Msample = 14.12, SD = 9.04), 14.9% are eligible for Special Educational Needs support (Msample = 10.91, SD = 5.39) and 50.1% have English as an Additional Language (Msample = 36.22, SD = 17.65) (Department for Education, Citation2020). The Income Deprivation Affecting Children Index (IDACI) reflects the proportion of children aged 0 to 15 living in income deprived families. The average IDACI rank in London is 14,410, compared to 14,198 in our sample (SD = 9,112). Information about each individual school is provided in .

Parental consent was obtained for 250 monolingual English-speaking children: 126 children in Reception (37.3% boys; M = 57.9 months, SD = 3.71, range: 49–65) and 124 children in Year 1 (47.6% boys; M = 69.07 months; SD = 4.15, range: 61–76). Children gave verbal assent to take part in the activities. Ethical approval was obtained from UCL IOE Research Ethics Committee (REC 1207).

Questionnaires asking about education levels and household income were returned by 72% of the parents. The majority of respondents reported completing higher education (43.3% have a degree; 41.2% a postgraduate degree). Of the remaining respondents, 3.9% reported GCSEs as their highest level of qualification, 6.7% had A-levels and 3.3% a vocational qualification. Household income was above £45,200 for 85.9% of the returned questionnaires. The average disposable income for the financial year ending 2020 in the United Kingdom was £36, 900 (O’Neill, Citation2021).

Overall, while the schools in our sample were representative of primary schools in London, families who participated in the study had higher levels of education and income than the average.

Measures

The measures were selected according to three main criteria. They: (1) tapped into the oral language skills of interest (phonology, vocabulary, grammar and discourse skills); (2) were suitable for use with children in Reception and Year 1; and (3) had good reliability and validity as reported by previous studies. This resulted in a choice of four tests: (1) The British Picture Vocabulary test Third Edition (BPVS-3) (Dunn et al., Citation2009); (2) the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Second Edition (CELF-2) (Wiig et al., Citation2006; validated against the Preschool Early Literacy Indicator, Kaminski et al., Citation2014); (3) the Grammar and Phonology Screening test (GAPS) (Gardner et al., Citation2006; validated against the CELF and the Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition; Gathercole & Baddeley, Citation1996); and (4) the Test for Narrative Language Second Edition (TNL-2) (Gillam & Pearson, Citation2017; validated against the Narrative Coding Scheme, Heilmann et al., Citation2010).

While these measures could be considered global measures they can nevertheless be partitioned into components based on the aspect of language they primarily tap. The specific tasks used for the present study are detailed below. Internal consistency calculated with the Cronbach alpha on the sample is reported in . For all the CELF-2 subtests, administration instructions and discontinue rules were applied as per the manual (Wiig et al., Citation2006).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for all measures (raw scores).

Phonology

GAPS, Non-words Repetition

Children were introduced to an alien and asked to repeat what the experimenter said to the alien. Eight non-words were presented, varying in complexity based on marked onset, rhyme and final consonant. An item was counted as correct if children repeated the entire non-word correctly. The sum score for all items was calculated (maximum: 8).

Vocabulary

Receptive Vocabulary

BPVS-3

Children pointed to the picture among four which represented the word spoken by the experimenter. The testing procedure was followed and the total score was calculated as per the manual’s instructions (Dunn et al., Citation2009).

The study originally included two additional measures of receptive vocabulary, the CELF-2 Word Classes subtest (Receptive section), for children aged 5–6, and the CELF-2 Basic Concepts, for children aged 3–4 (Wiig et al., Citation2006). Given the poor internal consistency of the Basic Concepts measure on our sample (α = .27) and to provide equivalence of reliable measures across both year groups, the CELF-2 Basic Concepts and the CELF-2 Word Classes subtests were not considered for subsequent analyses.Footnote1

Expressive Vocabulary

CELF-2 Expressive Vocabulary

Children were presented with a picture and asked to respond to a prompt from the examiner (e.g. ‘What is this?’; ‘What is he doing?’) to generate a noun or a verb. There were 20 trials, scored as per the test manual. The sum score from all the performed items was calculated (maximum: 40).

Grammar

Receptive Grammar

CELF-2 Sentence Structure

Children pointed to the picture corresponding to a prompt sentence, among four alternatives. This task assesses understanding of grammatical conventions (e.g. prepositional phrases, copulas, infinitive, negation, etc.). One point was awarded per correct answer. The sum score from all the performed items was calculated (maximum: 22).

CELF-2 Concepts and Following Directions

This task measured children’s ability to: (a) interpret directions of increasing length and complexity; (b) remember the names, characteristics and order of mention of pictures; (c) identify target objects from several choices. Children saw a series of pictures depicting animals in different settings and were asked to point to the animals in the order specified by the experimenter. The instructions increased in complexity over time. One point was given per correct answer. The sum score from all the performed items was calculated (maximum: 22).

Expressive Grammar

CELF-2 Word Structure

This task assessed the production of prepositions, plurals, possessive nouns, tense marking, copula, pronouns and derivational forms. Children finished a sentence started by the experimenter. Each item was scored 1 if the child used the target grammatical structure, and 0 otherwise. The sum score from all the performed items was calculated (maximum: 24).

GAPS Sentence Repetition

Children repeated a story, one sentence at a time. There were 11 sentences accompanied by a picture, capturing different grammatical constructs. One point was given for each sentence that was correctly repeated.

Discourse skills

TNL-2, Receptive and Expressive Discourse Skills

In the TNL-2 the experimenter alternated between asking children comprehension questions about three oral stories (Receptive section), and prompting children to retell or create their own stories (Expressive section). The three receptive tasks are referred to as Story Comprehension 1, Story Comprehension 2 and Story Comprehension 3, containing 14, 12 and 12 comprehension questions respectively. Most of the questions were scored either 0 (incorrect) or 1 (correct), but some were worth 2 or 3 points because they included two or three pieces of information. The maximum score was 20, 14, 13 for Story Comprehension 1, Story Comprehension 2 and Story Comprehension 3 respectively. Stories were supported with a picture and told by the experimenter. The three expressive tasks are referred to as Story Generation 1, Story Generation 2 and Story Generation 3 for consistency. Although, of note, the first story generation task was in the form of an oral re-tell (children retelling everything they remembered from Story 1) and the remaining generation tasks were produced by the child from a picture prompt.

Story Generations 1, 2 and 3 were audio-recorded using Audacity software and transcribed verbatim including any phonological and grammatical mistakes, repetitions, hesitations and pauses. The first and second authors used both the audio and the transcribed version of the narratives to score them according to the TNL-2 manual (Gillam & Pearson, Citation2017; maximum score of 31, 27 and 30 for the three generation tasks respectively). Utterances that were not related to the narrative prompt were not included. Interrater reliability was calculated with Cronbach’s alpha for 20% of the narratives. Agreement was high (.90, .84 and .89, respectively).

Procedure

Children were tested individually in a quiet room at their school. Six experienced researchers, trained in the administration of the tests, conducted the assessments. The test battery was carried out over two separate sessions, to prevent fatigue effects (Session 1: GAPS, CELF-2; Session 2: BPVS-3, TNL-2). Breaks were factored in as required.

Results

Data analysis

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the last author, upon reasonable request. Raw scores are provided in and will be used in the following analyses.

There was a ceiling effect on the Non-Word Repetition and Sentence Repetition tasks. A proportion of children were not able to produce an oral narrative and scored 0 on the expressive discourse tasks (this proportion varies between 6.5% and 15% across tasks and year groups). One child exceptionally had the maximum score at the Story Generation 1 task. Five children in Year 1 performed 3SD below the mean at the receptive vocabulary task. Across all the other tasks, a maximum of three children per year group performed particularly low compared to the rest of the sample (< 3SD below the mean). Outliers and children scoring 0 on the expressive discourse tasks were kept in the analyses because they are considered as part of the natural variation in abilities among monolingual children.

In total, 1.47% data points were missing in the Reception cohort, and 0.68% data points were missing in the Year 1 cohort. This was due to children being absent for a session, declining to participate in an activity, or administration error. Little’s (Citation1988) MCAR test was non-significant (Reception: χ2(37) = 48, p = .106; Year 1: χ2(15) = 17.35, p = .298), indicating that the data were missing completely at random.

Correlations between all the raw scores are provided in .

Table 2. Correlations between all the variables.

Confirmatory Factor Analyses (CFA) were carried out on raw scores using MPlus 6.12. The maximum likelihood estimator was selected to deal with missing data (Enders & Bandalos, Citation2001), and the robust estimator was used because it does not assume normal multivariate distributions. CFA are theory-driven and allow to compare the six alternative models of the dimensionality of oral language highlighted in the introduction and summarised in (see Schreiber et al., Citation2006 for guidance on CFA diagrams).

Figure 1. Alternative CFA models representing the dimensionality of oral language based on language components and/or modalities. (R): Receptive; (E): Expressive.

Figure 1. Alternative CFA models representing the dimensionality of oral language based on language components and/or modalities. (R): Receptive; (E): Expressive.

Figure 1. Continued.

Figure 1. Continued.

Figure 1. Continued.

Figure 1. Continued.

Model fit was assessed with multiple indicators (Byrne, Citation2013; Wang & Wang, Citation2012). A non-significant χ2, a Comparative Fit Index (CFI) >.95, a Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) >.95, a Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) <.05 and Standardised Root Mean Square Residuals (SRMR) <.08 indicate good fit. A CFI >.90, a TLI >.90, RMSEA <.08 and SRMR <.08 indicate adequate fit. A significant χ2 is not a reason by itself to reject a model given its high sensitivity to sample size (Wang & Wang, Citation2012) and should be interpreted in light of the other indices. As lower χ2 values indicate better fit, the Satorra-Bentler Chi-Square difference test was used to compare nested models (MPlus, Citation2020). Non-nested models were compared using the AIC and BIC indicators, lower values indicating a better fit.

Confirmatory factor analyses

All models initially had poor fit. Modification indices suggested by MPlus 6.12 were checked to identify any potential problem with the models’ specification. As a result the residual covariance between: (1) The GAPS Sentence Repetition and Non-word subtests; (2) The TNL-2 Story Comprehension 1 and Story Generation 1, which correspond to children answering questions about a story, and then retelling it; (3) The TNL-2 Expressive TasksFootnote2 was taken into account (Byrne, Citation2013). reports model fit indices for the six models and reports model comparisons.

Table 3. Model fit statistics for the six alternative CFA models in Reception and Year 1.

Table 4. Model comparisons (Satorra-Bentler Chi-Square Difference Test).

Reception

The unidimensional model (Model A) had an adequate fit. Models B, C, D, E and F all had a good fit to the data, and were better than Model A. Among the two nested models (B and C) separating language skills solely based on language components, Model C, which separated Phonology, Vocabulary, Grammar and Discourse, revealed a very high correlation between the Vocabulary and Grammar factors (r = .95), and was not better than Model B, which only separated Phonology, Core language skills and Discourse. Among the nested models separating skills based on language modality, Model E, which discriminated Receptive Core language skills, Receptive Discourse, Expressive Core language skills, Expressive Discourse skills was better than Model D, which only discriminated Receptive and Expressive skills (two factors that were highly correlated, r = .90). Model F, which further separated out Phonology did not have a better fit than Model E. AIC and BIC indicators, which allow comparisons across all models (nested and non-nested), indicated that Model E (in ) had the best fit to the data.

Figure 2. Best fitting model in Reception: all paths significant at the <.001 level.

Figure 2. Best fitting model in Reception: all paths significant at the <.001 level.

Year 1

The unidimensional model (Model A) had an adequate fit. Model D, which revealed a very high correlation between Receptive and Expressive skills (r = .98), had an adequate fit but was not better than Model A. Models E and F, distinguishing language skills based on both language components and modalities, did not converge. Models B and C, based on language components, had a good fit, and were both better than Model A. Model C, which separated Phonology, Vocabulary, Grammar and Discourse, revealed a very high correlation between the Vocabulary and Grammar factors (r = .90), and was not better than Model B, which only separated Phonology, Core language skills and Discourse. Model B should therefore be preferred for the sake of parsimony (see ). Across all models, Model B had the lowest AIC and BIC values.

Figure 3. Best fitting model in Year 1: all paths significant at the <.001 level.

Figure 3. Best fitting model in Year 1: all paths significant at the <.001 level.

Discussion

An understanding of the key components of oral language is necessary to support children who struggle with oral language and to provide effective language learning environments that ultimately facilitate literacy and learning (Oracy All-Party Parliamentary Group, Citation2021). This study examined the dimensions of oral language in a cohort of typically-developing monolingual children in English schools at the beginning of formal education. Measures captured phonology, vocabulary, grammar and discourse skills. Both receptive and expressive skills were assessed to provide a comprehensive profile of children’s language skills at this point in development.

We predicted that modality would not be a key factor in the representation of the structure of language. However, we anticipated that phonology, core language skills and discourse skills would represent three dimensions of oral language that would become more differentiated across age groups. Our data indicated that the relationships between language skills were best represented by a model distinguishing receptive core language, receptive discourse, expressive core language and expressive discourse in Reception. Language skills were therefore classified based on both components and modalities. To our knowledge this is the first time that it has been demonstrated that both oral language components and modality are central dimensions to capture at the start of formal education. By the age of six years (children in Year 1), the best model distinguished phonology, core language and discourse, but modality was no longer a significant factor.

In both cohorts, vocabulary and grammar were strongly correlated and grouped under the construct of core language skills, a finding which is consistent with previous research in English and Norwegian samples (Bornstein et al., Citation2016; Karlsen et al., Citation2021). Core language skills were distinguished from, yet highly correlated with, discourse skills. This is consistent with the results obtained from American samples of a similar age range (Language and Reading Research Consortium, Citation2015, Citation2017; Tomblin & Zhang, Citation2006). Whereas most correlations between core language skills and discourse skills were very high (> .80), in Reception, expressive discourse skills were unique in terms of their lower correlations with the other dimensions assessed.

This is a new finding and highlights the unique features and challenges of expressive discourse upon school entry. Children are expected to use expressive language for a wide range of functions in the primary classroom (Shiel et al., Citation2012), where verbal interaction is a key tool for learning across the curriculum (Alexander, Citation2013). Creating a narrative is a more decontextualised use of language than conversation (Curenton & Justice, Citation2004; Snow, Citation1991). It requires the child to use language precisely to create and continually update a story line, while accessing specific knowledge about phonology, vocabulary and grammar. Between 4 and 5 years of age, expressive discourse skills improve (Lindgren, Citation2019; Schneider et al., Citation2006), but their improvement will be impacted by effective language learning opportunities. It has been argued that more emphasis should be placed on professional training focused on supporting classroom language interactions that facilitates such extended discourse (Jacoby & Lesaux, Citation2014). Teachers also use spoken language to assess learning and their perceptions of expressive language ability have been significantly correlated with their perceptions of a child’s overall development (Vega et al., Citation2018). There is also evidence that teachers adjust the complexity of their language in response to the language used by their pupils (Justice et al., Citation2013), whereby children’s expressive language may predict the quality of language provision they receive. A focus on the development of expressive language skills is, thus, consistent with evidence from educational settings and our model of language skills at this point in development.

Implications for monitoring and supporting language development

Expressive discourse skills are fundamental for the development of literacy skills (Kim & Schatschneider, Citation2017), and evidence-informed resources which support teachers and schools to develop these discourse skills upon school entry are an important step for the development of children’s literacy skills. Oral narratives can be elicited by encouraging children to share life events and personal stories. Asking open-ended ‘how’ and ‘why’ questions when reading a book or commenting on everyday life situations promotes both the use of extended discourse and consciousness of how supplementary information enriches communication (Petersen & Spencer, Citation2016; Pinto et al., Citation2015). Specific pedagogical approaches have been developed to support the development of story elements in children’s narratives (e.g. character, setting, initiating event, response, consequence), to help them tell and retell stories (Gillam et al., Citation2014; Spencer & Petersen, Citation2018) and specific assessments have been designed to help teachers to evaluate the quality of children’s narrative production (Justice et al., Citation2010; Petersen & Spencer, Citation2012).

Limitations of the current study

In our models, phonology was included in the same factor as expressive vocabulary and grammar in Reception, but was a separate oral language factor in Year 1. These results differ from data from Greek-speaking children where phonology represented a separate dimension in children as young as four years old (Mouzaki et al., Citation2020). The morphological complexity of the Greek language may make phonology more salient at an earlier age. These differences highlight both the importance of capturing similar dimensions across different languages and testing language models across different languages. Alternatively (or in addition), the choice of tasks might have impacted the results. In our study, phonological processing was measured using a non-word repetition task which required children to accurately perceive and repeat phonemes. Receptive and expressive skills were both combined in the same task, instead of being measured in two separate tasks, as we did for the other language components. The task was performed well by the majority of children, reflecting the emphasis on phonics in the current English national curriculum. In Mouzaki et al. (Citation2020)’s study, three tasks were used to measure phonological awareness, which is the ability to isolate and manipulate phonemes within words. The relationship between phonological awareness and oral language skills – specifically vocabulary development – has been demonstrated in children in early primary school (Cooper et al., Citation2002; Hipfner-Boucher et al., Citation2014). The present findings could be further extended with a more comprehensive assessment of phonology which includes both phonological awareness and processing measures.

Furthermore, our sample included monolingual children with no reported developmental difficulties attending schools which reflected average demographic factors. As such our data may not generalise to children who are disadvantaged or experiencing developmental challenges. Nonetheless the data provide an indicative benchmark to guide teaching and assessment. The current data set is also cross-sectional. To establish benchmarks for development longitudinal data are required to guide teaching and inform the identification of children who are struggling with oral language (Schmitt et al., Citation2017). Future studies should include diverse samples and track development over time.

Conclusions

To our knowledge this is the first study to capture the components of the language system as children enter formal education in England. The current data have implications for theory and practice. From a theoretical view the results point to the importance of considering both expressive language and receptive language when modelling language components. This distinction has not been clearly articulated in the data capturing language dimensions to date, despite the fact that it often underpins the identification of children with language learning difficulties (Bishop & Donlan, Citation2005). An evidence-informed model of oral language skills supports the development of a comprehensive oral language curriculum and can provide teachers with the information they require to develop teaching and learning activities. As we have demonstrated, oral language skills are multidimensional and a curriculum which emphasises quality language instruction across these components should support accelerated language growth (Justice et al., Citation2008).

The present study provides new data which extends our understanding of the differentiation between core language skills (vocabulary, grammar) and discourse skills for children as they enter formal education in England. The inclusion of both receptive and expressive measures captured the specificity of expressive discourse skills in Reception. These data point to the types of language skills which should be embedded and monitored in classrooms to enhance the development of oral language skills which underpin reading and writing.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the teachers and headteachers who supported this study, along with the children and their families. We are grateful to Jana Parolkova, Elizabeth Booth, Mafalda Duque and James Cordiner for their help with data collection and data entry. We would like to thank our advisory board for their insightful feedback on the study design and on an earlier version of this manuscript. Finally, we are grateful to The Heather van Der Lely Foundation for supporting this work.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by The Heather van Der Lely Foundation Trust.

Notes on contributors

Jessica Massonnié

Dr. Jessica Massonnié is a Research Fellow at University College London Institute of Education. Her work focuses on understanding how we can best help children to thrive at school, by considering their learning skills and the complex educational factors influencing their success and wellbeing. She is currently involved in the Key to Oral Language research project reported in this paper, in the GCRF UKRI Action Against Stunting Hub, and is completing research on noise in school settings. She was the EARLI SIG 22 Neuroscience & Education JURE Coordinator for the period 2019-2021.

Anna Llaurado

Dr. Anna Llaurado is a Research Fellow at University College London Institute of Education. Her research interests are in the development of writing skills during the school years with a particular focus on how this process is shaped by differences across languages. Recently, her participation in the project reported here has branched her work into the development of oral skills in young children with a particular interest on how this supports literacy acquisition in the early years of schooling. She is associate editor of Cultura y Educación.

Emma Sumner

Dr. Emma Sumner (CPsychol) is a Lecturer in Psychology and Special Educational Needs at University College London, Institute of Education. Her research interests cover language and literacy development, with a particular focus on understanding why children present with difficulties in oral language, reading and writing. She is currently involved in a number of projects, one of which is reported here and others which involve developing digital technologies to help support beginner and struggling readers (funded by Innovation Action EU Horizon 2020) and also examining how best to support students with handwriting difficulties (UK Occupational Therapy Research Foundation). She is associate editor for Research in Developmental Disabilities.

Julie E. Dockrell

Prof. Julie E. Dockrell (FRCSLT, FAcSS) is Professor of Psychology and Special Needs at University College London, Institute of Education. Her research interests are in patterns of language development and the ways in which oral language skills impact on children’s learning, interaction and attainments. A central theme in this research has been the application of evidence-based research to support children’s learning. She has led projects on language development funded by the DfE, ESRC, Nuffield Foundation and Leverhulme Trust. She has published in a wide range of journals and written books and book chapters on language development and language difficulties. She was the previous editor of the British Journal of Educational Psychology, associate editor for JSLHR and Learning and Instruction. She was a co-director of the Better Communication Research Programme, UK.

Notes

1. Leaving these subtests in the analyses did not change the conclusions.

2. This was done for all models except Models E and F which group all three variables in a common factor.

References

  • Alexander, R. J. (2013). Improving oracy and classroom talk: Achievements and challenges. Primary First 10, 22–29. https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/76321/
  • Anthony, J. L., Davis, C., Williams, J. M., & Anthony, T. I. (2014). Preschoolers‘ oral language abilities: A multilevel examination of dimensionality. Learning and Individual Differences, 35, 56–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.07.004
  • Bates, E., & Goodman, J. (1999). On the emergence of grammar from the Lexicon. In B. MacWhinney (Ed)., The emergence of language (pp. 52-72). Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Donlan, C. (2005). The role of syntax in encoding and recall of pictorial narratives: Evidence from specific language impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 25–46. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151004x20685
  • Bishop, D. V., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130(6), 858–886. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.858
  • Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Putnick, D. L. (2016). Stability of core language skill across the first decade of life in children at biological and social risk. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(12), 1434–1443. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12632
  • Brinchmann, E. I., Braeken, J., & Lyster, S. A. H. (2019). Is there a direct relation between the development of vocabulary and grammar? Developmental Science, 22(1), e12709. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12709
  • Bullock, A. (1975). The Bullock Report. A language for life. Her Majesty’s Stationery Office.
  • Byrne, B. M. (2013). Structural equation modeling with Mplus: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Routledge.
  • Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42(6), 741–748. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00770
  • Conti‐Ramsden, G., Durkin, K., Simkin, Z., & Knox, E. (2009). Specific language impairment and school outcomes. I: Identifying and explaining variability at the end of compulsory education. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44(1), 15–35. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820801921601
  • Cooper, D. H., Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Schatschneider, C. (2002). The contribution of oral language skills to the development of phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(3), 399–416. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716402003053
  • Curenton, S. M., & Justice, L. M. (2004). African American and Caucasian preschoolers’ use of decontextualized language. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35(3), 240–253. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2004/023)
  • Department for Education and Skills. (2004). Speaking, listening, learning: Working with children in key stages 1 and 2. (Vols 0163–2004). Crown.
  • Department for Education. (2014). National curriculum in England: English programmes of study. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/national-curriculum-in-england-english-programmes-of-study/national-curriculum-in-england-english-programmes-of-study
  • Department for Education. (2020). Statistics: School and pupil numbers. https://www.gov.uk/government/collections/statistics-school-and-pupil-numbers
  • Department for Education. (2021, March 31). Statutory framework for the early years foundation stage. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/early-years-foundation-stage-framework–2
  • Dobinson, K. L., & Dockrell, J. E. (2021). Universal strategies for the improvement of expressive language skills in the primary classroom: A systematic review. First Language, 41(5), 527–554. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723721989471
  • Dockrell, J. E., Connelly, V., & Arfè, B. (2019). Struggling writers in elementary school: Capturing drivers of performance. Learning and Instruction, 60, 75–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.11.009
  • Dockrell, J. E., Howell, P., Leung, D., & Furgad, X. (2017). Children with speech language and communication needs in England: Challenges for practice. Front: Educ, 2, 35. https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2017.00035
  • Dockrell, J. E., & Marshall, C. R. (2015). Measurement issues: Assessing language skills in young children. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 20(2), 116–125. https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12072
  • Dockrell, J. E., Stuart, M., & King, D. (2010). Supporting early oral language skills for English language learners in inner city preschool provision. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 497–515. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709910x493080
  • Dunn, L. M., Dunn, D. M., Sewell, J., Styles, B., Brzyska, B., Shamsan, Y., & Burge, B. (2009). The British picture vocabulary scale (Third ed.). GL Assessment.
  • Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the national reading panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36(3), 250–287. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.36.3.2
  • Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 8(3), 430–457. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5
  • Foorman, B. R., Herrera, S., Petscher, Y., Mitchell, A., & Truckenmiller, A. (2015). The structure of oral language and reading and their relation to comprehension in Kindergarten through Grade 2. Reading and Writing, 28(5), 655–681. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9544-5
  • Fricke, S., Bowyer‐Crane, C., Haley, A. J., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2013). Efficacy of language intervention in the early years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(3), 280–290. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12010
  • Gardner, H., Froud, K., McClelland, A., & van der Lely, H. K. (2006). Development of the Grammar and Phonology Screening (GAPS) test to assess key markers of specific language and literacy difficulties in young children. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(5), 513–540. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820500442644
  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. (1996). The children’s test of nonword repetition: The psychological corporation. Harcourt Brace & Company Publishers.
  • Gillam, R. B., & Pearson, N. A. (2017). Test of narrative language (Second ed.). PRO-ED.
  • Gillam, S. L., Olszewski, A., Fargo, J., & Gillam, R. B. (2014). Classroom-based narrative and vocabulary instruction: Results of an early-stage, nonrandomized comparison study. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 45(3), 204–219. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_LSHSS-13-0008
  • Ginsborg, J. (2006). The effects of socio-economic status on children’s language acquisition and useIn J. Clegg. (Eds). Language and social disadvantage: Theory into practice (pp. 9–27). Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (2003). The early catastrophe: The 30 million word gap by age 3. American Educator, 27(1), 4–9.
  • Heilmann, J., Miller, J. F., Nockerts, A., & Dunaway, C. (2010). Properties of the narrative scoring scheme using narrative retells in young school-age children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(2), 154–166. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0024)
  • Hipfner-Boucher, K., Milburn, T., Weitzman, E., Greenberg, J., Pelletier, J., & Girolametto, L. (2014). Relationships between preschoolers’ oral language and phonological awareness. First Language, 34(2), 178–197. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723714525945
  • Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 4–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027238
  • Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., West, G., Lervag, A., & Melby-Lervag, M. (2020). Children’s language skills can be improved: Lessons from psychological science for educational policy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(4), 372–377. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420923684
  • Jacoby, J. W., & Lesaux, N. K. (2014). Support for extended discourse in teacher talk with linguistically diverse preschoolers. Early Education and Development, 25(8), 1162–1179. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2014.907695
  • Jones, D. (2017). Talking about talk: Reviewing oracy in English primary education. Early Child Development and Care, 187(3–4), 498–508. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1211125
  • Justice, L. M., Bowles, R., Pence, K., & Gosse, C. (2010). A scalable tool for assessing children’s language abilities within a narrative context: The NAP (Narrative Assessment Protocol). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(2), 218–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.11.002
  • Justice, L. M., Mashburn, A., Pence, K. L., & Wiggins, A. (2008). Experimental evaluation of a preschool language curriculum: Influence on children’s expressive language skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51(4), 983–1001. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/072)
  • Justice, L. M., McGinty, A. S., Zucker, T., Cabell, S. Q., & Piasta, S. B. (2013). Bi-directional dynamics underlie the complexity of talk in teacher–child play-based conversations in classrooms serving at-risk pupils. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(3), 496–508. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.02.005
  • Kaldahl, A.-G. (2019). Assessing oracy: Chasing the teachers’ unspoken oracy construct across disciplines in the landscape between policy and freedom. L1 Educational Studies in Language and Literature, 19 (AssessingOracy) 1–24. https://doi.org/10.17239/l1esll-2019.19.03.02
  • Kaminski, R. A., Abbott, M., Bravo Aguayo, K., Latimer, R., & Good, I. I. I. R. H. (2014). The preschool early literacy indicators: Validity and benchmark goals. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 34(2), 71–82. https://doi.org/10.1177/0271121414527003
  • Karlsen, J., Hjetland, H. N., Hagtvet, B. E., Braeken, J., & Melby-Lervag, M. (2021). The concurrent and longitudinal relationship between narrative skills and other language skills in children. First Language, 41(5), 555–572. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723721995688
  • Kim, Y. S. G., & Schatschneider, C. (2017). Expanding the developmental models of writing: A direct and indirect effects model of developmental writing (DIEW). Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(1), 35–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000129
  • Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2015). The dimensionality of language ability in young children. Child Development, 86(6), 1948–1965. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12450
  • Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2017). Oral language and listening comprehension: Same or different constructs? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(5), 1273–1284. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-16-0039
  • Levine, D., Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., de Villiers, J., Iglesias, A., & Wilson, M. S. (2020). Evaluating socioeconomic gaps in preschoolers’ vocabulary, syntax and language process skills with the Quick Interactive Language Screener (QUILS). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50(1), 114–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.11.006
  • Lindgren, J. (2019). Comprehension and production of narrative macrostructure in Swedish: A longitudinal study from age 4 to 7. First Language, 39(4), 412–432. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723719844089
  • Little, R. J. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 1198–1202. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722
  • Lonigan, C. J., & Milburn, T. F. (2017). Identifying the dimensionality of oral language skills of children with typical development in preschool through fifth grade. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(8), 2185–2198. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-15-0402
  • Massonnié, J., Bianco, M., Lima, L., & Bressoux, P. (2019). Longitudinal predictors of reading comprehension in French at first grade: Unpacking the oral comprehension component of the simple view. Learning and Instruction, 60, 166–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.01.005
  • Mouzaki, A., Spyropoulou, E., Ralli, A., Antoniou, F., Diamanti, V., & Papaioannou, S. (2020). The dimensionality of oral language ability: Evidence from young greek children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(8), 2732–2751. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00008
  • MPlus. (2020). Chi-square difference testing using the Satorra-Bentler Scaled chi-square. https://www.statmodel.com/chidiff.shtml
  • Nation, K., Clarke, P., Marshall, C. M., & Durand, M. (2004). Hidden language impairments in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 199–211. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/017)
  • O’Neill, J. (2021, January 21). Average household income, UK: financial year 2020. Office for National Statistics. https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/personalandhouseholdfinances/incomeandwealth/bulletins/householddisposableincomeandinequality/financialyear2020#average-household-income-datamovies_l_5d277587e4b02a5a5d57b59e
  • Oracy All-Party Parliamentary Group. (2021). Speak for change. Final report and recommendations from the Oracy All-Party Parliamentary Group Inquiry (All-Party Parliamentary Group).
  • Pelatti, C. Y., Piasta, S. B., Justice, L. M., & O’Connell, A. (2014). Language- and literacy-learning opportunities in early childhood classrooms: Children’s typical experiences and within-classroom variability. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 445–456. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.05.004
  • Petersen, D. B., & Spencer, T. D. (2012). The narrative language measures: Tools for language screening, progress monitoring, and intervention planning. Perspectives on Language Learning and Education, 19(4), 119–129. https://doi.org/10.1044/lle19.4.119
  • Petersen, D. B., & Spencer, T. D. (2016). Using narrative intervention to accelerate canonical story grammar and complex language growth in culturally diverse preschoolers. Topics in Language Disorders, 36(1), 6–19. https://doi.org/10.1097/tld.0000000000000078
  • Pinker, S. (1997). Words and rules in the human brain. Nature, 387(6633), 547–548. https://doi.org/10.1038/42347
  • Pinto, G., Tarchi, C., & Bigozzi, L. (2015). The relationship between oral and written narratives: A three-year longitudinal study of narrative cohesion, coherence, and structure. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(4), 551–569. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12091
  • Protopapas, A. (2017). Learning to read Greek. In Verhoeven, Ludo, and Perfetti, Charles. (eds). Learning to read across languages and writing systems (pp. 155–180). Cambridge University Press.
  • Ragnarsdóttir, H., Simonsen, H., & Plunkett, K. (1999). The acquisition of past tense morphology in Icelandic and Norwegian children: An experimental study. Journal of Child Language, 26(3), 577–618. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000999003918
  • Rose, J. (2006). Independent review of the teaching of early reading. Department for Education and Skills.
  • Schmitt, M. B., Logan, J. A. R., Tambyraja, S. R., Farquharson, K., & Justice, L. M. (2017). Establishing language benchmarks for children with typically developing language and children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(2), 364–378. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0273
  • Schneider, P., Hayward, D., & Dubé, R. V. (2006). Storytelling from pictures using the Edmonton narrative norms instrument. Journal of Speech Language Pathology and Audiology, 30(4), 224–238. https://cjslpa.ca/files/2006_JSLPA_Vol_30/No_04_209-263/Schneider_Hayward_Dube_JSLPA_2006.pdf
  • Schreiber, J. B., Nora, A., Stage, F. K., Barlow, E. A., & King, J. (2006). Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: A review. The Journal of Educational Research, 99(6), 323–338. https://doi.org/10.3200/JOER.99.6.323-338
  • Shiel, G., Cregan, A., McGough, A., & Archer, P. (2012). Oral language in early childhood and primary education (3-8 years). National Council for Curriculum and Assessment.
  • Silva, M., & Cain, K. (2015). The relations between lower and higher level comprehension skills and their role in prediction of early reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(2), 321–331. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037769
  • Snow, C. E. (1991). The theoretical basis for relationships between language and literacy in development. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6(1), 5–10. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568549109594817
  • Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2011). Evidence-based interventions for reading and language difficulties: Creating a virtuous circle. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(1), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02014.x
  • Spencer, T. D., & Petersen, D. B. (2018). Bridging oral and written language: An oral narrative language intervention study with writing outcomes. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49(3), 569–581. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0030
  • Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2006). The dimensionality of language ability in school-age children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(6), 1193–1208. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/086)
  • Vega, V., Osman, Y., Katz, B., & Giordano, K. (2018). Hidden expectations: Do teachers use expressive language ability as a proxy for overall development? Early Child Development and Care, 190(11), 1682–1690. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2018.1546176
  • Wang, J., & Wang, X. (2012). Structural equation modeling: Applications using Mplus. John Wiley & Sons.
  • Wiig, E., Secord, W. A., & Semel, E. (2006). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals preschool—Second UK Edition. Pearson.
  • Wilkinson, A. (1968). Oracy in English teaching. Elementary English, 45(6), 743–747 https://www.jstor.org/stable/41386406.

Appendix A

Socio-demographic characteristics of the schools in the sample.