1,501
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Youth Perspectives on the Effects of a Family-centered Media Literacy Intervention to Encourage Healthier Eating

ORCID Icon, , , , &

ABSTRACT

A pretest-posttest field test with control group (N = 189 parent-child dyads) tested a structural model representing youths’ (ages 9–14) perspectives to examine the efficacy of a family-centered, media literacy-oriented intervention promoting fruit and vegetable consumption. The intervention facilitated critical discussion about nutrition and media, mentored by the parent. Results showed that youths’ increases in fruit and vegetable consumption flowed from parent-child discussion of nutrition labels, which was predicted by child-initiated discussion, critical thinking about media sources, and critical thinking about media content. Multivariate analyses revealed that the intervention was productive for all participating age groups and for all dependent variables. The results suggest that a developmental progression from critical thinking about source to critical thinking about content affects behavior change and can be catalyzed through media literacy education and encouragement to discuss media messages (i.e. practice) with parents.

Introduction

A dose-dependent response exists between media screen time in childhood and lifetime obesity risk (Crespo et al., Citation2001; Tahir et al., Citation2018). In addition to displacement of physical activity and mindless eating (Marsh et al., Citation2013), explanations often focus on the effects of food marketing on youth (Boyland & Halford, Citation2013; Powell et al., Citation2013; Wootan et al., Citation2019; Zimmerman & Bell, Citation2010). Many of these effects are manifested within the family environment, making it important for prevention efforts to address them in the context of family communication dynamics and a developmentally based sensitivity to youths’ perspectives about food marketing and family discussions.

This study reports on results from an intervention designed to reflect an understanding that the family’s management of the media environment must be responsive to the child’s developmental level and increasing independence. A curriculum was designed to address this issue by cultivating a positive environment for family interactions regarding the media and nutrition, focusing particularly on marketing. The curriculum aimed to help families cultivate an open and supportive communication environment that fostered critical thinking and shared decision making. By recognizing that children have agency for discussion and decision making, parents can help youth to cultivate effective skills as their children develop increasing autonomy. By encouraging youth to ask questions and by providing a mutually supportive environment for practicing parent-youth interaction skills, youth can gain confidence in their parents as expert information sources for nutrition and media information. This can prevent marketing pressures from creating a negative communication context for families. In this report we report results from the youths’ perspective.

Food marketing effects take place in the family context

Home and parenting characteristics predict children’s early BMI levels and later risk for obesity-related health problems (Boonpleng et al., Citation2013; East et al., Citation2019), motivating family-based interventions to improve modifiable child, parent and household factors (Shier et al., Citation2016). Yet most obesity-prevention interventions addressing media influences have focused on reducing youths’ screen time rather than on promoting critical discussion of food marketing and strategic use of credible information sources (Harris et al., Citation2009). Screen-reduction effects have been modest (Maniccia et al., Citation2011) and may be impractical or unacceptable to parents (Minges et al., Citation2015). This strategy also overlooks the influence of marketing that reaches far beyond the home television screen.

Food marketing promoting calorie dense, nutrient poor foods are ubiquitous (Harris et al., Citation2009), reaching children via radio, movies, billboards, print media (including food packaging and in-store promotions), and digital media (Olson et al., Citation2013). In the U.S., industry self-regulation has been ineffective (Olson et al., Citation2013), and the nutrition criteria established by the Children’s Food and Beverage Advertising Initiative has been criticized as inadequate (Wootan et al., Citation2019).

Youth awash in this environment tend to consider media messages as authoritative and persuasive information sources, including for food choices (Folta et al., Citation2006; Goris et al., Citation2010; Zimmerman & Bell, Citation2010). Children’s resulting lobbying of parents about food choices frequently succeeds, can create family conflict, and increases in effectiveness with age (Henry & Borzekowski, Citation2011; Hingle et al., Citation2010; Linn & Novosat, Citation2008). Youth also attain the ability to make independent food choices as they age and must learn how to navigate the media environment successfully on their own.

Although adolescence is a period of increased autonomy from parents (e.g., Eccles et al., Citation1991; Erikson, Citation1959; Koepke & Denissen, Citation2012; Steinberg & Silk, Citation2002), parents continue to influence healthy food consumption during this time period by providing access to healthy foods in the home and by modeling their consumption (Yee et al., Citation2017). However, as children transition into adolescence, they spend more time eating in their parents’ absence (Lachat et al., Citation2012) and spend increased time with peers (Lam et al., Citation2014). Compared to younger children, adolescents make many more food choices on their own—e.g., eating fast food with friends, buying snacks at the local convenience store, drinking coffee at the local coffee shop, or eating at a friend’s house. In an analysis of the 2003–2006 National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys, Poti and Popkin (Citation2011) found that adolescents consumed 36.8% of their calories outside of the home. Studies also have shown that adolescents who frequently eat alone consume greater quantities of fast food, sweets, and sugar-sweetened drinks (Reicks et al., Citation2019), and that eating with peers is associated with higher levels of consumption of energy-dense and low-nutrient foods (Ragelienė & Grønhøj, Citation2020). The adolescent’s developing autonomy is reflected in how adolescents and parents view adolescent food choices. Parents typically view these decisions as prudential issues (i.e., relevant to the child’s health or well-being) and therefore feel justified in enforcing rules regulating the child’s consumption; adolescents, in contrast, typically view this as a personal issue—i.e., an area under their own personal jurisdiction (Smetana & Asquith, Citation1994). Therefore, it is critical, especially during adolescence, to directly target adolescents’ attitudes toward eating rather than assuming that educating parents will necessarily lead to the desired changes in adolescent attitudes and behavior.

Involving parents in childhood nutrition promotion that addresses children’s decision making and the media has had promising but limited results. For example, Hindin et al. (Citation2004) successfully taught Head Start parents how to discuss food advertising with their children, critically analyze commercials and prepared them to manage requests for advertised food but did not assess effects on children. (Evans et al., Citation2006) efforts to change both parent and child media awareness and eating behaviors found that parents provided more fruits and vegetables but providing them did not increase child consumption. Similarly, a media literacy-based program provided by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) for 11 to 13-year-old adolescents achieved some attitudinal changes but no behavioral ones (Eunice Kennedy Shriver NICHH, Citation2013). These results suggest that programs need to account more effectively for the fact that parents are influenced by youth requests and that youth food consumption depends considerably on what foods parents choose to provide. Family communication dynamics around sources of information about food may represent a promising intervention target.

In response to this need, we field tested a media literacy-based intervention for families with 9–14 year olds to promote Child Initiated Discussions about persuasive messages (food marketing) and more credible sources of information (food labels). The strategy acknowledged that parent-initiated conversations in a domain in which many children believe they have legitimate decision-making authority can yield youth resistance (Lagattuta et al., Citation2010). A more beneficial approach might encourage child interest in media literacy issues and thereby increase the frequency of child-originated conversations that are likely to have a greater impact on child learning because they build on the child’s interest (Brown, Citation1997; Kaiser et al., Citation2018). Moreover, in response to child initiations, parents can scaffold the development of further understanding (Wood et al., Citation1976).

Evidence suggests media literacy can be learned and can improve decision making

The theoretical model guiding our intervention holds that critical thinking about media messages begins by understanding that every message is created by someone pursuing a goal. Marketing messages, for example, are designed to promote products rather than to provide a balanced presentation of product benefits and drawbacks. Thus, while marketing messages can entertain and inform, consumers must treat them skeptically with respect to accuracy and completeness. Promoting Critical Thinking about Sources therefore can motivate Critical Thinking about Content. Critical thinking is consideration of a variety of solutions to a question and analysis of potential outcomes for alternative strategies (National Center for Education Statistics, Citation2000). Critical thinking about sources provides the foundation for critical thinking about message content, encompassing an understanding that messages are created, that creators of messages have purposes for doing so, that they have areas of expertise, and that they have a viewpoint (Austin et al.,Citation2016; Jacks & Devine, Citation2000; Sagarin et al., Citation2002). Skills for critically assessing content, which include elements such as currency, completeness, accuracy, perspective, and techniques used to create the message, then can reduce the influence of affect on decisions. (Austin et al., Citation2007; Kupersmidt et al., Citation2010). Rigorously designed media literacy programs have been recommended by the American Academy of Pediatrics (Strasburger, Citation2011) and can affect health outcomes with effect sizes generally in the range of 0.32 to 0.41 (Jeong et al., Citation2012; Xie et al., Citation2019). Our theoretical model therefore incorporates consideration of the roles of Critical Thinking about Sources and Critical thinking about Content in the development of critical thinking about media and decision making about food choices.

How the curriculum was designed to be responsive to youth development

The FoodMania curriculum was designed to maximize success across age groups while acknowledging that developmental differences would exist that could affect cognitive responses, understanding and recall of information along with affective responses to the media, marketing and family dynamics. The curriculum was modified from a format employed previously for 11–13 year olds regarding media influences on food purchases but retired in 2017 (Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Citation2013). It was developed in a family-centered collaboration among Washington State University and University of Washington faculty (Austin et al. Citation2017), based on the Message Interpretation Processing Model (MIP; Austin et al. Citation2000; Austin et al., Citation2007; Pinkleton et al. Citation2013) and following the Strengthening Families program design (Molgaard & Spoth, Citation2001; Nathanson, Citation2004). The intervention model and structural model operationalizing the intervention applied social, cognitive, and dual-processing theories along with the MIP model to demonstrate how family members’ communication and interactions regarding food marketing, food labels and food consumption reflected the results of the curriculum as experienced by youth.

The final six-week program employed a two-hour unit each week during which parents and youth met separately for one hour and jointly for one hour. Units convened at a community location (e.g., school, community center), led by university extension faculty. Lessons included information about food marketing techniques, specific practice to discuss and compare nutrition facts labels, expectancies related to mediating advertising influences and eating fruits and vegetables, and how to apply critical thinking toward food advertising. The curriculum (see supplementary materials) incorporated activities such as deconstructing food ads, reading and comparing food labels, tasting foods and drinks, designing a media campaign, and discussion strategies. The program was delivered by Cooperative Extension educators who are accustomed to delivering programming to mixed age groups.

Regarding developmental issues, Rozendaal et al. (Citation2011) have suggested that advertising literacy requires mastery of 1) cognitive advertising literacy, which refers to recognition of advertising, knowledge of selling and persuasive intent, tactics used, and information about the industry; 2) affective/attitudinal advertising literacy, which refers to individuals’ abilities to reduce their susceptibility based on skepticism and the cultivation of negative attitudes; and 3) advertising literacy performance, which refers to individuals’ abilities to retrieve relevant information apply it as needed. Consistent with their recommendation, the FoodMania curriculum incorporated the practice of advertising literacy performance to assist those children whose less mature cognitive abilities might otherwise limit their independent use of the conceptual or cognitive advertising literacy elements present in the curriculum.

As well, Hwang et al. (Citation2018) have recommended that advertising literacy curricula might include components of content (themes and ideas), grammar (tactics) and structure (social and economic structure of the advertising industry), They found, however, that while structure literacy enhanced knowledge gain for older youth it appeared to confuse younger youth. FoodMania did not focus on structure literacy other than to make sure the youth understand that marketers hire experts who help them to target youth specifically and to the extent it came up in discussions with parents regarding marketing research. Parents of course could bring this up with their older youth as they thought appropriate. Youth also could ask questions suited to their developmental level (child-initiated discussion).

In addition, the curriculum included tactile activities and rehearsal with parents to help youth who might still be developing their understanding of and ability to manage their responses to abstractions such as marketers’ intentions to persuade people to believe things, which is more sophisticated than understanding the intent to get people to buy things (Lapierre, Citation2015, Citation2019). These require increasingly sophisticated understandings of how the mind works. A less developed theory of mind can lead to difficulties understanding concepts such as advertisers, audiences, and profit (Moses & Baldwin, Citation2005). Youth in focus groups had indicated a desire to do activities with parents, so their involvement also was expected to foster positive reinforcement of curriculum content.

Although the curriculum included components to boost affective/attitudinal literacy, it specifically did not cultivate a dislike of media or marketers. Austin et al. (Citation2015) found that “demonizing media messages teens find desirable” is unnecessary and potentially counterproductive. Given that this could present a problematic issue particularly for older youth more reliant on media for social purposes, the curriculum was designed to cultivate self-discovery and collaborative exploration. For example, youth participated in taste tests and practiced deconstructing advertising. They then tried using marketing techniques to create their own cereal package, fruit and vegetable company, taglines and nutrition promotion campaigns.

Despite these accommodations, it was possible that the increased base of existing knowledge and experience present among older youth would lead to some age differences in intervention results. Xie et al. (Citation2019) have found that media literacy interventions are largely consistent across age groups, but older youth might have less to learn. They may have learned about these topics previously in school, for example. Including primary caregivers in the program provided an important benefit because they are accustomed to adapting information to the unique needs and interests of their children.

A family-centered program provides an optimum context for media literacy-related effects

The theoretical basis for our intervention posits that a family-centered media literacy program will facilitate opportunities for youth to develop and practice critical thinking skills applied to food marketing and use of nutrition food labels. This is important for both parents and youth because acquisition of media literacy skills may not necessarily result in the application of them (Austin et al., Citation2002; Duran et al., Citation2008). In addition, the family provides an ideal context for providing the practical experience and developmentally appropriate interaction through family discussion and parental reinforcement of critical thinking skills and skepticism related to media messages (Buijzen, Citation2009; Calvert, Citation2008). As youth grow older, critical discussion of media also is likely to be more effective than restricting media usage (Buijzen, Citation2009). Some evidence to support a family-centered strategy exists, such as from a program for 4th and 5th grade students that incorporated a child-to-parent component, improving parent social support, availability and accessibility of fruits and vegetables in the home (Evans et al., Citation2006). Our theoretical model therefore incorporates the opportunity for rehearsal of these skills through discussion: in this case Talk about Nutrition Labels, which were a focus of the intervention curriculum. Most importantly, the model focuses on the moderating role of Child Initiated Discussion because of its potential for magnifying the impact of family discussion about food marketing and food selections (Brown, Citation1997; Kaiser et al., Citation2018; Wood et al., Citation1976).

Discussion is vital because children progress from understanding that a source may try to sell something to a more abstract understanding that a source intends to change how someone thinks (Carter et al., Citation2011; Rozendaal et al., Citation2010). Even a rudimentary level of understanding of selling intent can associate with parents’ reported purchasing behaviors (Lapierre, Citation2019). Youth who can achieve this first step, however, still need help to master a “stop and think” response, which develops with executive function (Rozendaal et al., Citation2011) but can be assisted by others such as through media literacy education Kupersmidt et al. (Citation2010). As they enter adolescence, youths’ reasoning, abstract thinking and metacognitive skills continue to develop (Dumontheil, Citation2014; Gauffroy & Barrouillet, Citation2011; Mills & Keil, Citation2004; Weil et al., Citation2013). They also increasingly make independent food choices, such as from a store on their route home after school (Carter et al., Citation2011). Parental influence throughout this developmental process is important, even while it may increasingly be manifested indirectly, through youths’ use of decision-making skills learned and practiced through family interactions. This makes it important for primary caregivers to develop skills for effective interactions regarding food marketing, nutrition and food selection, and to do so before children reach adolescence.

In sum, an intervention that engages parents with children around interpretation of food marketing and nutrition labels to make choices about foods to consume provides a natural way to build on a parent’s responsibility to create and manage a nurturing home environment. Parent-child interactions around eating behaviors and physical activity are instrumental to the success of family-based obesity prevention (Kitzman-Ulrich et al., Citation2010) through the cultivation of certain behaviors, expectations, beliefs and norms (Rhee, Citation2008). Studies tracking intervention outcomes related to general family management skills have found an indirect effect on pediatric weight gain from toddlerhood to middle childhood (Smith et al., Citation2015). Parental influence manifested through the practice of media literacy skills for food marketing and discussion of nutrition labels therefore has the potential to provide long-lasting effects.

Value of youth reports

Reflecting the importance of the child’s perspective and agency, our test of this theoretical model focused on youth self-reports. Both Daly (Citation2009) and Hill (Citation2006) have asserted that concerns about the potential for children to adversely affect research quality (Campbell, Citation2008; Moules & O’Brien, Citation2012); youths’ abilities to participate (Clarke, Citation2015); and that children might suffer adverse effects from research participation (Moules & O’Brien, Citation2012; Powell & Smith, Citation2009) have led to research in which youth are used as subjects, but rarely as active research partners. The Convention on the Rights of Children in 1989 (Unicef, Citation1989), however, has emphasized that children are social actors who should be seen as essential for the planning, implementation and evaluation of health research (Salsberg et al., Citation2015). (Boaz, Robert, et al., Citation2016) suggest that properly integrating and assessing youth participation in research requires health researchers to reimagine how their research is designed, conducted and communicated. Similarly, a recent systematic review of health promotion research involving youth (Larsson et al., Citation2018) found that while many studies wanted to increase youth participation in the research process, few studies included participation beyond that of research subjects.

Accordingly, this analysis focused on the child’s perspective to assess the results of the family-centered media literacy intervention. Previous research comparing youth and parental reports on parental mediation of television influences has found unique value to the use of youth reports. Fujioka and Austin (Citation2003), for example, found that the more youth noticed their parents’ positive reinforcement of television messages, the more they accepted those messages into their attitudes and beliefs. In their study, focused on alcohol-related content, they also found that the child’s perceptions predicted self-reported decision-making outcome variables more reliably than the parental reports did. Youths’ interpretations therefore can provide unique and valuable information to guide an assessment of a family-based intervention, despite the methodological limitations of self-reports.

The theoretical model tested herein followed on previous tests of two complementary models that incorporated both parent and youth reports from one wave of these data (Austin et al., Citation2018) and from two waves (Austin et al., Citation2020a, Citation2020b). In one of the previous models tested (Austin et al., Citation2020a), several relationships among variables tested in this study were confirmed in a structural model which hypothesized that critical parental discussion of food marketing (Negative Mediation) would increase youths’ Critical Thinking about Content, which would increase their Child Initiated Discussion and, in turn, their fruit and vegetable (ns) consumption. In this study, however, we wished to address two key issues not tested in that model: first, a more complete developmental model of media literacy learning that would incorporate the role of youths’ Critical Thinking about Media Sources, which was not included in the previous model; and second, the role of the child’s perspective and Child Initiated Discussion from that vantage point.

This project therefore focused on answering the following research questions:

  • RQ1: To what extent will child’s age interact with the intervention to affect results on the youth-reported dependent variables?

  • RQ2: Will a family-centered media literacy intervention’s effects on youth-reported outcomes of Vegetables Eaten Yesterday and Fruit Eaten Yesterday be manifested through progressive effects of the intervention on Child Initiated Discussion, which will affect Critical Thinking about Sources, Critical Thinking about Content, the effects of which are mediated through Talk about Nutrition Labels with parents?

illustrates the hypothesized process model. The latent constructs Child Initiated Discussion (CID), Critical Thinking about Sources (CTS), and Critical Thinking about Content (CTC) are represented by ovals (individual indicators not shown). The single outcome measures Talk about Nutrition Labels (TNL), Fruit Eaten Yesterday (FEY), and Vegetables Eaten Yesterday (VEY) are represented with boxes. We anticipated that, based the research literature on parental mediation processes and the value of child-initiated discussion, that, relying on the youth’s reports:

Figure 1. The basic hypothesized youth process model. Not shown, but included in the model, were age and pretest levels of each construct and measure.

Figure 1. The basic hypothesized youth process model. Not shown, but included in the model, were age and pretest levels of each construct and measure.
  • H1: Child Initiated Discussion about food advertising and nutrition will positively associate with Critical Thinking about Sources for food advertising;

  • H2: Child Initiated Discussion will positively associate with Critical Thinking about Content of food advertising;

  • H3: Child Initiated Discussion will positively associate with parent-child Talk about Nutrition Labels;

  • H4: Critical Thinking about Message Sources will positively associate with Parent-Child Talk about Nutrition Labels;

  • H5: Critical Thinking about Message Content will positively associate with Parent-Child Talk about Nutrition Labels;

  • H6: Parent-Child Talk about Nutrition Labels will positively associate with child reports of Fruit Eaten Yesterday;

  • H7: Parent-Child Talk about Nutrition Labels will positively associate with child reports of Vegetables Eaten Yesterday.

Methods

Following initial development, the FoodMania curriculum underwent an initial pilot test with 59 families followed by focus groups with parents, youth, and educators to further refine the curriculum and evaluation measures. Data collection procedures and the intervention were approved by the authors’ institutional review board for the use of human subjects (#15012).

Following training to maximize fidelity, the research team collected pretest and posttest data from both parents and youth using a self-administered survey prior to the start of the intervention and following the final session. Families (N = 189 dyads) from three urban and two rural counties in Washington state participated through Extension programs. Treatment and control group families had sociocultural backgrounds typical for their respective 4-H and SNAP-Ed audiences. Initially, participants self-selected into either the intervention or control group. Because intervention groups filled first, recruitment continued to find comparable controls. Data were collected no more than 2 weeks prior to the first session and at the final session for the intervention group, or after six weeks for the control group. Attrition rates pretest to posttest were 16.93% for parents and 17.46% for youth. Missing data rates were 1.06% and 2.12% for parents and youth, respectively. Results based largely on the adult responses are reported elsewhere (Austin et al., Citation2020a, Citation2020b).

Nutrition items were selected from the National Collaborative on Childhood Obesity Research Measures Registry that aligned with the curriculum (McKinnon et al., Citation2012). Youth were asked about the number of fruits (M = 3.41, SD = 1.58) and vegetables (M = 3.32, SD = 1.55) eaten yesterday and reported on a variety of constructs (Hoelscher et al., Citation2003). This dietary assessment approach is used in the Coordinated Approached to Child Health program and community-based settings (Penkilo et al., Citation2008; Springer et al., Citation2013). Food frequency questionnaires not reliant on portion sizes, parent assistance or recall of previous days of food intake tend to have the best reliability and validity for youth (Kolodziejczyk et al., Citation2012). Vegetables were defined as salads, boiled, baked, mashed potatoes and all cooked and uncooked vegetables. Youth were instructed, “do not count French fries or chips” with a graphic visual of 14 commonly consumed fresh, whole vegetables provided. Youth were instructed to not count fruit juice and to refer to a graphic visual of 12 commonly consumed whole, fresh fruits.

Other measures were not time-based due to poor youth recall ability that tends to reflect a 24-hour period (Baxter et al., Citation2004) but instead were measured at pretest and posttest, with pretest measures controlled in the posttest analyses.

Child Initiated Discussion (M = 2.59, SD =.75, α = .78) was measured with “I start conversations with my parents about:” “selling techniques that food or drink advertisers use;” “what is real or not real in food advertising;” “nutritional content of foods;” “healthier food choices” (graphically presented as NO!; 2, No; 3, Yes; 4, YES!). This was included to enable a test of whether youth in the control and intervention groups differed in how frequently they talked about these topics with their family members.

Critical Thinking about Sources (M = 2.57, SD =.72, α = .77) was measured with I think about how someone created a food or drink ad that I see; I think about who made the food or drink ad; I think about what the creator of a food or drink ad wants me to think; I think about why someone created a food or drink ad I see. (graphically presented as NO!; 2, No; 3, Yes; 4, YES!) based on the strategy employed by Austin and Johnson (Citation1997). This was intended to reflect discussions about food marketing.

Critical Thinking about Content (M = 2.78, SD =.74, α = .70) was measured with “I think twice about what I see in food or drink ads;” “I think about things I see in food or drink ads before I believe them;” “I ask somebody before I believe something I see in food or drink ads;” “I think about what a food ad is not showing me about the food.” (graphically presented as NO!; 2, No; 3, Yes; 4, YES!). This also was intended to reflect discussions about food marketing.

Parent-child Talk about Nutrition Labels (M = 2.92, SD = 1.14) was measured with “How often do you and a parent (your mom or dad) talk about the nutrition information from the label of a packaged food? (Never, Rarely, Sometimes, Often, Very Often). This was intended to help families determine food selections, such as when in a store. This measure was included to test whether intervention and control groups differed in their application of specific lesson content focused on reading and comparing nutrition labels.

Intraclass correlations and design effects were first estimated to determine whether the clustered nature of the intervention administration affected the data in a meaningful way (Muthén & Satorra, Citation1995). As reported elsewhere (Austin et al., Citation2020a) and with selected results displayed in , multiple analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was used to estimate mean differences between the Intervention and Control groups on the latent constructs and single measure outcomes with covariates controlling for pretest levels of each dependent variable. The age by condition interaction results were then used to inform the building of a structural equation process model. Both MANCOVAs were estimated in MPLUS 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2017) using the Satorra and Bentler (Citation1994) correction for non-normal data to ensure accurate standard errors.

Table 1. Standardized mean differences MANCOVA.

The hypothesized model was based on theory and previously tested alternative models (Austin et al., Citation2020a). The conversations and thinking tested in the model were theorized to take place in an iterative fashion and are represented in a parsimonious manner to lead to the specific behavior of fruits and veggies eaten yesterday at the end of that process. The hypothesized model reversed the path of Child Initiated Discussion and Critical Thinking about Content from a previously tested model to test the role of the youth’s perspective and agency, add the role of Critical Thinking about Sources, and test the potential for Talk about Nutrition Labels to act as a mediating variable on the outcome variables of Fruit and Vegetables Eaten Yesterday. To test the hypothesized model, a multigroup structural equation model (MGSEM) was fit to the data to estimate the process by which youth critical thinking and discussion about nutrition impact the consumption of healthy foods (Hoyle, Citation1995). The MGSEM groups were the Control (n = 83) and Intervention (n = 106) conditions. Age could then be incorporated as a control variable to account for any interaction effects detected in the MANCOVA. All elements in the MGSEM were controlled for pretest levels (not shown in ). Each indicator within a construct was correlated with itself across time (Little et al., Citation2007). The model was fit in MPLUS 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2017). None of the 189 cases in the sample were excluded due to excessive missing data. Guidelines from Hu and Bentler (Citation1999) were used to assess the fit of the model (e.g. CFI >.95, RMSEA <.06, SRMR <.08). Because we were sensitive to the impact of age on all outcomes in the model, we placed age as a control on all constructs and outcomes to provide accurate estimates of age’s total effect on each element measured within the model and to align the SEM with our MANCOVA results.

The assumption of multivariate normality was found lacking for at least two of our outcomes, Fruit and Vegetables eaten yesterday, which tested as significantly (p < .05) platykurtic. It has been established that a lack of normality does not affect the accuracy of maximum likelihood estimates but can lead to Type I or Type II errors due to inaccurate standard errors (Finney & DiStefano, Citation2006). It was not possible in MPLUS to implement the Satorra and Bentler (Citation1994) correction for nonnormal data because it is not available in the multigroup environment. We therefore chose to use the Bayes estimator.

Bayesian estimation provides greater flexibility under small sample size conditions and does not require distributional assumptions, such as multivariate normality, required with standard frequentist approaches (Levy & Choi, Citation2013; van de Schoot et al., Citation2015). Estimates from our maximum likelihood analysis were used to specify prior distributions for the structural paths. Large prior distribution variances were specified, however, to indicate a moderate level of certainty about the specified estimates on the part of the authors. Large prior variances also allowed the data to drive the estimation process as opposed to prior beliefs given the exploratory nature of the research. Model fit was assessed using guidelines from Levy and Choi (Citation2013) and van de Schoot et al. (Citation2015).

A model building process was initiated by specifying a single group baseline model with no distinction made between the Control and Intervention groups using diffuse priors that allowed the data to drive the estimation process. The Baseline assumed no meaningful differences in structural paths between the Control and Intervention groups. A multigroup Bayesian model was then estimated using the diffuse default priors available in MPLUS under the assumption of no prior knowledge concerning the paths and allowing both groups to be estimated without constraints. Maximum likelihood (ML) was then employed on a multigroup model and the resulting estimates were used as our priors in the Bayesian model. The Bayesian model was estimated with both Intervention and Control group paths constrained to use the same set of priors. A final model was then estimated by eliminating the constraint and allowing the two groups to be estimated independently using our priors. This final model tested our hypothesis that the intervention model would produce larger and more significant path effects compared with the control model while also reducing the likelihood of Type I error experienced under ML estimation due to a lack of multivariate normality. We made use of the Deviance Information Criteria (DIC) and the Posterior Predictive P-value (PPP) to compare results during the model building process (Zyphur & Oswald, Citation2015). Specifically, the DIC was used as a comparative index (similar to AIC or BIC) in which lower DIC values indicate better relative model fit.

Results

Intraclass correlations and design effects showed that the average ICC was 1.6% across all 30 indicators and measures, and all were less than 5%. We therefore concluded that standard errors for paths and mean differences would not be noticeably affected by the clustered nature of the data.

MANCOVA age x interaction effects

The three significant age by condition interaction effects from are presented in . Child Initiated Discussion (Panel A), Critical Thinking about Sources (Panel B), and Fruit Eaten Yesterday (Panel C) are shown with the Intervention group (solid line) and Control group (dashed line) with youth age on the X axis. For each construct, the Intervention group shows a significant improvement over the Control for most ages. Youth in the Intervention group reported larger increases of Child Initiated Discussion and Critical Thinking about Sources as age increased with little or no differences for youth of ages 9 and 10 years. Youth in the Intervention group also reported eating more fruit the previous day compared with youth in the Control group. Interestingly, the slope of the Control group increases with age while the slope of the Intervention decreases even though there is an overall increase for the Intervention group compared to Control. While the interaction effects in Panels A and B reflect positive change with age in the Intervention group, the negative Intervention slope for Fruit Eaten Yesterday may reflect that for fruit consumption the intervention has a greater impact on younger children producing a greater increase in healthy consumption compared with older intervention youth.

Figure 2. The age by condition interaction effects (N = 143) on child initiated discussion (Panel A), critical thinking about source (Panel B), and fruit eaten yesterday (Panel C). A version of the results presented in panels a and C have been reported previously in a multivariate analysis (N = 137) with a larger model (Austin et al., Citation2020b). Both versions controlled for pretest levels of the dependent variables.

Figure 2. The age by condition interaction effects (N = 143) on child initiated discussion (Panel A), critical thinking about source (Panel B), and fruit eaten yesterday (Panel C). A version of the results presented in panels a and C have been reported previously in a multivariate analysis (N = 137) with a larger model (Austin et al., Citation2020b). Both versions controlled for pretest levels of the dependent variables.

Table 2. Standardized mean differences MANCOVA controlling for youth age for dependent variables showing a significant age by condition interaction.

MGSEM results

The results of the MANCOVA models indicated that Youth Age should be included as a covariate in the multigroup structural model for terms demonstrating the age by condition interaction. The MGSEM results showed that the intervention effects drove improvements in the home dietary environment as reflected by increased youth consumption of fruits and vegetables in the Intervention group compared to the control. shows results of the Bayes-estimated MGSEM in which Panel A displays significant paths for the Control group and Panel B displays significant paths for the Intervention group. Of the four MGSEM models examined, the hypothesized final model, estimated using the ML priors and no constraints between groups, produced the lowest DIC (11594.609) indicating the best fit to the data. The model PPP, however, was only .005 indicating underestimation of the observed data by the model, i.e. a conservative estimate of the likely population parameters.

Figure 3. The final multigroup structural equation model showing differences between control group (Panel A) and intervention group (Panel B).

Figure 3. The final multigroup structural equation model showing differences between control group (Panel A) and intervention group (Panel B).

The results in show that the intervention effects produced more significant paths (p < .05) and larger effect sizes compare to the control. The age of the intervention group youth also produced significant effects on three of the six outcomes. The effect of youth age on Child Initiated Discussion and Critical Thinking about Sources reflected the positive effects found in the MANCOVA, while the total effect of age on Talk about Nutrition Labels and Vegetables Eaten Yesterday were both non-significant (, p > .05). Finally, the total effect of age on Fruit Eaten Yesterday (TE = −0.130, p < .01) also reflects the interaction results from the MANCOVA in which the positive effect on fruit consumption in the intervention group was less for older youth compared to younger participants.

Table 3. Control and intervention group estimates from the MGSEM model.

Main and total effects for Youth Age and Child Initiated Discussion were calculated for Talk about Nutrition Labels, Vegetables Eaten Yesterday, and Fruit Eaten Yesterday from the final MGSEM model as shown in for both Control and Intervention (MacKinnon, Citation2012). Overall, effect sizes in the intervention group are larger and more are statistically significant (p < .05). The effects of Child Initiated Discussion to Talk about Nutrition Labels (b* = 0.661) and to Critical Thinking about Content (b* = 0.451) were considerably larger in the Intervention model compared with the Control. One path in the intervention model appeared smaller than in the control, from Child Initiated Discussion to Critical Thinking about Sources for which the control group exhibited a larger effect size (b* = 0.458, p < .001) compared with the intervention (b* = 0.366, p < .001). This seeming discrepancy is due to the significant effect of age on Critical Thinking about Sources in the intervention model and is another reflection of the interaction effect between youth age and condition observed in the MANCOVA and shown in Panel B. Despite the smaller effect size in the intervention group, there is an overall greater increase in Critical Thinking about Sources in the intervention group compared with the control.

Finally, the Bayesian 95% credibility intervals shown in for the Intervention group are considerably smaller, in most cases, compared to the Control indicating an increase in estimate precision as a result of the intervention. Credibility intervals are similar to frequentist confidence intervals with the important conceptual difference that the Bayesian 95% CI can be interpreted as a 95% probability that the true population parameter lies within the interval (Levy & Choi, Citation2013).

Discussion

This study tested a structural model representing the youths’ perspective to examine the efficacy of a family-centered media literacy-oriented intervention to promote healthy food consumption. The intervention facilitated Child Initiated Discussion for improving 9–14 year olds’ critical thinking about media messages and promoted parent-child discussion of factual nutrition content. The intervention involved both youth and parents in its design, implementation, and measurement of results. It did not focus on encouraging parents to reduce youths’ screen time. As predicted, the program demonstrated improvements in youths’ healthy eating behaviors by increasing productive interactions among family members, with food marketing and nutrition as the focus for discussion.

Multivariate analyses of the results revealed that the intervention was productive for all participating age groups. Older children improved more than younger children on Child-Initiated Discussion and Critical Thinking about Sources, while younger children benefitted more for Fruit Eaten Yesterday, perhaps reflecting younger children’s greater dependence on parents for providing fruit at home. The findings that the intervention had greater effects on older than younger children for both Child Initiated Discussion and Critical Thinking About Sources are consistent with the increases in abstract thinking and complex reasoning that occur between middle childhood and early adolescence (e.g., Dumontheil, Citation2014; Gauffroy & Barrouillet, Citation2011; Inhelder & Piaget, Citation1958). These particular age differences may reflect that as children develop their ability to reason and think abstractly, they are better equipped to initiate conversations about the intentions and strategies of advertisers (Hwang et al., Citation2018).

The results highlighted the importance of the family-centered context for intervention. As expected, the intervention’s effects on Child Initiated Discussion mentored by the parent were the catalyst for critical thinking and behavior change. The confirmed model, which accounted for changes from pretest to posttest, showed that youths’ increases in fruit and vegetable consumption flowed from parent-child Talk About Nutrition Labels, which was predicted by Child Initiated Discussion. Particularly through Child Initiated Discussion, the intervention also improved youths’ critical thinking about sources and message content, independent skills necessary for their longer-term decision making about food and nutrition.

Not surprisingly, study results also provide support for a developmental perspective to media literacy education. While child-initiated discussion had an influence on Critical Thinking About Sources for the control group and the intervention group, it had an effect on Critical Thinking About Content only for the intervention group. This suggests that children already had been developing skills for considering the persuasive intent of sources, which appears to progress from an understanding of a selling function to a deeper understanding of persuasive intent (Carter et al., Citation2011; Grohs et al., Citation2012; Lapierre, Citation2015; Rozendaal et al., Citation2010). Skills for considering message content features, such as techniques for making food and its consumers look especially attractive in marketing messages, were better developed among the intervention group and especially so for the older youth. Moreover, Child Initiated Discussion predicted Critical Thinking About Content only for the intervention group, suggesting that these youth were practicing a new skill that did not exist in the control group. Meanwhile, older youth benefitted more from the intervention for Critical Thinking About Sources, with all ages benefitting equally for Critical Thinking About Content. These findings appear to indicate that older youth were developmentally primed to build critical thinking momentum on their previously existing knowledge base about sources. They are at the beginning stages, however, for their mastery of critical thinking skills about message content as their abilities for abstract thinking, critical reasoning and metacognition begin to develop and grow in sophistication (Brown & DeLoache, Citation1988; Gauffroy & Barrouillet, Citation2011; Mills & Keil, Citation2004; Weil et al., Citation2013). In other words, while some media literacy skills likely develop naturally through social interactions and cognitive maturation, the speed and depth of mastery can be increased through instruction and opportunities for practice.

The structural model tested in this study followed on a previous test of a family-based structural model showing that the program’s effects on parents’ critical discussion of food marketing (Negative Mediation) associated with increases in youths’ critical thinking about food marketing content, which positively associated with their Child-Initiated Discussion and, in turn, with youths’ consumption with fruit – although not with vegetables. In the model tested herein, we wanted to investigate how Talk about Nutrition Labels might serve as a mediating variable – discussion practice – to predict the behavioral outcomes of fruit and vegetable consumption. Using the youths’ perspectives exclusively, we used Child Initiated Discussion as the predictor variable, and we incorporated Critical Thinking about Sources into the model along with Critical Thinking about Content. The total effect size for Child Initiated Discussion was almost identical in both analyses for vegetables (.091 in this study, p = .027; .101 in Austin et al., Citation2020a, ns) and for fruits (.150 in this study, p = .001; .195, p < .05), demonstrating good consistency in model fit. In addition, the correlation was virtually the same in both analyses between Critical Thinking about Content and Child Initiated Discussion despite the additional variables and the reversed relationship hypothesized in this model, which we had proposed from the child’s vantage point rather than from the parent’s.

The model was confirmed as parsimonious, with a good fit, documenting strengthened relationships among the variables in the model at posttest as hypothesized to occur from the intervention. It should be emphasized that structural models are theorized to be causal but actually test hypothesized associations that cannot be documented as causal without a highly controlled situation. Our study involved a field test in a community setting with self-reported measures. This study did not include 24-hour dietary recalls, BMI measures and other measures that could provide behavioral or longer-term documentation of behavioral change. Also, as noted in (Austin et al., Citation2020a), the limited cultural relevance and language accessibility of the program dampened its reach and effectiveness for Hispanic/Latinx and other groups: Too often, widely available programs lack community values, translations and culturally responsive recommendations, information sources and worldviews, and so it would be useful to produce and test more culturally relevant or modifiable versions of FoodMania given its promising results. Media literacy may play an important role in closing equity gaps in health and other socio-economic structures (Austin et al., Citation2021; Koonce, Citation2017; Media Literacy Now, Citation2020).

It is encouraging that the intervention demonstrated results across the age span of 9–14 years even while revealing some apparently developmentally related differences in study outcomes. The curriculum’s focus on content (themes and ideas) and grammar (tactics) literacy but not on structure (social and economic structure of the advertising industry), literacy may have enhanced its overall success, consistent with the findings of Hwang et al. (Citation2018). Although program developers had expressed concern about a lack of age-specific program content and delivery strategies, parents commonly teach and nurture youth of varied ages and sophistication. The curriculum also included specific opportunities for rehearsal and cooperative reinforcement of lesson content, consistent with the recommendation of Rozendaal et al. (Citation2011), through hands-on activities that youth helped design through the program development process. This made our approach relevant for a family-centered context and family-based community activities that often must have relevance for youth across a wide age range.

Consistent with scholars’ recommendations to include children as research partners rather than just as subjects, a strength of this study was that the intervention involved youth in program development and assessment. Although the study collected data from parents and some analyses have incorporated parental feedback (e.g. Austin et al., Citation2018), the models tested in this study focused on the youth perspective and the variables emerged from youth self-reports. Previous research has suggested that parents and youth can differ meaningfully on their interpretations of communicative acts such as parental mediation practices, making the analysis of youth perspectives valuable to consider independently despite limitations of reliability and recall (Fujioka & Austin, Citation2003). The results suggest that the family-centered intervention was empowering for the youth by promoting youth-initiated discussion, thereby producing beneficial effects for family decision making processes and youth behaviors.

It will be useful to extend tests of the study’s hypotheses to other topics, more generalizable samples, and designs that facilitate the analysis of sustained effects and less intensive, flexible and modifiable delivery modalities. Meta-analyses of media literacy interventions (e.g. Xie et al., Citation2019) indicate that media literacy interventions can have lasting effects, yet few studies currently exist to verify this across topics and accounting for potential mechanisms of change, decay or strengthening of effects over time. The family-centered model also could make it possible to sensitively address topics with stigma such as substance use and mental health, to misinformation and disinformation more broadly, and to civic engagement more specifically. It also is possible that the model could be applied to media literacy education for science and health literacy, given that the principles for science literacy are so parallel to those for media literacy, and much learning about science takes place through news, entertainment and social media. Science literacy similarly emphasizes critical thinking and consideration of evidence provided and missing, consideration of multiple sources for information, and how data are collected and interpreted (Baker, Citation2020; McClune & Jarman, Citation2010; Ryder, Citation2001).

Our reliance on self-reports, however valuable for their perspective, nevertheless can include self-report bias and may reflect limitations resulting from participants’ abilities to understand and respond to survey questions. One limitation to the findings may be the readability of the questionnaire, which a Gunning-Fogg analysis indicated was approximately 8.2. Adults did help the youth answer the questionnaire but this questionnaire would still be generally above grade level for the youth participating the study. It will be important to continue to develop better tools for assessing youth perspectives.

Conclusion

Overall, the results of this family-centered test of a media literacy intervention for healthier food consumption suggest that a developmental progression exists from critical thinking about source to critical thinking about content that can be catalyzed through media literacy education, particularly to the extent that children are encouraged to discuss media messages (i.e. practice) with parents. Children who develop persuasion knowledge still need to practice it and be motivated to use it, and this can happen productively through family discussion and parental reinforcement that cultivates the child’s agency.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download PDF (337.7 KB)

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Supplemental data

Supplemental data for this article can be accessed online at https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2022.2160078

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute of Food and Agriculture, U.S. Department of Agriculture, Grant number 2012-68001-19618. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the view of the U.S. Department of Agriculture.

References

  • Austin, E. W., Austin, B. W., French, B. F., & Cohen, M. A. (2018). The effects of a nutrition media literacy intervention on parents’ and youths’ communication about food. Journal of Health Communication, 23(2), 190–199. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2018.1423649
  • Austin, E. W., Austin, B. W., & Kaiser, C. (2020a). Effects of family-centered media literacy training on family nutrition outcomes. Prevention Science, 21(3), 308–318. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-020-01101-x
  • Austin, E. W., Austin, B., Kaiser, C. K., Edwards, Z., Parker, L., & Power, T. G. (2020b). A media literacy-based nutrition program fosters parent–child food marketing discussions, improves home food environment, and youth consumption of fruits and vegetables. Childhood Obesity, 16(S1), S-33. https://doi.org/10.1089/chi.2019.0240
  • Austin, E. W., Borah, P., & Domgaard, S. (2021). COVID-19 disinformation and political engagement among communities of color: The role of media literacy. Harvard Kennedy School (HKS) Misinformation Review, 1, 7. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-58
  • Austin E. W., Cohen M., Deen M. K., Parker L., Power T., Barale K., Brandt B., Brown S., Cummins M., Fees J., Scanga L. H., Varrella G., Viebrock M., Kistler M., Johnson B., Adler C., Hua V., Austin B. W., Edwards Z., Kallman D., Kaiser C. K., & Stefani W. (2017). FoodMania! Curriculum Project. Pullman, WA: Washington State University. https://els2.comotion.uw.edu/product/foodmania-curriculum
  • Austin, E. W., & Johnson, K. K. (1997). Immediate and delayed effects of media literacy training on third grader’s decision making for alcohol. Health Communication, 9(4), 323–349. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327027hc0904_3
  • Austin, E. W., Miller, A. C. R., Silva, J., Guerra, P., Geisler, N., Gamboa, L., Phakakayai, O., & Kuechle, B. (2002). The effects of increased cognitive involvement on college students’ interpretations of magazine advertisements for alcohol. Communication Research, 29(2), 155–179. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650202029002003
  • Austin, E. W., Muldrow, A., & Austin, B. W. (2016). Examining how media literacy and personality factors predict skepticism toward alcohol advertising. Journal of Health Communication, 21(5), 600–609. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2016.1153761
  • Austin, E. W., Pinkleton, B. E., Chen, Y., & Austin, B. W. (2015). Processing of sexual media messages improves due to media literacy effects on perceived message desirability. Mass Communication and Society, 18(4), 399–421. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2014.1001909
  • Austin, E. W., Pinkleton, B. E., & Fujioka, Y. (2000). The role of interpretation processes and parental discussion in the media’s effects on adolescents’ use of alcohol. Pediatrics, 105(2), 343–349. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.105.2.343
  • Austin, E. W., Pinkleton, B. E., & Funabiki, R. P. (2007). The desirability paradox in the effects of media literacy training. Communication Research, 34(5), 483–506. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650207305233
  • Baker, F. (2020, March 26). Why media literacy matters in science class. Middle Web. https://www.middleweb.com/42576/why-media-literacy-matters-in-science-class/
  • Baxter, S. D., Smith, A. F., Litaker, M. S., Guinn, C. H., Shaffer, N. M., Baglio, M. L., & Frye, F. H. A. (2004). Recency affects reporting accuracy of children’s dietary recalls. Annals of Epidemiology, 14(6), 385–390. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2003.07.003
  • Boaz, A., Robert, G., Locock, L., Sturmey, G., Gager, M., Vougioukalou, S., Ziebland, S., Fielden, J., McDermottand, M., & Anne Reff Pedersen, A. (2016). What patients do and their impact on implementation: An ethnographic study of participatory quality improvement projects in English acute hospitals. Journal of Health Organization and Management, 30(2), 258–278. https://doi.org/10.1108/JHOM-02-2015-0027
  • Boonpleng, W., Park, C. G., Gallo, A. M., Corte, C., McCreary, L., & Bergren, M. D. (2013). Ecological influences of early childhood obesity: A multilevel analysis. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 35(6), 742–759. https://doi.org/10.1177/0193945913480275
  • Boyland, E. J., & Halford, J. C. (2013). Television advertising and branding. Effects on eating behaviour and food preferences in children. Appetite, 62, 236–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.032
  • Brown, A. L. (1997). Transforming schools into communities of thinking and learning about serious matters. The American Psychologist, 52(4), 399–413. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.4.399
  • Brown, A. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (1988). Metacognitive skills. In K. Richardson & S. Sheldon (Eds.), Cognitive development to adolescence: A reader (pp. 139–150). Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Buijzen, M. (2009). The effectiveness of parental communication in modifying the relation between food advertising and children’s consumption behaviour. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 105–121. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151008X334719
  • Calvert, S. L. (2008). Children as consumers: Advertising and marketing. The Future of Children, 18(1), 205–234. https://doi.org/10.1353/foc.0.0001
  • Campbell, A. (2008). For their own good: Recruiting children for research. Childhood, 15(1), 30–49. https://doi.org/10.1177/0907568207086834
  • Carter, O. B., Patterson, L. J., Donovan, R. J., Ewing, M. T., & Roberts, C. M. (2011). Children’s understanding of the selling versus persuasive intent of junk food advertising: Implications for regulation. Social Science & Medicine, 72(6), 962–968. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.01.018
  • Clarke, S. (2015). A “Child’s rights perspective”: The “right” of children and young people to participate in health care research. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 38(3), 161–180. https://doi.org/10.3109/01460862.2015.1042171
  • Crespo, C. J., Smit, E., Troiano, R. P., Bartlett, S. J., Macera, C. A., & Andersen, R. E. (2001). Television watching, energy intake, and obesity in US children: Results from the third national health and nutrition examination survey, 1988-1994. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 155(3), 360–365. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.155.3.360
  • Daly, W. (2009). “Adding their flavour to the mix”: Involving children and young people in care in research design. Australian Social Work, 62(4), 460–475. https://doi.org/10.1080/03124070903265732
  • Dumontheil, I. (2014). Development of abstract thinking during childhood and adolescence: The role of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 57–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.07.009
  • Duran, R. L., Yousman, B., Walsh, K. M., & Longshore, M. A. (2008). Holistic media education: An assessment of the effectiveness of a college course in media literacy. Communication Quarterly, 56(1), 49–68. https://doi.org/10.1080/01463370701839198
  • East, P., Delker, E., Blanco, E., Burrows, R., Lozoff, B., & Gahagan, S. (2019). Home and family environment related to development of obesity: A 21-year longitudinal study. Childhood Obesity, 15(3), 156–166. https://doi.org/10.1089/chi.2018.0222
  • Eccles, J. S., Buchanan, C. M., Flanagan, C., Fuligni, A., Midgley, C., & Yee, D. (1991). Control versus autonomy during early adolescence. The Journal of Social Issues, 47(4), 53–68. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1991.tb01834.x
  • Erikson, E. E. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. International Universities Press.
  • Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, NIH, DHHS. (2013). Media-smart youth upgraded: Eat, think, and be active facilitator’s packet. U.S. Government Printing Office.
  • Evans, A. E., Dave, J., Tanner, A., Duhe, S., Condrasky, M., Wilson, D., Griffin, S., Palmer, M., & Evans, M. (2006). Changing the home nutrition environment: Effects of a nutrition and media literacy pilot intervention. Family & Community Health, 29(1), 43–54. https://doi.org/10.1097/00003727-200601000-00006
  • Finney, S. J., & DiStefano, C. (2006). Non-normal and categorical data in structural equation modelling. In G. R. Hancock & R. O. Muller (Eds.), Structural equation modelling: A second course (pp. 269–314). Information Age Publishing.
  • Folta, S. C., Goldberg, J. P., Economos, C., Bell, R., & Meltzer, R. (2006). Food advertising targeted at school-age children: A content analysis. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 38(4), 244–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2006.04.146
  • Fujioka, Y., & Austin, E. W. (2003). The implications of vantage point in parental mediation of television and child’s attitudes toward drinking alcohol. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 47(3), 418–434. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4703_6
  • Gauffroy, C., & Barrouillet, P. (2011). The primacy of thinking about possibilities in the development of reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 100–1011. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023269
  • Goris, J. M., Petersen, S., Stamatakis, E., & Veerman, J. L. (2010). Television food advertising and the prevalence of childhood overweight and obesity: A multicountry comparison. Public Health Nutrition, 13(7), 1003–1012. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980009992850
  • Grohs, R., Wagner, U., & Steiner, R. (2012). An investigation of children’s ability to identify sponsors and understand sponsorship intentions. Psychology & Marketing, 29(11), 907–917. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20573
  • Harris, J. L., Brownell, K. D., & Bargh, J. A. (2009). The food marketing defense model: Integrating psychological research to protect youth and inform public policy. Social Issues and Policy Review, 3(1), 211–271. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-2409.2009.01015.x
  • Henry, H. K., & Borzekowski, D. L. (2011). The nag factor: A mixed-methodology study in the US of young children’s requests for advertised products. Journal of Children and Media, 5(3), 298–317. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2011.584380
  • Hill, M. (2006). Children’s voices on ways of having a voice: Children’s and young people’s perspectives on methods used in research and consultation. Childhood, 13(1), 69–89. https://doi.org/10.1177/0907568206059972
  • Hindin, T. J., Contento, I. R., & Gussow, J. D. (2004). A media literacy nutrition education curriculum for Head Start parents about the effects of television advertising on their children’s food requests. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 104(2), 192–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2003.11.006
  • Hingle, M. D., O’Connor, T. M., Dave, J. M., & Baranowski, T. (2010). Parental involvement in interventions to improve child dietary intake: A systematic review. Preventive Medicine, 51(2), 103–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.04.014
  • Hoelscher, D., Day, R., Kelder, S., & Ward, J. (2003). Reproducibility and validity of the secondary level school-based nutrition monitoring student questionnaire. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 103(2), 186–194. https://doi.org/10.1053/jada.2003.50031
  • Hoyle, R. H. (1995). Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications. Sage Publications.
  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
  • Hwang, Y., Yum, J. Y., & Jeong, S. H. (2018). What components should be included in advertising literacy education? Effect of component types and the moderating role of age. Journal of Advertising, 47(4), 347–361. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2018.1546628
  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). Growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence: An essay on the construction of formal operational structures. Basic Books. https://doi.org/10.1037/10034-000
  • Jacks, J. Z., & Devine, P. G. (2000). Attitude importance, forewarning of message content, and resistance to persuasion. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22(1), 19–29. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324834BASP2201_3
  • Jeong, S. H., Cho, H., & Hwang, Y. (2012). Media literacy interventions: A meta-analytic review. The Journal of Communication, 62(3), 454–472. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01643.x
  • Kaiser, I., Mayer, J., & Malai, D. (2018). Self-generation in the context of inquiry-based learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2440. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02440
  • Kitzman-Ulrich, H., Wilson, D., George, S., Lawman, H., Segal, M., & Fairchild, A. (2010). The integration of a family systems approach for understanding youth obesity, physical activity, and dietary programs. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13(3), 231–253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-010-0073-0
  • Koepke, S., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2012). Dynamics of identity development and separation-individuation in parent-child relationships during adolescence and emerging adulthood—a conceptual integration. Developmental Review, 32(1), 67–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.01.001
  • Kolodziejczyk, J. K., Merchant, G., & Norman, G. J. (2012). Reliability and validity of child/adolescent food frequency questionnaires that assess foods and/or food groups. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 55(1), 4–13. https://doi.org/10.1097/MPG.0b013e318251550e
  • Koonce, J. (2017). The roles of digital literacies and critical literacy for black adolescent females. Journal of Literacy and Technology, 18(1), 80–104. http://www.literacyandtechnology.org/uploads/1/3/6/8/136889/jlt_v18_1_koonce.pdf
  • Kupersmidt, J. B., Scull, T. M., & Austin, E. W. (2010). Media literacy education for elementary school substance use prevention: Study of media detective. Pediatrics, 126(3), 525–531. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-0068
  • Lachat, C., Nago, E., Verstraeten, R., Roberfroid, D., Van Camp, J., & Kolsteren, P. (2012). Eating out of home and its association with dietary intake: A systematic review of the evidence. Obesity Reviews, 13(4), 329–346. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00953.x
  • Lagattuta, K. H., Nucci, L., & Bosacki, S. L. (2010). Bridging theory of mind and the personal domain: Children’s reasoning about resistance to parental control. Child Development, 81(2), 616–635. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01419.x
  • Lam, C. B., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C. (2014). Time with peers from middle childhood to late adolescence: Developmental course and adjustment correlates. Child Development, 85(4), 1677–1693. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12235
  • Lapierre, M. A. (2015). Development and persuasion understanding: Predicting knowledge of persuasion/selling intent from children’s theory of mind. The Journal of Communication, 65(3), 423–442. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12155
  • Lapierre, M. A. (2019). Advertising literacy and executive function: Testing their influence on children’s consumer behavior. Media Psychology, 22(1), 39–59. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2017.1345638
  • Larsson, I., Staland-Nyman, C., Svedberg, P., Nygren, J. M., & Carlsson, M. (2018). Children and young people’s participation in developing interventions in health and well-being: A scoping review. BMC Health Services Research, 18(1), 507. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-018-3219-2
  • Levy, R., & Choi, J. (2013). Bayesian structural equation modeling. In G. R. Hancock & R. O. Muller (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: A second course (2nd ed., pp. 563–623). Information Age Publishing.
  • Linn, S., & Novosat, C. L. (2008). Calories for sale: Food marketing to children in the twenty-first century. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 615(1), 133–155. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716207308487
  • Little, T. D., Preacher, K. J., Selig, J. P., & Card, N. A. (2007). New developments in latent variable panel analyses of longitudinal data. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31(4), 357–365. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025407077757
  • MacKinnon, D. (2012). Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. Routledge.
  • Maniccia, D. M., Davison, K. K., Marshall, S. J., Manganello, J. A., & Dennison, B. A. (2011). A meta-analysis of interventions that target children’s screen time for reduction. Pediatrics, 128(1), e193–210. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-2353
  • Marsh, S., Mhurchu, C. N., & Maddison, R. (2013). The non-advertising effects of screen-based sedentary activities on acute eating behaviours in children, adolescents, and young adults. A systematic review. Appetite, 71, 259–273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.017
  • McClune, B., & Jarman, R. (2010). Critical reading of science‐based news reports: Establishing a knowledge, skills and attitudes framework. International Journal of Science Education, 32(6), 727–752. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500690902777402
  • McKinnon, R. A., Reedy, J., Berrigan, D., & Krebs-Smith, S. M. (2012). The National Collaborative on Childhood Obesity Research catalogue of surveillance systems and measures registry. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 42(4), 433–435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2012.01.004
  • Media Literacy Now. (2020). U.S. media literacy policy report 2020. https://medialiteracynow.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/01/U.S.-Media-Literacy-Policy-Report-2020.pdf
  • Mills, C. M., & Keil, F. C. (2004). Knowing the limit of one’s understanding: The development of an awareness of an illusion of explanatory depth. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(1), 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2003.09.003
  • Minges, K. E., Owen, N., Salmon, J., Chao, A., Dunstan, D. W., & Whittemore, R. (2015). Reducing youth screen time: Qualitative metasynthesis of findings on barriers and facilitators. Health Psychology, 34(4), 381. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000172
  • Molgaard, V., & Spoth, R. (2001). The strengthening families program for young adolescents: Overview and outcomes. Residential Treatment for Children & Youth, 18(3), 15–29. https://doi.org/10.1300/J007v18n03_03
  • Moses, L. J., & Baldwin, D. A. (2005). What can the study of cognitive development reveal about children’s ability to appreciate and cope with advertising? Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 24(2), 186–201. https://doi.org/10.1509/jppm.2005.24.2.186
  • Moules, T., & O’Brien, N. (2012). Participation in perspective: Reflections from research projects. Nurse Researcher, 19(2), 17–22. https://doi.org/10.7748/nr2012.01.19.2.17.c8904
  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.
  • Muthén, B. O., & Satorra, A. (1995). Complex sample data in structural equation modeling. Sociological Methodology, 25, 267–316. https://doi.org/10.2307/271070
  • Nathanson, A. I. (2004). Factual and evaluative approaches to modifying children’s responses to violent television. The Journal of Communication, 54(2), 321–336. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2004.tb02631.x
  • National Center for Education Statistics. (2000). The National Assessment of College Student Learning: Identification of the skills to be taught, learned, and assessed (A report on the proceedings of the study design workshop. Research and development report). U.S. Government Printing Office.
  • Olson, S., Parker, L., & Breiner, H. (Eds.). (2013). Challenges and opportunities for change in food marketing to children and youth: Workshop summary. National Academies Press.
  • Penkilo, M., George, G. C., & Hoelscher, D. M. (2008). Reproducibility of the school-based nutrition monitoring questionnaire among fourth-grade students in Texas. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 40(1), 20–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2007.04.375
  • Pinkleton, B. E., Austin, E. W., Chen, Y., & Cohen, M. (2013). Assessing effects of a media literacy-based intervention on U.S. adolescents’ responses to and interpretations of sexual media messages. Journal of Children and Media, 7, 1748–2801. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2013.781512
  • Poti, J. M., & Popkin, B. M. (2011). Trends in energy intake among US children by eating location and food source, 1977–2006. Journal of the American Dietary Association, 111(8), 1156–1164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2011.05.007
  • Powell, L. M., Schermbeck, R. M., & Chaloupka, F. J. (2013). Nutritional content of food and beverage products in television advertisements seen on children’s programming. Childhood Obesity, 9(6), 524–531. https://doi.org/10.1089/chi.2013.0072
  • Powell, M. A., & Smith, A. B. (2009). Children’s participation rights in research. Childhood, 16(1), 124–142. https://doi.org/10.1177/0907568208101694
  • Ragelienė, T., & Grønhøj, A. (2020). The influence of peers′ and siblings′ on children’s and adolescents′ healthy eating behavior. A systematic literature review. Appetite, 148, 104592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2020.104592
  • Reicks, M., Davey, C., Anderson, A. K., Banna, J., Cluskey, M., Gunther, C., Jones, B., Richards, R., Topham, G., & Wong, S. S. (2019). Frequency of eating alone is associated with adolescent dietary intake, perceived food-related parenting practices and weight status: Cross-sectional Family Life, Activity, Sun, Health, and Eating (FLASHE) study results. Public Health Nutrition, 22(9), 1555–1556. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980019000107
  • Rhee, K. E. (2008). Childhood overweight and the relationship between parent behaviors, parenting style, and family functioning. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 615(1), 11–37. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716207308400
  • Rozendaal, E., Buijzen, M., & Valkenburg, P. (2010). Comparing children’s and adults’ cognitive advertising competences in the Netherlands. Journal of Children and Media, 4(1), 77–89. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482790903407333
  • Rozendaal, E., Lapierre, M. A., van Reijmersdal, E. A., & Buijzen, M. (2011). Reconsidering advertising literacy as a defense against advertising effects. Media Psychology, 14(4), 333–354. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2011.620540
  • Ryder, J. (2001). Identifying science understanding for functional scientific literacy. Studies in Science Education, 36(1), 1–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057260108560166
  • Sagarin, B. J., Cialdini, R. B., Rice, W. E., & Serna, S. B. (2002). Dispelling the illusion of invulnerability: The motivations and mechanisms of resistance to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(3), 526. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.3.526
  • Salsberg, J., Parry, D., Pluye, P., Macridis, S., Herbert, C. P., & Macaulay, A. C. (2015). Successful strategies to engage research partners for translating evidence into action in community health: A critical review. Journal of Environmental and Public Health, 2015, 191856. https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/191856
  • Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In A. von Eye & C. C. Clogg (Eds.), Latent variables analysis: Applications for developmental research (pp. 399–419). Sage Publications.
  • Shier, V., Nicosia, N., & Datar, A. (2016). Neighborhood and home food environment and children’s diet and obesity: Evidence from military personnel’s installation assignment. Social Science & Medicine, 158, 122–131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.03.043
  • Smetana, J. G., & Asquith, P. (1994). Adolescents’ and parents’ conceptions of parental authority and personal autonomy. Child Development, 65(4), 1147–1162. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131311
  • Smith, J. D., Montano, Z., Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D. S., & Wilson, M. N. (2015). Preventing weight gain and obesity: Indirect effects of the family check-up in early childhood. Prevention Science, 16(3), 408–419. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-014-0505-z
  • Springer, A. E., Kelder, S. H., Byrd-Williams, C. E., Pasch, K. E., Ranjit, N., Delk, J. E., & Hoelscher, D. M. (2013). Promoting energy-balance behaviors among ethnically diverse adolescents: Overview and baseline findings of the Central Texas Catch middle school project. Health Education & Behavior, 40(5), 559–570. https://doi.org/10.1177/1090198112459516
  • Steinberg, L., & Silk, J. S. (2002). Parenting adolescents. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 103–134). Erlbaum.
  • Strasburger, V. C. (2011). Children, adolescents, obesity, and the media. Pediatrics, 128(1), 201–208. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1066
  • Tahir, M. J., Willett, W., & Forman, M. R. (2018). The association of television viewing in childhood with overweight and obesity throughout the life course. American Journal of Epidemiology, 188(2), 282–293. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwy236
  • Unicef. (1989). Convention on the rights of the child. https://www.unicef.org/child-rights-convention
  • van de Schoot, R., Broere, J. J., Perryck, K. H., Zondervan Zwijnenburg, M., & Van Loey, N. E. (2015). Analyzing small data sets using Bayesian estimation: The case of posttraumatic stress symptoms following mechanical ventilation in burn survivors. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 6(1), 25216. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v6.25216
  • Weil, L. G., Fleming, S. M., Dumontheil, I., Kilford, E. J., Weil, R. S., Rees, G., Dolan, R. J., & Blakemore, S. (2013). The development of metacognitive ability in adolescence. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(1), 264–271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.01.004
  • Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17(2), 89–100. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00381.x
  • Wootan, M. G., Almy, J., Ugalde, M., & Kaminski, M. (2019). How do nutrition guidelines compare for industry to market food and beverage products to children? World Health Organization nutrient profile standards versus the U.S. children’s food and beverage advertising initiative. Childhood Obesity, 15(3), 194–199. https://doi.org/10.1089/chi.2018.0256
  • Xie, X., Gai, X., & Zhou, Y. (2019). A meta-analysis of media literacy interventions for deviant behaviors. Computers & Education, 139, 146–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2019.05.008
  • Yee, A. Z., Lwin, M. O., & Ho, S. S. (2017). The influence of parental practices on child promotive and preventive food consumption behaviors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 14(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0501-3
  • Zimmerman, F. J., & Bell, J. F. (2010). Associations of television content type and obesity in children. American Journal of Public Health, 100(2), 334–340. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2008.155119
  • Zyphur, M. J., & Oswald, F. L. (2015). Bayesian estimation and inference: A user’s guide. Journal of Management, 41(2), 390–420. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206313501200