5,231
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Perfectionism, mattering, stress, and self-regulation of home learning of UK gifted and talented students during the COVID-19 pandemic

&

ABSTRACT

The COVID-19 pandemic forced the closure of schools in the UK and students had to study at home with limited access to the support they would have normally received. We designed this study to assess the experience of gifted and talented (GAT) students during this period and to identify factors related to their stress and self-regulation of learning. The factors we focused on were perfectionism and feelings of mattering. Three-hundred and eleven GAT students completed an online questionnaire when schools were closed and they were studying at home. Analyses revealed that striving for perfection was related to better effort regulation and better time and environment management. However, negative reactions to imperfection and feeling like they did not matter (anti-mattering) were related to higher academic stress, as well as worse effort regulation and worse time and environment management. The findings suggest aspects of both perfectionism and mattering may be important for GAT students’ ability to study effectively during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has caused considerable disruption and stress for teachers, parents, and students. Following school closures, students were required to study at home for extended periods of time. Students would have faced no situation like this before – studying largely independently, with minimum feedback, and uncertainty regarding progression, assessment, and achievement. For extended periods, the pandemic also included home-confinement and social distancing, that meant accessing social and educational support for students and their families was more difficult. In the current study we were interested in what factors were related to both the academic stress gifted and talented students (GAT) experienced and their ability to self-regulate their learning during the pandemic. In deciding on which factors to focus upon, we chose perfectionism and feelings of mattering.

Perfectionism is a personality trait that is often discussed in context of gifted and talented students (Rice & Ray, Citation2018). Although there are different approaches to studying perfectionism, it is typically considered to include two broad dimensions – perfectionistic strivings (PS) and perfectionistic concerns (PC). PS refers to the features of perfectionism associated with personal standards (typically described as high, exceptionally high, or unrealistically high) whereas PC refer to the features of perfectionism associated with harsh self-evaluation (doubts, fears, feelings of inadequacy and negative reactions; Stoeber & Otto, Citation2006). We focus on two subdimensions of PS and PC in this study – striving for perfection and negative reactions to imperfection. These are key aspects of perfectionism and central to PS (striving for perfection) and PC (negative reactions to imperfection; Stoeber & Rennert, Citation2007).

In keeping with the notion that perfectionism may be important in GAT students, research has found that while this group has similar levels of PC to other students, they tend to have slightly higher levels of PS (Stricker et al., Citation2020). In addition, a recent review of research in this area found PS and PC, separately and in combination, to be related to the achievement, motivation, and wellbeing of GAT students (Grugan et al., Citation2021). Notably, the findings of the review suggested that PC was likely to be problematic for GAT students whereas PS were more mixed and under some circumstances may coincide with some benefits, notably higher academic achievement. These differences were typically more pronounced when the relationship between PS and PC were statistically controlled (e.g., Chan, Citation2007). Surprisingly, to date, no study has examined the relationship between perfectionism, academic stress or self-regulation of learning either in typical circumstances or during extended periods of life stress and home study.

Mattering is a feeling of being depended on, important to others, and receiving due attention from others (Rosenberg & McCullough, Citation1981). It is thought to be fundamental to wellbeing and resilience, providing a psychological resource and buffer when confronted by adversity (Flett & Zangeneh, Citation2020). People can also feel like they don’t matter. Rather than corresponding with the absence of feelings of mattering, anti-mattering is a feeling of being invisible, insignificant, and uncared for (Flett, Citation2018a). Likewise, instead of simply signaling lower levels of an important psychological resource, feelings of anti-mattering are thought to have an additional psychological toll and underpin the development of loneliness, a sense of social disconnection, and being marginalized (Flett, Citation2018a). In support of these ideas, recent research examining mattering and anti-mattering in students has found them to be related to loneliness, self-esteem, and distress in opposing directions (Besser et al., Citation2020; McComb et al., Citation2020).

Both mattering and perfectionism are thought to be important in regards to how people respond during times of life stress, including during the COVID-19 pandemic. The key issue central to both perfectionism and mattering in this regard is that the typical anxiety, worry, and stress associated with uncertainty and a lack of control will be heightened among people who are more perfectionistic or fear they do not matter (Casale & Flett, Citation2020; Flett & Hewitt, Citation2020). As aptly stated by Flett and Hewitt (Citation2020, p. 84), “perfectionism is problematic at the best of times, but it is especially problematic during the worst of times.” The effects of low feelings of mattering and anti-mattering, too, will only be exacerbated by the sense of being alone that follows actual physical and social isolation (Casale & Flett, Citation2020). The current study is based on the assertion that these proposed effects will be evident in GAT students.

With this in mind, here we focus on the experiences of GAT students as they study at home with less support and greater uncertainty. We do so by assessing the degree of academic stress GAT students report. That is, their worry, preoccupation, and concern regarding grades and school (Macgeorge et al., Citation2005). We take academic stress as a signal of the psychological toll studying at home may be having on GAT students. In this regard, it is noteworthy that increased stress among students has been a common finding internationally during the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., Giannopoulou, et al., Citation2021) and that research has also found students to report more stress following home-schooling during this period (e.g., Pieh et al., Citation2021). However, while research has been able to capture the experience of students generally, there have been few accounts of the experiences of GAT students during this period.

We also assessed self-regulation of learning, in particular, two types of resource management strategies – effort regulation and time and study environment management. Effort regulation refers to students’ ability to control effort and attention despite distractions whereas time and study environment management refers to a students’ ability to use the time and spaces they have to study effectively (Pintrich et al., Citation1993). Underlying the examination of these self-regulation strategies is evidence that they will be important in whether GAT students maintain their own learning during prolonged absences from school. Self-regulation of learning is related to greater academic achievement, generally (e.g., Dent & Koenka, Citation2016), and its importance is evident in research examining home schooling during the COVID-19 pandemic, specifically. In one recent study, for example, students’ self-regulation was positively related to learning independence which in turn was related to lower rating of task difficulty and greater task enjoyment of school work during school closure (Blume et al., Citation2021). In another revealing recent study, students’ cognitive emotion regulation strategies were related to general stress, which in turn is related to the quality of home-schooling relationships with parents (Aznar et al., Citation2021).

The present study

The aim of the present study was to examine the relationships between perfectionism, mattering, anti-mattering, academic stress, and self-regulation of home learning during the COVID-19 pandemic among GAT students in the UK. Based on previous research, we expected negative reactions to imperfection and anti-mattering to be related to higher stress and poor self-regulation of home learning, whereas we expected striving for perfection and anti-mattering to either be related to lower stress and better self-regulation of home learning, or be unrelated to these variables.

Method

Procedure and participants

An advert for an online questionnaire was distributed to a network of secondary schools via the National Association of Able Children in Education. A sample of 720 students responded to the advert and completed the questionnaire. Of the respondents, 311 identified themselves as GAT students (the remaining students indicated they were not or did not know) and are the focus of this study. Note that in the UK the common terminology is used in schools is “more able and talented” and “more able learners” so these phrases were used. The GAT students were, on average, 14.93 years old (SD = 1.25; range = 13 to 18) and were in UK school year 8 (n = 11), year 9 (n = 111), year 10 (n = 88), year 11 (n = 31), and year 12 (n = 64; 6 did not provide this information). They reported spending, on average, 4.69 hours per day studying (SD = 2.17) with most involving using a screen (M = 4.24, SD = 2.15).

The questionnaire was opened on 02/06/20 and closed on 19/06/20 shortly after schools officially opened. Schools in the UK were officially closed due to the COVID pandemic on 20/03/2020 with learning primarily taking place at home from that point onwards. Secondary schools were officially reopened for year groups 10 and 12 (students who were 14 to 15 years old and 16 to 17 years old) on 15/06/20 with government advice remaining that schools continue to teach these students at home. The study was approved by a university research ethics committee. The response rate to the advert of the online questionnaire determined the sample size.

Measures

Perfectionism

Perfectionism was measured using the Multidimensional Inventory of Perfectionism – Education Version (Stoeber & Rennert, Citation2007) which differentiates striving for perfection as an indicator of PS (5 items; “At school … I strive to be as perfect as possible”) and negative reactions to imperfection as an indicator of PC (5 items; “At school … I get completely furious if I make mistakes”). The item instructions were modified so as to ask participants about their experiences when studying (“At school … ” was replaced with “When studying … ”). Participants responded on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The subscales have demonstrated reliability and validity in previous studies in an education setting (e.g., Stoeber & Rennert, Citation2007).

Mattering and anti-mattering

Feelings of mattering were measured using the Rosenberg General Mattering Scale (GMS; Marcus & Rosenberg, Citation1987) and the Anti-Mattering Scale (AMS; Flett et al, Citation2022). These provide separate scores for mattering (5 items; “How important do you feel you are to other people”) and anti-mattering (5-items; “How much do you feel like you don’t matter”). Participants were asked to indicate how they had felt over the past last month by responding on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 4 (a lot). The scales have demonstrated reliability and validity in previous studies (e.g., Besser et al., Citation2020).

Academic stress

Academic stress was measured using the 3-item scale from Macgeorge et al. (Citation2005; “I am spending a lot of time thinking about how this term could negatively affect my educational and career goals,” “I am worrying a great deal about the effect this term will have on my future,” and “I find myself very concerned about the grades I am likely to receive in the future”). Participants were asked to respond to each item in relation to the current school term by responding on a scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 7 (strongly disagree; reversed for purpose of analyses). The scale has demonstrated reliability and validity in previous research (e.g., Hystad et al., Citation2009).

Self-regulation of learning

Self-regulation of learning was measured using the effort regulation (4 items; “I often feel so lazy or bored when I study that I quit before I finish what I planned to do” [reversed item]) and time and study environment (8 items; “I have a regular place set aside for studying”) subscales from the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (Pintrich et al., Citation1993). Participants were asked to respond to each item in relation to their experiences during COVID-19 on a scale from 1 (not at all true of me) to 7 (very true of me). The MSLQ has demonstrated reliability and validity in previous studies (see, Duncan & McKeachie, Citation2005).

Preliminary analyses

We inspected the data for missing values. Because very few item responses were missing (i = 16), missing responses were replaced with the mean of the item responses of the corresponding scale (Graham et al., Citation2003). Next, we computed MacDonald’s omega to assess internal reliability of the measures, which were all satisfactory (see, ). Then, following recommendations by Tabachnick and Fidell (Citation2007), we screened for univariate (z-score > +/-3.29) and multivariate outliers (Mahalanobis distance > 24.32, df = 7, p < .001). Three multivariate outliers were removed.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics, omega, and Pearson’s correlations

We also assessed the factor structure of the instruments using both confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM). In both cases we used robust maximum likelihood estimation with the addition of oblique target rotation in the ESEM. Fit was evaluated using chi-square (χ2/df), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), standard root mean square residual (SRMR), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). Standard recommendations for adequate fit were used; χ2/df ≤3, CFI ≥.90, TLI ≥.90, SRMR ≤.10, RMSEA ≤.10 (Marsh et al., Citation2004). Factor loadings were considered meaningful when ≥.32 (Worthington & Whittaker, Citation2006).

Instruments displayed adequate fit in CFAs with all items loading on intended factors (>.30). This was similar for ESEMs with two exceptions. First, in the ESEM for mattering and anti-mattering, three anti-mattering items loaded meaningfully on the mattering latent variable, but these were negative so not unexpected or considered problematic. Second, the ESEM for the self-regulation scale produced less than adequate fit, meaningful cross-loadings (two items from the effort regulation subscale) and items not loading meaningfully on target factor (three items from the time and study environment subscale). However, the more stringent CFA supported the factor structure of the self-regulation scales and produced meaningful loadings of items so this was not considered problematic. The results of these analyses are reported in the supplementary materials. Analyses were conducted using SPSS 28.0 (IBM Corp) and Mplus 8.1 (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2017).

Results

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations

There was no evidence of ceiling or floor effects for the instruments. Striving for perfection was positively correlated with academic stress, effort regulation, and time and environment management, whereas negative reactions to imperfection was positively correlated with anti-mattering and academic stress but negatively correlated with mattering, effort regulation, and time and environment management. Mattering was negatively correlated with academic stress and positively correlated with effort regulation, and time and environment management whereas anti-mattering displayed the opposite set of correlations. These analyses are presented in .

Regression analyses

In order to examine the degree to which perfectionism and mattering were related to academic stress, effort regulation, and time and environment management, and what their unique relationships were, a series of multiple regressions were conducted. The results are presented in . Note that, in multiple regression, regression coefficients are the relationship between one variable and the dependent variable (e.g., negative reactions to imperfection and academic stress) when statistically controlling for the relationship between the other variables (e.g., negative reactions to imperfection and strivings for perfection).

Table 2. Summary of multiple regression analyses

Academic stress

Negative reactions to imperfection (β = −.37, B = .64, p = .001) and anti-mattering were positively related to academic stress (β = .15, B = .35, p = .040). there were no other statistically significant relationships. In total, 16% of the variance was explained in the model (R= .16, p < .001; adjusted R= .15). In a separate model, we also examined if an interaction between striving for perfection and negative reactions was also related to stress, but this was not the case.

Effort regulation

Striving for perfection was positively related to effort regulation (β =.36, B = .47, p = .001) whereas negative reactions to imperfection (β = −.30, B = −.25, p = .002). and anti-mattering were negatively related to effort regulation (β = −.41, B = −.26, p = .001). The relationship between mattering and effort regulation was not statistically significant. In total, 19% of the variance was explained in the model (R= .19, p < .001; adjusted R= .18). Again, in a separate model, we examined if an interaction between striving for perfection and negative reactions to imperfection was related to effort regulation, but this was not the case.

Time and environment management

Striving for perfection was positively related to time and environment management (β =.30, B = .35, p = .001) whereas negative reactions to imperfection (β = −.22, B = −.24, p = .002) and anti-mattering were negatively related to effort regulation (β = −.33, B = −.47, p = .001). The relationship between mattering and time and environment management was not statistically significant. In total, 19% of the variance was explained in the model (R= .19, p < .001; adjusted R= .18). Like with the other two dependent variables, in a separate model, an interaction between striving for perfection and negative reactions to imperfection was not related to time and environment management.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to examine the relationships between perfectionism, mattering, anti-mattering, academic stress, and self-regulation of home learning during the COVID-19 pandemic among GAT students in the UK. As we expected, PC and anti-mattering were related to higher stress and worse self-regulation of home learning. In addition, as expected, PS were related to better self-regulation of home learning but not academic stress, and mattering was not related to any of the dependent variables.

The findings regarding PS are broadly in line with research in this area that indicates that this aspect of perfectionism partly underpins the motivation exhibited by GAT students. This aspect of perfectionism has been found to be related to more favorable attitudes toward academic achievement and higher academic aspirations, as well as a greater sense of academic competence in these students (Grugan et al., Citation2021). It is possible that PS underpin a greater ability to concentrate and persist with academic tasks, and that this persistence is evident in GAT students are in charge of their own learning. This may partly explain why PS has been found to be related to better academic performance, generally, and in GAT students. In context of the present study, the finding suggests GAT students higher in PS may be better able to avoid distractions and use time and space effectively than those lower in PS.

A note of caution regarding the benefits of perfectionism comes in the form of the findings regarding PC, though, a dimension that is typically positively related to PS as was the case in the current study. This dimension of perfectionism was related to higher levels of stress and worse home study self-regulation. These findings are, again, aligned to research more broadly for GAT students (Grugan et al., Citation2021). However, given that many of the external factors that help regulate learning (e.g., support, feedback, assessment) are not present when students are studying at home, we speculate that PC could potentially be more problematic in a home learning context. Regardless, given the emerging picture of the correlates of PC in GAT students, reducing PC appears an important focus. Recent research that suggests that mindfulness may be one way to do so (Olton-Weber et al., Citation2020).

In line with idea that anti-mattering is not merely the absence of mattering, we also found this factor to be important to GAT students’ at-home study experiences and did so when taking into account feelings of mattering. It is perhaps unsurprising that when GAT students report greater feelings of loneliness and insignificance, they also report more stress and poorer self-regulation of learning. While there is a great deal of research examining perfectionism in GAT students, to our knowledge, this is the first to examine mattering in this group. The findings are especially novel in this regard, and point to the possibility that feelings of anti-mattering have more to tell us about the experiences of GAT students. For now, the findings indicate that parents, teachers, and school leaders may need to consider ways in which to protect GAT students from feeling invisible, uncared for, and insignificant. The recent writing of Flett (Citation2018b) is an excellent starting point in this regard and places feelings of mattering at the center of mentally healthy schools.

Limitations and future research

The present study has several limitations. First, the study adopted a cross-sectional design. Although this approach allowed us to capture students’ experiences during lockdown, it did not permit us to examine changes that occurred before or after lockdown. As we seek to understand the long-term consequences of the pandemic for students’ learning, it would be interesting for future research to track changes in these relationships over time. Secondly, we relied on students’ self-reported stress and regulation. Objective measures (e.g., actual study time) or reports from others such as parents would help triangulate reports from students. Third, for the sake of brevity, we measured only a small number of indicators of student experience and self-regulations skills. Future work should consider including other indicators to gain a fuller picture of how different factors like mattering affect students when studying independently (e.g., other self-regulation strategies). Finally, it remains unclear whether lockdown has negatively affected actual student attainment. Measures of student attainment are required to do this and extend examination of the influence of perfectionism and mattering, especially if further lockdowns occur.

Conclusion

We examined the experiences of GAT students during lockdown and explored several factors that could be related to their stress and self-regulation of learning. We found that PS, PC, and anti-mattering were important. PC and feelings of anti-mattering, in particular, appear to be problematic and were related to greater academic stress and worse self-regulation of learning.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download Zip (18.1 KB)

Disclosure statement

Professor Andrew P. Hill is a Research Advisor for the National Association for Able Children in Education. This is an unpaid role. The authors have no other potential conflict of interests to be report.

Data availability statement

Data and code is available here: https://doi.org/10.25421/yorksj.19122479.

Supplementary material

Supplemental data for this article can be accessed on the publisher’s website.

Additional information

Funding

The authors would like to acknowledge and thank the National Association of Able Children in Education for commissioning this research.

Notes on contributors

Andrew P. Hill

Andrew P. Hill is a Professor (Full) in the School of Science, Technology and Health at York St John University. His main area of research is the effect of perfectionism in different achievement contexts, including education.

Daniel J. Madigan

Daniel J. Madigan is an Associate Professor in the School of Science, Technology and Health at York St John University. His main area of research is the effect of burnout and other motivational phenomenon in different achievement contexts, including education.

References

  • Aznar, A., Sowden, P., Bayless, S., Ross, K., Warhurst, A., & Pachi, D. (2021). Home-schooling during COVID-19 lockdown: Effects of coping style, home space, and everyday creativity on stress and home-schooling outcomes. Couple and Family Psychology: Research and Practice, 10(4), 294–312. https://doi.org/10.1037/cfp0000182
  • Besser, A., Flett, G. L., Nepon, T., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2020). Personality, cognition, and adaptability to the COVID-19 pandemic: Associations with loneliness, distress, and positive and negative mood states. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00421-x
  • Blume, F., Schmidt, A., Kramer, A. C., Schmiedek, F., & Neubauer, A. B. (2021). Homeschooling during the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic: The role of students’ trait self-regulation and task attributes of daily learning tasks for students’ daily self-regulation. Zeitschrift Für Erziehungswissenschaft, 24(2), 367–391. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11618-021-01011-w
  • Casale, S., & Flett, G. L. (2020). Interpersonally-based fears during the COVID-19 pandemic: Reflections on the fear of missing out and the fear of not mattering constructs. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 17(2), 88–93. https://doi.org/10.36131/CN20200211
  • Chan, D. W. (2007). Positive and negative perfectionism among Chinese gifted students in Hong Kong: Their relationships to general self-efficacy and subjective well-being. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 31(1), 77–102. https://doi.org/10.4219/jeg-2007-512
  • Dent, A. L., & Koenka, A. C. (2016). The relation between self-regulated learning and academic achievement across childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 28(3), 425–474. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9320-8
  • Duncan, T. G., & McKeachie, W. J. (2005). The making of the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire. Educational Psychologist, 40(2), 117–128. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4002_6
  • Flett, G. L. (2018a). Resilience to interpersonal stress: Why mattering matters when building the foundation of mentally healthy schools. In D. Saklofske, and G. Flett (Eds.), Handbook of school-based mental health promotion (pp. 383–410). Springer.
  • Flett, G. L. (2018b). The psychology of mattering: Understanding the human need to be significant. Academic Press/ Elsevier.
  • Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2020). The perfectionism pandemic meets COVID-19: Understanding the stress, distress, and problems in living for perfectionists during the global health crisis. Journal of Concurrent Disorders, 2(1), 80–105. https://doi.org/10.54127/AXGJ8297
  • Flett, G. L., Nepon, T., Goldberg, J. O., Rose, A. L., Atkey, S. K., & Zaki-Azat, J. (2022). The Anti-Mattering Scale: Development, Psychometric Properties and Associations With Well-Being and Distress Measures in Adolescents and Emerging Adults. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 07342829211050544.
  • Flett, G. L., & Zangeneh, M. (2020). Mattering as a vital support for people during the COVID-19 pandemic: The benefits of feeling and knowing that someone cares during times of crisis. Journal of Concurrent Disorders, 2(1), 106–123. https://doi.org/10.54127/ALMC5515
  • Giannopoulou, I., Efstathiou, V., Triantafyllou, G., Korkoliakou, P., & Douzenis, A. (2021). Adding stress to the stressed: Senior high school students’ mental health amidst the COVID-19 nationwide lockdown in Greece. Psychiatry Research, 295, 113560.
  • Graham, J. W., Cumsille, P. E., & Elek-Fisk, E. F. (2003). Methods for handling missing data. In J. A. Schinka & W. F. Velicer (Eds.), Research methods in psychology (pp. 87–112). Wiley.
  • Grugan, M. C., Hill, A. P., Madigan, D. J., Donachie, T. C., Olsson, L. F., & Etherson, M. E. (2021). Perfectionism in academically gifted students: A systematic review. Educational Psychology Review, 33(4), 1631–1673. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-021-09597-7
  • Hystad, S. W., Eid, J., Laberg, J. C., Johnsen, B. H., & Bartone, P. T. (2009). Academic stress and health: Exploring the moderating role of personality hardiness. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 53(5), 421–429. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313830903180349
  • IBM Corp. ( Released 2019). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 26.0.
  • Ioanna, G., Vasiliki, E., Georgia, T., Panajota, K., & Athanasios, D. (2020). Adding stress to the stressed: Senior high school students’ mental health amidst the COVID-19 nationwide lockdown in Greece. Psychiatry Research, 295, 113560. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113560
  • Macgeorge, E. L., Samter, W., & Gillihan, S. J. (2005). Academic stress, supportive communication, and health. Communication Education, 54(4), 365–372. https://doi.org/10.1080/03634520500442236
  • Marcus, F. M., & Rosenberg, M. (1987). Mattering: It’s measurement and significance in everyday life. The 57th Annual Eastern Sociological Society Meeting, Boston, Massachusetts.
  • Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing hu and bentler’s (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 11(3), 320–341. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2
  • McComb, S. E., Goldberg, J. O., Flett, G. L., & Rose, A. L. (2020). The double jeopardy of feeling lonely and unimportant: State and trait loneliness and feelings and fears of not mattering. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.563420
  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.
  • Olton-Weber, S., Hess, R., & Ritchotte, J. A. (2020). Reducing levels of perfectionism in gifted and talented youth through a mindfulness intervention. Gifted Child Quarterly, 64(4), 319–330. https://doi.org/10.1177/0016986220953392
  • Pieh, C., Dale, R., Plener, P. L., Humer, E., & Probst, T. (2021). Stress levels in high-school students after a semester of home-schooling. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-021-01826-2
  • Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & Mckeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and predictive validity of the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53(3), 801–813. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164493053003024
  • Rice, K. G., & Ray, M. E. (2018). Perfectionism and the gifted. In S. I. Pfeiffer (Ed.), Handbook of giftedness in children: Psychoeducational theory, research, and best practices (Vol. 2, pp. 227–254). Springer.
  • Rosenberg, M., & McCullough, B. C. (1981). Mattering: Inferred significance and mental health among adolescents. Research in Community & Mental Health, 2, 163–182.
  • Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(4), 295–319. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_2
  • Stoeber, J., & Rennert, D. (2007). Perfectionism in school teachers: Relations with stress appraisals, coping styles, and burnout. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 21(1), 37–53. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615800701742461
  • Stricker, J., Buecker, S., Schneider, M., & Preckel, F. (2020). Intellectual giftedness and multidimensional perfectionism: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology Review, 32(2), 391–414. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09504-1
  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Pearson Education.
  • Worthington, R. L., & Whittaker, T. A. (2006). Scale development research: A content analysis and recommendations for best practices. The Counseling Psychologist, 34(6), 806–838. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000006288127