529
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
ARTICLES

Charitable Giving: Even the Willing Need to be Persuaded

, &
Pages 115-127 | Published online: 18 May 2012
 

Abstract

Nonprofits that depend on charitable giving operate in a competitive context and cannot simply expect to receive donations because they represent a “good” or “deserving” cause. To increase the chances of receiving donations, they have to communicate the positive impact of the donor's actions (perceived efficacy) and ensure that the appeals are seen as motivating (persuasiveness). We sampled donors from the database of a local charity and found that perceived efficacy for the charity was significantly influential. We also found that persuasiveness of an appeal fully mediated their charitable giving behavior. Thus, to receive a fair share, or any share, of an individual's donations, even the willing need to be persuaded.

Acknowledgments

This article was accepted by Claude Martin and James Leigh, previous editors of the Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising.

The authors thank Deepshika Bijjula and Julianne Wickland for help in designing and administering the survey and collating data and the Amherst Survival Center for access to their database.

Notes

Note. Adj. R 2 = .42, F 3, 278 = 67.64.

a Appeal was coded as a dummy variable and guilt and altruism appeals were combined.

b VIF represents the variance inflation factor, where values above 10 indicate multicollinearity.

1ANOVA analyses showed that these appeals were not significantly different in their persuasiveness (F2, 279 = 1.61, p>.3), perceived information content (F2, 274 = 1.44, p>.24), and readability (F2, 274 = 2.76, p>.07). Therefore, we collapsed all of our data and restricted our attention to the other remaining research questions. However, to be consistent with our a priori theoretical prediction, we entered Appeal as the first independent variable in the hierarchical regression.

Note. Sig., significance.

−2 log-likelihood (0) = 194.08.

−2 log-likelihood (β) = –159.9.

Nagelkerke R 2 = .16, adjusted ρ2 = .21.

Note. Sig., significance.

−2 log-likelihood (0) = –194.08.

−2 log-likelihood (β) = –166.14.

Nagelkerke R 2 = .12, adjusted ρ2 = .15.

Note. Sig., significance.

−2 log-likelihood (0) = -194.08.

−2 log-likelihood (β) = -156.13.

Nagelkerke R 2 = .19, adjusted ρ2 = .22.

Log in via your institution

Log in to Taylor & Francis Online

PDF download + Online access

  • 48 hours access to article PDF & online version
  • Article PDF can be downloaded
  • Article PDF can be printed
USD 53.00 Add to cart

Issue Purchase

  • 30 days online access to complete issue
  • Article PDFs can be downloaded
  • Article PDFs can be printed
USD 152.00 Add to cart

* Local tax will be added as applicable

Related Research

People also read lists articles that other readers of this article have read.

Recommended articles lists articles that we recommend and is powered by our AI driven recommendation engine.

Cited by lists all citing articles based on Crossref citations.
Articles with the Crossref icon will open in a new tab.