698
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Loneliness and objective social isolation are differentially associated with anomalous perceptions in community-dwelling older adults

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 130-146 | Received 19 Apr 2022, Accepted 11 Nov 2022, Published online: 06 Feb 2023

ABSTRACT

Introduction

Anomalous perceptions are characterised by the subjective experience of a range of distorted and/or hallucinatory percepts. Whilst considerable attention has been paid to the neurocognitive processes contributing to anomalous perceptions amongst older adults, less is known about the social factors (e.g. social isolation, loneliness). Furthermore, it is unknown whether loneliness and social isolation are associated with different types of anomalous perceptions, including anomalous body-centred self-experiences and anomalous external experiences.

Methods

This study examined the cross-sectional relationships between loneliness, objective social isolation, and anomalous perceptions in a sample of community-dwelling older adults (N = 242, Mage = 71.87 ± 7.73, range = 52-91, 67.8% female) using structural equation modelling.

Results

Higher levels of loneliness were associated with more anomalous body-centred self-experiences and anomalous external experiences. Those reporting more loneliness also reported higher levels of anxiety and depression; however, the relationship between loneliness and anomalous perceptions was not mediated by these factors. Social disconnection from a religious group was associated with more anomalous external experiences and being married/living with a partner was associated with more anomalous body-centred self-experiences.

Conclusions

These findings suggest that loneliness and social isolation have differential associations with anomalous perceptions in older adults and provide additional evidence that attending to loneliness in older adults is important.

Anomalous perceptions refer to a broad range of perceptual distortions (e.g. changes in perceived size) and/or hallucinatory experiences (e.g. “hearing” voices that other people cannot) in any sensory modality (Bell et al., Citation2006). Although hallucinations represent one example of the general category, most research has focused on hallucinations specifically, rather than seeking to understand the phenomenology and potential mechanisms of anomalous perceptions more broadly. Additionally, while hallucinations are relatively common in the general population (Badcock et al., Citation2017), most studies have investigated anomalous and hallucinatory experiences in younger populations or in older adults with clinical disorders (e.g. Parkinson’s disease). Thus, knowledge of the mechanisms underlying anomalous perceptions in community-dwelling older adults (without psychotic disorders/dementia) remains limited.

While some anomalous perceptions appear of little clinical importance, some cause the individual and/or their family considerable distress. Indeed, amongst older adults, anomalous perceptions, including hallucinations, are associated with increased risk of cognitive decline (Gasca-Salas et al., Citation2016), residential care placement (Scarmeas et al., Citation2005) and caregiver stress (Chiu et al., Citation2017). In the context of global population ageing, these findings highlight the need to investigate the potential correlates of anomalous perceptions in older adults. Perceptual anomalies may even offer insights into the development of mental health problems in older adults, and improved assessment and intervention strategies.

Using the Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale (CAPS; Bell et al., Citation2006), Kelsall-Foreman et al. (Citation2020) showed that anomalous perceptions in community-dwelling older and younger adults comprise two factors: anomalous body-centred self-experiences (alterations in body, touch, smell, and taste perception; Factor 1), and anomalous external experiences (auditory, visual, and sensed presence hallucinations; Factor 2). These factors describe experiences with potentially different causes and consequences. Whilst the neurocognitive processes related to anomalous perceptual experiences amongst older adults have been studied, less is known about social factors that may be associated with anomalous perceptions, such as social isolation and loneliness.

Objective social isolation and loneliness are related, but separable constructs (Menec et al., Citation2019). Social isolation is a quantitative measure of the paucity of an individual’s social network and contact with others, often indexed by marital status, social network size, and frequency of contacts (Badcock et al., Citation2022). In contrast, loneliness occurs when an individual perceives their social relationships to be fewer in quality and quantity than desired (Hawkley, Citation2018). Individuals may be objectively socially isolated but not lonely, whereas others may have many social connections but feel lonely (Coyle & Dugan, Citation2012). Consequently, examining both social isolation and loneliness is recommended to characterise an individual’s overall social context (Newall & Menec, Citation2019), and because these experiences may be related.

Examining both allows researchers to investigate their unique and combined ability to predict anomalous perceptions, independently of potential confounding factors. In particular, age and sex have been linked to variations in the experience of anomalous perceptions (Östling et al., Citation2013; Scott et al., Citation2008), loneliness (van den Broek, Citation2017), and social isolation (Vandervoort, Citation2000), though not consistently. Furthermore, increased levels of depression and anxiety are common consequences of both (Beutel et al., Citation2017; Santini et al., Citation2020), whilst cognitive models outline a direct role of emotion in the onset of hallucinations (Paulik et al., Citation2006). It is important, therefore, to consider affective states as potential mediators between an individual’s social context and their experience of anomalous perceptions.

A much-cited account of the induction of hallucinations is the social deafferentation (SDA) hypothesis (Hoffman, Citation2007, Citation2008), which states that “high levels of social withdrawal/isolation in vulnerable individuals prompt social cognitive programmes to produce spurious social meaning in the form of complex, emotionally compelling hallucinations and delusions representing other persons or agents” (Hoffman, Citation2007, p. 1066). The SDA hypothesis was proposed to explain hallucinations in schizophrenia; however, it has not been explored much in non-clinical, older adult samples, nor does it distinguish between the roles of social isolation and loneliness. One study (El Haj et al., Citation2016), found that social isolation significantly predicted hallucinations in both older adults with Alzheimer’s disease and age-matched healthy controls, despite controlling for loneliness. This suggests that objective (rather than subjective) social isolation is associated with the production of anomalous perceptions, though whether this includes one or both factors of anomalous perceptions identified by Kelsall-Foreman et al. (Citation2020) was not explored.

Given the social nature of the hallucinatory experiences described in the SDA hypothesis, it may be that social isolation is associated with anomalous perceptions involving other people or agents (termed anomalous external perceptions [Factor 2]; Kelsall-Foreman et al., Citation2020), whilst a relationship with anomalous body-centred self-experiences (Factor 1; Kelsall-Foreman et al., Citation2020) is less clear. On the other hand, in an extension of the SDA hypothesis in a sample of individuals with schizophrenia and matched controls, Michael and Park (Citation2016) found high levels of loneliness were associated with increased risk of experiencing spurious anomalous bodily experiences, leaving open the possibility that loneliness is also associated with anomalous body-centred self-experiences (Kelsall-Foreman et al.’s [Citation2020] Factor 1).

This study examined cross-sectional relationships between loneliness, objective social isolation, and anomalous perceptions in community-dwelling older adults, hypothesising that (H1) higher levels of loneliness would relate to higher scores on a latent variable measuring anomalous body-centred self-experiences (Michael & Park, Citation2016), and (H2) higher levels of objective social isolation would relate to higher scores on a latent variable measuring anomalous external experiences (Hoffman, Citation2007, Citation2008), after adjusting for age and sex. Novel to this study, we also conducted exploratory analyses examining the relationships between loneliness and anomalous external experiences, and between social isolation and anomalous body-centred self-experiences. A secondary aim considered whether anxiety and depression mediated any relationships found.

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited through the Healthy Ageing Research Programme (HARP) which is a longitudinal study of community-dwelling adults aged 50 + years that aims to investigate age-related changes in neuropsychological functioning and behaviour. Participants within the longitudinal study were initially recruited through several different avenues, including local bulletin boards and newspapers, community presentations, online via social media, and through personal connections. Given the focus on examining anomalous perceptions in older adults without psychotic disorders or dementia, participants with evidence of cognitive impairment (<18 on the telephone adapted version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment [T-MoCA; Pendlebury et al., Citation2013, n = 15] or <24 on the Folstein Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE; Folstein et al., Citation1975; O’Bryant et al., Citation2008, n = 2]), history of neurological (e.g. stroke, Parkinson’s disease, n = 20) and/or psychiatric conditions associated with anomalous and hallucinatory experiences (e.g. schizophrenia, post-traumatic stress disorder, n = 13) were excluded. Those who declined cognitive screening (n = 18) or provided no data for key measures (n = 6) were excluded, leaving 242 participants (range 52–91 years). Approval was provided by the Human Research Ethics Committee, University of Western Australia (RA/4/1/5361).

Materials

The UCLA Loneliness Scale-3 (UCLA-LS3; Russell, Citation1996) contains 20-items assessing feelings of loneliness, each rated from 1 (“never”) to 4 (“always”). Responses are summed (range 20-80), with higher scores indicating greater loneliness. The UCLA-LS3 has excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .89-.94) and validity (Russell, Citation1996).

Objective social isolation was measured using the Brief Social Activity Index (BSAI; Hawkley et al., Citation2005), which summarises an individual’s social contacts across a range of social domains, with good construct validity in older adults (Hawkley et al., Citation2005). The 4 BSAI items examine: (1) whether the individual is married/living with a partner (“yes” = 1, “no”  = 0), (2) number of friends/relatives spoken to every 2 weeks (≥2 = 1, ≤ 2 = 0), (3) membership of a religious group/church (“yes” = 1, “no” = 0), and (4) membership in social/sporting/neighbourhood groups (≥2 = 1, ≤ 2 = 0). Responses are summed (range 0-4), with lower scores indicating greater social isolation. In this sample, the BSAI total score had poor internal consistency (McDonald’s Omega total [ωt] = .38). Consequently, each BSAI item was modelled simultaneously.

The CAPS (Bell et al., Citation2006) has 32 items assessing the tendency to experience perceptual anomalies across five sensory modalities, tapping changes in sensory intensity/distortion of perceptual input, and hallucinatory experiences. Participants answer with “yes” or “no”. The total CAPS score is the number of items endorsed “yes” (range 0-32)Footnote1, with higher scores indicating more anomalous perceptions. The CAPS total score has good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .87; Bell et al., Citation2006) and convergent validity (Jaén-Moreno et al., Citation2014). The current study utilised the two-factor (23-item) form of the CAPS identified by Kelsall-Foreman et al. (Citation2020). The first factor is labelled “anomalous body-centred self-experiences” and encompasses items related to alterations in body, touch, smell, and taste perception (CAPS items 8, 9, 14, 17, 18, 20, 21, 25, 29, and 30). One example item from this factor is CAPS Q8 which asks “Do you ever detect smells which don’t seem to come from your surroundings?”. The second factor is labelled “anomalous external experiences” and encompasses items related to auditory, visual, and sensed presence hallucinations (CAPS items 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 15, 22, 23, 27, 31, and 32). One example item from this factor is CAPS Q2 which asks “Do you ever sense the presence of another being, despite being unable to see any evidence?”.

The T-MoCA (Pendlebury et al., Citation2013) is a brief (10-15mins), valid, and reliable measure of cognitive status. Scores range from 0 to 22, where a cut-off of <18 indicates mild cognitive impairment (Pendlebury et al., Citation2013).

The Folstein MMSE (Folstein et al., Citation1975) is a brief (5-10mins), well-established instrument assessing overall cognitive status (Folstein et al., Citation1975). Scores range from 0 to 30, with a recommended cut-off score to detect cognitive impairment of <24 (O’Bryant et al., Citation2008).

The Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9; Kroenke et al., Citation2001) contains 9 items examining depressive symptoms over the last 2 weeks, with good validity in older adults (Costa et al., Citation2016). Higher scores indicate greater levels of depressive symptoms (range 0-27).

The Generalised Anxiety Disorder Scale-7 (GAD-7; Spitzer et al., Citation2006) has 7 items examining generalised anxiety symptom severity over the last 2 weeks, valid in clinical and research settings (Spitzer et al., Citation2006). Higher scores indicate greater levels of anxiety symptoms (range 0-21).

Procedure

Participants completed the questionnaires online or by post and were informed that return of completed questionnaires indicated consent to participate. The questionnaires took approximately one hour. Within 4–6 weeks of questionnaire completion, participants completed the T-MoCA or MMSE with a trained assessor.

Data analysis

Descriptive analyses were conducted in SPSS (IBM Corp, released Citation2017) and JASP (JASP Team, Citation2019). The GAD-7 and PHQ-9 each had one missing value (0.059% and 0.046% missing values, respectively), so Little’s MCAR was not calculated for these data. Little’s (Citation1988) MCAR test was used to examine missing values for the UCLA-LS3 (0.124% missing values), BSAI (2.789%), and CAPS (0.181%). Data from the UCLA-LS3 and BSAI were missing completely at random (MCAR; Little’s MCAR p = .540 and p = .866, respectively). Given the very small proportions of missing values in all measures (<5%), expectation maximisation (25 iterations) was used to replace values in the BSAI, GAD-7, UCLA-LS3, and PHQ-9. The CAPS was not MCAR (p < .001), though imputation would not have been appropriate because of the categorical nature of the data. Because factor analysis using maximum likelihood can handle cases with missing values (0.181% for CAPS), all cases were included in the analyses. The internal consistency of the UCLA-LS3, BSAI, CAPS, PHQ-9, and GAD-7 were calculated using ωt, with values > .70 considered adequate (Nunnally, Citation1978).

First, we used Mplus version 8.0 (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2017) to test whether the original 2-factor CAPS model identified by Kelsall-Foreman et al. (Citation2020) gave good fit to the current data. Next, structural equation modelling was conducted to test associations between loneliness, social isolation, anomalous body-centred self-experiences (Factor 1), and anomalous external experiences (Factor 2). Lastly, depression and anxiety were examined as mediators in the relationships between loneliness, social isolation, and perceptual anomalies. Drawing on prior evidence, sex and age were included as covariates in all models.

A robust weighted least-squares mean and variance estimator was used given the categorical nature of CAPS data (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2017). Fit statistics were: chi-square (χ2), standardised root mean square residual (SRMR; Jöreskog & Sörbom, Citation1998), comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, Citation1990), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI; Tucker & Lewis, Citation1973), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Steiger, Citation1990). Recommended cut-offs for reasonable model fit to the data were: SRMR .06-.08, CFI .90-.95, TLI .90-.95, and RMSEA .05-.08 (Byrne, Citation2001; Hu & Bentler, Citation1999; Tabachnick & Fidell, Citation2001), and for good model fit were: SRMR < .06, CFI ≥ .95, TLI ≥ .95, and RMSEA < .05 (Hu & Bentler, Citation1999; Tabachnick & Fidell, Citation2001; Yu, Citation2002).

Results

Descriptive statistics

reports sample demographics. reports means, SDs, ranges, and ωt for the UCLA-LS3, CAPS Factor 1 and 2, PHQ-9 and GAD-7: 58.7% of participants reported at least one anomalous perceptual experience (see )Footnote2, 16.1% reported a PHQ-9 score above the recommended cut-off of 5 (Pellas & Damberg, Citation2021), while 10.7% of participants reported a GAD-7 score above the recommended cut-off of 5 (Wild et al., Citation2014).Footnote3

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample (N = 242).

Table 2. Characteristics of the UCLA-LS3, BSAI, CAPS, PHQ-9, and GAD-7 (N = 242).

Table 3. Frequency counts for total number of CAPS items endorsed (N = 242).

provides fit statistics for all models. Of the 242 participants, CAPS data from 194 (80%) were also used in Kelsall-Foreman et al. (Citation2020). Model fit was adequate to good; in particular, the RMSEA revealed good fit. However, CAPS Item 21 had a negative residual variance. The residual variance of a categorical item is not a model parameter and thus cannot be fixed to zero (Muthén, Citation2013), so this item was removed from subsequent analyses.

Table 4. Structural equation models of standardised path coefficients between CAPS factors, loneliness (UCLA-LS3), objective social isolation (BSAI), anxiety (GAD-7), and depression (PHQ-9), covarying for age and sex, with fit statistics (N = 242).

In Model 1 (), higher levels of loneliness were associated with higher levels of anomalous body-centred self-experiencesFootnote4 and higher levels of anomalous external experiences. When examining social isolation, being married/living with a partner was associated with higher levels of anomalous body-centred self-experiences and being a member of a religious group was associated with lower levels of anomalous external experiences. Lower levels of loneliness were associated with being married or living with a partner, and with seeing a greater number of friends/relatives every 2 weeks. Seeing a greater number of friends/relatives every 2 weeks was associated with membership in more social/sporting/neighbourhood groups.

Figure 1. Structural equation model examining direct relationships between loneliness (UCLA-LS3), objective social isolation (BSAI items 1-4), anomalous body-centred self-experiences (Self [F1]) and anomalous external experiences (External [F2]), covarying for age and sex.

Note. Dotted lines indicate non-significant pathways, and solid lines indicate significant pathways. Squares = observed variables, circles = latent variables.

Figure 1. Structural equation model examining direct relationships between loneliness (UCLA-LS3), objective social isolation (BSAI items 1-4), anomalous body-centred self-experiences (Self [F1]) and anomalous external experiences (External [F2]), covarying for age and sex.Note. Dotted lines indicate non-significant pathways, and solid lines indicate significant pathways. Squares = observed variables, circles = latent variables.

Males were more likely to be older. Older and female participants were less likely to be married/living with a partner, and older participants were more likely to see more friends/relative every 2 weeks and less likely to have membership in social/sporting/neighbourhood groups.

Anxiety and depression were significantly associated (r = .64, p < .001), as was loneliness with both anxiety (r = .34, p < .001) and depression (r = .49, p < .001). However, only the social isolation item assessing the number of friends/relatives spoken to every 2 weeks was significantly related to depression (r = -.19, p < .001), but not to CAPS Factor 1 or 2, meaning it was not appropriate to test for mediation using the social isolation items. Consequently, the mediation analyses focused on the mediating effect of depression and anxiety between loneliness and anomalous perceptions.

Higher levels of loneliness were associated with higher levels of anomalous body-centred self-experiences and higher levels of anomalous external experiences (). Higher levels of loneliness were also related to higher levels of anxiety and depression. However, anxiety and depression were not associated with anomalous experiences of any kind, meaning there was no mediating effect between loneliness and anomalous perceptual experiences, via anxiety or depression.

Figure 2. Structural equation model examining direct and indirect relationships between loneliness (UCLA-LS3), anxiety (GAD-7), depression (PHQ-9), anomalous body-centred self-experiences (Self [F1]), and anomalous external experiences (External [F2]), covarying for age and sex.

Note. Dotted lines indicate non-significant pathways, and solid lines indicate significant pathways. Squares = observed variables, circles = latent variables.

Figure 2. Structural equation model examining direct and indirect relationships between loneliness (UCLA-LS3), anxiety (GAD-7), depression (PHQ-9), anomalous body-centred self-experiences (Self [F1]), and anomalous external experiences (External [F2]), covarying for age and sex.Note. Dotted lines indicate non-significant pathways, and solid lines indicate significant pathways. Squares = observed variables, circles = latent variables.

Discussion

This study examined the cross-sectional relationships between objective social isolation, loneliness, and anomalous perceptions in community-dwelling older adults, after adjusting for age and sex, and considered whether anxiety and depression mediated any relationships found.

As hypothesised (H1), those who reported higher levels of loneliness reported more anomalous body-centred self-experiences. This finding builds on previous evidence by Michael and Park (Citation2016), who found that high levels of loneliness were associated with increased risk of spurious anomalous bodily experiences. In addition, our exploratory analyses showed that higher levels of loneliness are also related to more anomalous external experiences. There are several potential explanations for these findings. Loneliness may influence each dimension of perceptual anomalies via separate etiological pathways, though a more parsimonious interpretation is that loneliness may exert its effects on anomalous perceptions via a common mechanism: albeit, the nature of this mechanism is unclear, warranting investigation. Together, these findings demonstrate that loneliness is associated with an increase in a broad range of anomalous perceptions, though given the cross-sectional nature of the current study, the direction of these relationships remains unclear. For example, research shows that anomalous perceptions such as hallucinations can lead to loneliness (e.g. Michalska Da Rocha et al., Citation2018), which leaves open the possibility that anomalous perceptions may have indeed prompted increased levels of loneliness within the current study. Given the potential for reciprocal relationships between anomalous perceptual experiences and loneliness, future longitudinal studies will be important in determining causality.

These findings have important theoretical implications for the SDA hypothesis (Hoffman, Citation2007, Citation2008). First, this hypothesis proposes that social isolation is a direct, biologically-based precursor of hallucinations derived from a socially-wired brain that reorganises in the absence of social stimulation, in turn prompting anomalous perceptions. This hypothesis draws on prior sensory deafferentation literature (e.g. Menon et al., Citation2003) which posits that limited sensory input results in a compensatory increase in neural activation, but does not explicitly address the distinction between the role of objective and subjective social isolation (loneliness) in these experiences. Building on the SDA hypothesis, the current findings indicate that loneliness may also be related to the experience of anomalous perceptions, though, as noted above, the direction of this relationship is unclear given the cross-sectional nature of this study. Second, the SDA hypothesis assumes that “high levels” of social isolation are required to prompt hallucinatory experiences. The mean UCLA-LS3 score in this sample (36.72) was comparable to—or slightly lower than—previous data from healthy older adults (40.08; Anderson, Citation2010); only .03% of our participants would be classified as having “very high” levels of loneliness, with 18.6% having “moderately high” levels of loneliness.Footnote5 This implies that anomalous perceptions are particularly sensitive to feelings of loneliness (or vice versa), in that even relatively modest elevations of loneliness are related to the experience of anomalous perceptions, at least in older adults within the general population. This is consistent with a large body of literature that recognises loneliness as a major social stressor which can affect a broad range of emotional and cognitive outcomes (Lim et al., Citation2020). Lastly, though originally proposed to account for the induction of complex auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia, the current findings show that the SDA hypothesis may also be useful in explaining the onset of a broader suite of perceptual anomalies in community-dwelling older adults. This observation is important in view of recent evidence that multimodal hallucinations are a potential risk factor for the development of clinically relevant symptoms (Laloyaux et al., Citation2019).

Consistent with Beutel et al. (Citation2017), those reporting more loneliness also reported more anxiety and depression. However, the relationship between loneliness and anomalous perceptual experiences was not mediated by anxiety or depression. One possible explanation is that, overall, participants reported few symptoms of anxiety and depression: the recommended cut-off to detect significant anxiety and depression in older adults using the GAD-7 and PHQ-9 is 5 (Pellas & Damberg, Citation2021; Wild et al., Citation2014), yet the mean PHQ-9 and GAD-7 scores were 2.34 and 1.62, respectively, with few of the current sample scoring higher. Future studies could examine whether higher levels of anxiety and depression are indeed associated with anomalous perceptual experiences. Given that depression is evidenced as being related to both loneliness and anomalous perceptual experiences such as hallucinations (Hsueh et al., Citation2019; Östling et al., Citation2013; Paulik et al., Citation2006), it is also plausible that depression may be a confounding variable in the relationship between loneliness and anomalous perceptions. Potential confounders not measured in the current study, such as poor sleep (e.g. Hom et al., Citation2020) or impaired cognitive function (e.g. Cacioppo et al., Citation2014)—both of which occur in normal ageing—may also explain why anxiety and depression did not mediate the relationship between loneliness and perceptual anomalies. As such, it is important that future research investigates other social, cognitive, and biological mechanisms linking loneliness and anomalous perceptions, and considers mediating factors other than mood. The neural basis of loneliness has also been investigated, with evidence pointing to a range of structural and functional changes in cortical circuits linked to social cognition, such as the left posterior superior temporal sulcus (Nakagawa et al., Citation2015). Of note, a diverse array of cognitive and neural mechanisms has been implicated in the experience of hallucinations and other anomalous perceptions in clinical and non-clinical populations (Zmigrod et al., Citation2016).

A more variable pattern of relationships was observed between social isolation and anomalous perceptions. Preliminary analyses of the BSAI revealed poor internal consistency. Accordingly, the four items were entered as simultaneous predictors. Importantly, H2 was only partially supported since an increase in objective social isolation for only 1 of 4 items was associated with an increase in anomalous perceptions (as hypothesised). Specifically, social disconnection from a religious group was associated with more anomalous external experiences. Exploratory analyses of the relationship between social isolation and anomalous body-centred self-experiences revealed that being married/living with a partner—rather than an absence of intimate relationships—was associated with more anomalous body-centred self-experiences. Furthermore, no significant associations were found between the number of friends/relatives spoken to every 2 weeks or membership in social/sporting/neighbourhood groups and either type of anomalous perceptions. It is unclear why these differential patterns emerged, as El Haj et al. (Citation2016) found that social isolation significantly predicted anomalous and hallucinatory experiences in older adults (both with and without Alzheimer’s), even when controlling for loneliness. However, El Haj et al. (Citation2016) examined visual and auditory hallucinations, rather than the broad range of anomalous perceptions assessed here.

One potential explanation is that the nature of an individual’s social connection is important when examining risk for anomalous perceptions. That is, “strong” (i.e. BSAI Q1) and “weak” (i.e. BSAI Q3) social ties may differentially impact the experience of anomalous perceptions (Brooks, Citation2019). Indeed, previous research shows that social network size decreases with age (Cornwell et al., Citation2008), along with an increase in “strong” ties (e.g. spouses/partners, children) and a decrease in “weak” ties (e.g. acquaintances, fellow club members; Moore et al., Citation2016), which may serve as a protective mechanism with age. Another explanation is that, although being married/living with a partner is typically used as a proxy for increased social contact, it is possible that undetected social stress/negativity (Badcock et al., Citation2022), potentially related to illness and/or changes in role and functioning in married older adults, is associated with increased anomalous perceptions. For instance, being a spousal caregiver may limit or reduce social connection outside of the nuclear family (Li et al., Citation2021; Vasileiou et al., Citation2017), thus increasing anomalous perceptual experiences. Consequently, a more nuanced approach, such as investigating how the nature or quality of someone’s relationship may influence their social functioning, may be required to unravel the relationship between social isolation and anomalous perceptions.

These findings must be considered with some caveats. First, the social isolation measure was problematic. While previous research has often used a range of social isolation metrics (such as the four used here), we found the measure was psychometrically weak. Other measures of social isolation (e.g. the Lubben [Citation1988] Social Network Scale) may be more robust; however, given the current findings, measures assessing both “strong” and “weak” social ties may be appropriate in future studies. Secondly, as noted above, the cross-sectional nature of this study means that we cannot assume causality. Indeed, experiences such as hallucinations can lead to loneliness, suggesting that reciprocal effects are likely (Michalska Da Rocha et al., Citation2018). Longitudinal studies examining social isolation, loneliness, and later anomalous perceptual experiences are required to help unravel causality. Nevertheless, the findings are still informative for clinical practice. For example, they highlight that attending to loneliness in older adults should be a public health priority (Perissinotto et al., Citation2019), particularly given our ageing population, since loneliness is related not only to increased levels of anxiety and depression, but also to higher rates of anomalous and hallucinatory experiences. Thirdly, the current sample had relatively low diversity (most participants were female, Anglo-Australian, considered relatively well socially connected, with low levels of anxiety and depression); consequently, the generalisability of the findings cannot be assured. As also noted above, it is possible that variables such as depression, sleep, and cognition may be confounders in the relationship between loneliness and anomalous perceptions. Consequently, future research should seek to examine the relationships between loneliness, social isolation, and anomalous perceptions, alongside other potential confounds. Finally, the current analysis focussed on how loneliness and social isolation are related to the absence/presence of anomalous perceptions, regardless of individual variation in the frequency, intrusiveness, and distress associated with these experiences. Our results, suggest that these social factors (especially loneliness) may play a significant role in prompting the onset of anomalous perceptions, and are thus worthy of attention. That said, it will be important to explore if, and how, loneliness and social isolation are (or are not) associated with ratings on other phenomenological characteristics of anomalous perceptions using the CAPS subscales.

In summary, regardless of whether participants were socially isolated, those reporting higher levels of loneliness also reported more anomalous body-centred self-experiences and anomalous external experiences. Though some (but not all) components of social isolation (i.e. marital/living status and religious group involvement) were associated with perceptual anomalies, further research is recommended to investigate the relationship between the nature of someone’s social context (e.g. “strong” and “weak” social ties) and anomalous perceptions. Nevertheless, that this is likely a high-functioning, non-clinical sample with relatively low levels of anxiety, depression, loneliness, and social isolation, makes the reported findings of significant relationships even more striking. Together, these findings have important implications for theoretical models of perceptual anomalies, and for the treatment of loneliness as a precursor to these experiences. In particular, the current findings suggest that greater priority must be given to understanding the consequences of social risk factors for older adults, and that improved training for clinicians about loneliness and its effects in older adults is key. Lastly, these findings further highlight the importance of examining both objective social isolation and loneliness to provide a more complete picture of people’s overall social context.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (46.3 KB)

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Brandon Gavett for his valuable feedback on previous drafts of this work.

Data availability statement

The deidentified data that support the findings of this study are openly available in OSF at https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/WC92Y

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Additional information

Funding

Author IKF was supported by an Australian Government Research and Training Scholarship.

Notes

1 For each item endorsed “yes”, participants also rate the level of associated distress, intrusiveness, and frequency on a 5-point scale. Here, factor analysis is of the “yes/no” responses to all CAPS items; therefore, these subscales were not analysed further.

2 Table S1 shows frequency counts for each individual CAPS item.

3 Tables S2 and S3 show frequency counts for the GAD-7 and PHQ-9 total scores, respectively.

4 Standardised loadings for each model are reported in the associated figure and unstandardised loadings are reported in Table S4.

5 There are no specific guidelines for defining who is (or is not) lonely on the UCLA-LS3. However, unpublished recommendations (sent as a personal communication) from Russell (2017) suggest that criterion scores 2 SDs above the mean be employed to define “very high” levels of loneliness, and >1 SD above the mean be used to define “moderately high” loneliness.

References

  • Anderson, G. O. (2010). Loneliness among older adults: A national survey of adults 45 + . https://doi.org/10.26419/res.00064.001
  • Badcock, J. C., Dehon, H., & Larøi, F. (2017). Hallucinations in healthy older adults: An overview of the literature and perspectives for future research. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(1134), https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01134
  • Badcock, J. C., Holt-Lunstad, J., Garcia, E., Bombaci, P., & Lim, M. H. (2022). Position statement: Addressing social isolation and loneliness and the power of human connection. In Global Initiative on Loneliness and Connection. www.gilc.global
  • Bell, V., Halligan, P. W., & Ellis, H. D. (2006). The cardiff anomalous perceptions scale (CAPS): A new validated measure of anomalous perceptual experience. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(2), 366–377. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbj014
  • Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 238–246. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238
  • Beutel, M. E., Klein, E. M., Brähler, E., Reiner, I., Jünger, C., Michal, M., Wiltink, J., Wild, P. S., Münzel, T., Lackner, K. J., & Tibubos, A. N. (2017). Loneliness in the general population: Prevalence, determinants and relations to mental health. BMC Psychiatry, 17(1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1262-x
  • Brooks, B. A. (2019). The strength of weak ties. Nurse Leader, 17(2), 90–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mnl.2018.12.011
  • Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Lawrence Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203726532.
  • Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., & Boomsma, D. I. (2014). Evolutionary mechanisms for loneliness. Cognition and Emotion, 28(1), https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.837379
  • Chiu, P. Y., Hsu, M. H., Wang, C. W., Tsai, C. T., & Pai, M. C. (2017). Visual hallucinations in Alzheimer’s disease is significantly associated with clinical diagnostic features of dementia with Lewy bodies. PLoS ONE, 12(10), https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186886
  • Cornwell, B., Laumann, E. O., & Schumm, L. P. (2008). The social connectedness of older adults: A national profile. American Sociological Review, 73(2), 185–203. https://doi.org/10.1177/000312240807300201
  • Costa, M. V., Diniz, M. F., Nascimento, K. K., Pereira, K. S., Dias, N. S., Malloy-Diniz, L. F., & Diniz, B. S. (2016). Accuracy of three depression screening scales to diagnose major depressive episodes in older adults without neurocognitive disorders. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 38(2), 154–156. https://doi.org/10.1590/1516-4446-2015-1818
  • Coyle, C. E., & Dugan, E. (2012). Social isolation, loneliness and health among older adults. Journal of Aging and Health, 24(8), 1346–1363. https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264312460275
  • El Haj, M., Jardri, R., Larøi, F., & Antoine, P. (2016). Hallucinations, loneliness, and social isolation in Alzheimer’s disease. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 21(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2015.1121139
  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
  • Gasca-Salas, C., Clavero, P., García-García, D., Obeso, J. A., & Rodríguez-Oroz, M. C. (2016). Significance of visual hallucinations and cerebral hypometabolism in the risk of dementia in Parkinson’s disease patients with mild cognitive impairment. Human Brain Mapping, 37(3), 968–977. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23080
  • Hawkley, L. C. (2018). Loneliness. https://www.britannica.com/science/loneliness
  • Hawkley, L. C., Browne, M. W., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2005). How can I connect with thee? Let me count the ways. Psychological Science, 16(10), 798–804. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01617.x
  • Hoffman, R. E. (2007). A social deafferentation hypothesis for induction of active schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(5), 1066–1070. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm079
  • Hoffman, R. E. (2008). Auditory/verbal hallucinations, speech perception neurocircuitry, and the social deafferentation hypothesis. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 39(2), 87–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/155005940803900213
  • Hom, M. A., Chu, C., Rogers, M. L., & Joiner, T. E. (2020). A meta-analysis of the relationship between sleep problems and loneliness. Clinical Psychological Science, 8(5), 799–824. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702620922969
  • Hsueh, Y. C., Chen, C. Y., Hsiao, Y. C., & Lin, C. C. (2019). A longitudinal, cross-lagged panel analysis of loneliness and depression among community-based older adults. Journal of Elder Abuse & Neglect, 31(4–5), 281–293. https://doi.org/10.1080/08946566.2019.1660936
  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
  • IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 25.0.
  • Jaén-Moreno, M. J., Moreno-Diaz, M. J., Luque-Luque, R., & Bell, V. (2014). Validation of the spanish version of the cardiff anomalous perceptions scale in the general population. Actas Espanolas de Psiquiatria, 42(1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1037/t29560-000
  • JASP Team. (2019). JASP (Version 0.11.1). https://jasp-stats.org/
  • Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1998). Structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS command language. Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Kelsall-Foreman, I., Gavett, B. E., Bucks, R. S., Weinborn, M., & Badcock, J. C. (2020). Factor structure and age invariance of the cardiff anomalous perceptions scale (CAPS) in healthy older and younger adults. Psychological Assessment, 32(12), 1095–1105. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000947
  • Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (2001). The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16(9), 606–613. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x
  • Laloyaux, J., Bless, J. J., Hugdahl, K., Kråkvik, B., Vedul-Kjelsås, E., Kalhovde, A. M., & Larøi, F. (2019). Multimodal hallucinations are associated with poor mental health and negatively impact auditory hallucinations in the general population: Results from an epidemiological study. Schizophrenia Research, 210, 319–322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2019.06.005
  • Li, L., Wister, A. V., & Mitchell, B. (2021). Social isolation among spousal and adult-child caregivers: Findings from the Canadian longitudinal study on aging. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 76(7), 1415–1429. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa197
  • Lim, M. H., Eres, R., & Vasan, S. (2020). Understanding loneliness in the twenty-first century: An update on correlates, risk factors, and potential solutions. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 55(7), 793–810. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00127-020-01889-7
  • Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 1198–1202. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722
  • Lubben, J. E. (1988). Assessing social networks among elderly populations. Family and Community Health, 11(3), 42–52. https://doi.org/10.1097/00003727-198811000-00008
  • Menec, V. H., Newall, N. E., Mackenzie, C. S., Shooshtari, S., & Nowicki, S. (2019). Examining individual and geographic factors associated with social isolation and loneliness using Canadian longitudinal study on aging (CLSA) data. PLoS ONE, 14(2), https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211143
  • Menon, G. J., Rahman, I., Menon, S. J., & Dutton, G. N. (2003). Complex visual hallucinations in the visually impaired: The charles bonnet syndrome. Survey of Ophthalmology, 48(1), 58–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0039-6257(02)00414-9
  • Michael, J., & Park, S. (2016). Anomalous bodily experiences and perceived social isolation in schizophrenia: An extension of the social deafferentation hypothesis. Schizophrenia Research, 176(2–3), 392–397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.06.013
  • Michalska Da Rocha, B., Rhodes, S., Vasilopoulou, E., & Hutton, P. (2018). Loneliness in psychosis: A meta-analytical review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(1), 114–125. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx036
  • Moore, S., Teixeira, A., & Stewart, S. (2016). Do age, psychosocial, and health characteristics alter the weak and strong tie composition of network diversity and core network size in urban adults? SSM - Population Health, 2, 623–631. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2016.08.009
  • Muthén, L. (2013). Negative residual variance message. http://www.statmodel.com/cgi-bin/discus/show.cgi?9/17688
  • Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.
  • Nakagawa, S., Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Nouchi, R., Sekiguchi, A., Kotozaki, Y., Miyauchi, C. M., Iizuka, K., Yokoyama, R., Shinada, T., Yamamoto, Y., Hanawa, S., Araki, T., Hashizume, H., Kunitoki, K., Sassa, Y., & Kawashima, R. (2015). White matter structures associated with loneliness in young adults. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 17001. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep17001
  • Newall, N. E., & Menec, V. H. (2019). Loneliness and social isolation of older adults: Why it is important to examine these social aspects together. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 36(3), 925–939. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407517749045
  • Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). McGraw-Hill.
  • O’Bryant, S. E., Humphreys, J. D., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., Graff-Radford, N. R., Petersen, R. C., & Lucas, J. A. (2008). Detecting dementia with the mini-mental state examination in highly educated individuals. Archives of Neurology, 65(7), 963–967. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.65.7.963
  • Östling, S., Bäckman, K., Waern, M., Marlow, T., Braam, A. W., Fichter, M., Lawlor, B. A., Lobos, A., Reischies, F. M., Copeland, J. R. M., & Skoog, I. (2013). Paranoid symptoms and hallucinations among the older people in Western Europe. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28(6), 573–579. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.3861
  • Paulik, G., Badcock, J. C., & Maybery, M. T. (2006). The multifactorial structure of the predisposition to hallucinate and associations with anxiety, depression and stress. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(6), 1067–1076. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.04.012
  • Pellas, J., & Damberg, M. (2021). Accuracy in detecting major depressive episodes in older adults using the Swedish versions of the GDS-15 and PHQ-9. Upsala Journal of Medical Sciences, 126, 7848. https://doi.org/10.48101/ujms.v126.7848
  • Pendlebury, S. T., Welch, S. J. V., Cuthbertson, F. C., Mariz, J., Mehta, Z., & Rothwell, P. M. (2013). Telephone assessment of cognition after transient ischemic attack and stroke: Modified telephone interview of cognitive status and telephone Montreal cognitive assessment versus face-to-face Montreal cognitive assessment and neuropsychological battery. Stroke, 44(1), 227–229. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.112.673384
  • Perissinotto, C., Holt-Lunstad, J., Periyakoil, V. S., & Covinsky, K. (2019). A practical approach to assessing and mitigating loneliness and isolation in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 67(4), 657–662. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.15746
  • Russell, D. W. (1996). UCLA loneliness scale (version 3): Reliability, validity, and factor structure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66(1), 20–40. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa6601_2
  • Santini, Z. I., Jose, P. E., York Cornwell, E., Koyanagi, A., Nielsen, L., Hinrichsen, C., Meilstrup, C., Madsen, K. R., & Koushede, V. (2020). Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and symptoms of depression and anxiety among older Americans (NSHAP): A longitudinal mediation analysis. The Lancet Public Health, 5(1), e62–e70. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(19)30230-0
  • Scarmeas, N., Brandt, J., Albert, M., Hadjigeorgiou, G., Papadimitriou, A., Dubois, B., Sarazin, M., Devanand, D., Honig, L., Marder, K., Bell, K., Wegesin, D., Blacker, D., & Stern, Y. (2005). Delusions and hallucinations are associated with worse outcome in Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology, 62(10), 1601–1608. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.62.10.1601
  • Scott, J., Welham, J., Martin, G., Bor, W., Najman, J. M., O’Callaghan, M., Williams, G., Aird, R., & McGrath, J. (2008). Demographic correlates of psychotic-like experiences in young Australian adults. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 118(3), 230–237. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01214.x
  • Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B. W., & Löwe, B. (2006). A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Archives of Internal Medicine, 166(10), 1092–1097. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092
  • Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25(2), 173–180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4
  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Allyn and Bacon. https://lccn.loc.gov/2017040173
  • Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, 38(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02291170
  • van den Broek, T. (2017). Gender differences in the correlates of loneliness among Japanese persons aged 50–70. Australasian Journal on Ageing, 36(3), 234–237. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajag.12448
  • Vandervoort, D. (2000). Social isolation and gender. Current Psychology, 19(3), 229–236. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-000-1017-5
  • Vasileiou, K., Barnett, J., Barreto, M., Vines, J., Atkinson, M., Lawson, S., & Wilson, M. (2017). Experiences of loneliness associated with being an informal caregiver: A qualitative investigation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(APR), 585. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00585
  • Wild, B., Eckl, A., Herzog, W., Niehoff, D., Lechner, S., Maatouk, I., Schellberg, D., Brenner, H., Müller, H., & Löwe, B. (2014). Assessing generalized anxiety disorder in elderly people using the GAD-7 and GAD-2 scales: Results of a validation study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 1029–1038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2013.01.076
  • Yu, C. Y. (2002). Evaluating cutoff criteria of model fit indices for latent variable models with binary and continous outcomes.
  • Zmigrod, L., Garrison, J. R., Carr, J., & Simons, J. S. (2016). The neural mechanisms of hallucinations: A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 113–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.05.037