4,455
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Commentaries

Towards consensus on fear appeals: a rejoinder to the commentaries on Kok, Peters, Kessels, ten Hoor, and Ruiter (2018)

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 151-156 | Received 08 Mar 2018, Accepted 16 Mar 2018, Published online: 05 Apr 2018

We are glad that our – admittedly controversially titled – article succeeded in sparking lively debate (Borland, Citation2018; Brewer, Hall, & Noar, Citation2018; Malouff, Citation2018; Niederdeppe & Kemp, Citation2018; Peters & Shoots-Reinhard, Citation2018; Roberto, Mongeau, & Liu, Citation2018; White & Albarracín, Citation2018). We were faced with the considerable challenge of responding to so many commentaries. Therefore, this rejoinder has two parts. In this text, we will emphasise our common ground based on a number of major points that pervade across the commentaries, while we provide more comprehensive and detailed responses to each commentary in the supplementary file.Footnote1

We feel it is time for the debate about fear appeal effectiveness to mature into a more collaborative effort for two reasons. First, this debate does not help in achieving our mutual goal: optimally effective behaviour change interventions. Second, while this debate revealed a number of fundamental disagreements (e.g., regarding what constitutes evidence), it also revealed substantial consensus: enough to build on going forward.

Although our article concerns threatening communication in general and applies to all behaviours, the ensuing debate centred around the most ubiquitous instance of threatening communication: health communications on tobacco packaging. Presently, this space is used for pictorial warning labels, aiming to promote smoking cessation among smokers and discourage smoking initiation among vulnerable non-smokers, and many of the commentary authors are researchers who have conducted research on warning labels. Therefore, we will also use warning labels as an example; but note that the processing of threatening information is a characteristic of human psychology and so applies to all threatening communications.

Consensus 1: the urgency of the problem

First, the debate over fear appeal effectiveness made clear how committed all involved are to promoting the health of vulnerable populations. We all agree that current smokers and vulnerable ex-smokers deserve the best possible behaviour change interventions that our discipline of health psychology can devise. That consensus exists regarding this point may seem trivial, but we feel that it is important to keep in mind that we all want the same: promoting health.

Consensus 2: the relative roles of threat and efficacy

The core argument in our original article (Kok, Peters, Kessels, ten Hoor & Ruiter, Citation2018) was that without high efficacy (i.e., both high self-efficacy and high response efficacy; also see Roberto et al., Citation2018), threatening communication has no effect or may backfire. This thesis was heavily debated, yet a clear consensus also emerged. In their commentary, Brewer et al. (Citation2018 and Figure 1 in the supplemental part of this rejoinder) discuss the ‘weak’ and ‘strong’ hypotheses of Tannenbaum et al. (2015). The ‘weak hypothesis’ holds that when the targeted individuals do not believe they can change their behaviour, threatening communications have weak or no effects; whereas the ‘strong hypothesis’ holds that in that case, threatening communications backfire.

The consensus lies in that although we differ in our hypotheses regarding what exactly will happen under low efficacy, we all agree that threatening communications have at best small effects when targeting populations low in efficacy, unless they are accompanied by an intervention that successfully increases perceived efficacy.

We have lifted the relevant quotes from each of the seven commentaries to illustrate this consensus. The full list is included in the supplementary part of this rejoinder, but we include the first three quotes here as an illustration: ‘That motivational messages will be more effective when people believe they have the capacity to act is an outcome predicted by most health theories, including ones that would support use of threat communications […]’ (Borland, Citation2018), ‘[…] we agree with Kok et al. that the warnings would likely work even better with a self-efficacy message, and that self-efficacy messages should accompany fear communications whenever possible’ (Brewer et al., Citation2018), and ‘[…] the higher the self-efficacy individuals have for changing a health-related behavior, the more likely they are to make the change’ (Malouff, Citation2018).

Consensus 3: health behaviours have many determinants besides risk perceptions

The last point of agreement is that health behaviours (e.g., smoking cessation and initiation) are influenced by many determinants. Of course, efficacy, consisting of self-efficacy and response efficacy, is clearly a determinant we all agree is crucial; but the commentators also list, for example, disgust and negative affect as additional behavioural determinants, and many more determinants can be identified from theory and empirical studies (see Kok et al., Citation2018, and the first preliminary point in the supplemental file).

Building on consensus: designing the most effective behaviour change intervention

So, the commentators and ourselves agree that there are serious health problems, deserving of the most effective health communications we can develop; we agree that unless efficacy (i.e., both self-efficacy and response efficacy; see Roberto et al., Citation2018) is high, fear appeals have at best suboptimal effects and may even have null effects; and we agree that behaviour has many different determinants. Although this was not explicit from the debate, we assume that we all also agree that effective behaviour change interventions must target those determinants that most strongly predict the target behaviour, and they must do so with those behaviour change methods that have been empirically demonstrated to be effective in targeting these determinants.

We believe that there are two productive, parallel routes to take from here. The simpler route is based on our conviction that academic debates should conclude in an itinerary for future research to resolve the remaining disagreements. We have therefore made a start in the supplementary file (also see the linked project and components at the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/a2v46/). The first of these studies is a systematic literature synthesis of all determinants of smoking initiation and successful cessation. The overview produced by this synthesis can facilitate mapping the second route.

This second route involves a number of steps towards optimal health communication. Although applicable to all behaviour change and health promotion, we will explain these steps using the example of smoking. shows a hypothetical illustration of these steps. This illustration is based on the following reasoning.

Figure 1. An illustration of a hypothetical subset of potential sub-determinants (e.g., beliefs) that may be important for successful smoking cessation, the determinants they fall under, applications that could be used as health communications on tobacco packaging to target those sub-determinants, and the methods selected on the basis of those determinants, by virtue of which the applications can have an effect (most examples derived from Thrasher et al., Citation2015; see Kok et al., Citation2016, for the lists of methods).

Figure 1. An illustration of a hypothetical subset of potential sub-determinants (e.g., beliefs) that may be important for successful smoking cessation, the determinants they fall under, applications that could be used as health communications on tobacco packaging to target those sub-determinants, and the methods selected on the basis of those determinants, by virtue of which the applications can have an effect (most examples derived from Thrasher et al., Citation2015; see Kok et al., Citation2016, for the lists of methods).

Effectively changing behaviour always first requires a detailed understanding of the important and changeable personal and external factors driving that target behaviour. These factors can be described by social cognitive models of human behaviour (e.g., Conner & Norman, Citation2015)Footnote2, which hold that behaviour change is most likely when people have the motivation to change, and do not experience barriers or a lack of skills to conduct the new behaviour (e.g., Schwarzer & Luszczynska, Citation2015). Motivation (or intention) is determined by many psychological variables (e.g., general evaluations of behavioural outcomes, perceptions of behaviours and approval of important others, and self-efficacy; Ajzen, Citation2015), which consist of specific sub-determinants (e.g., beliefs) that people hold or acquire through exposure to information and education (Peters & Crutzen, Citation2017). Behaviour change interventions, then, consist of components that target those sub-determinants that most strongly predict the desirable behaviour as established using, for example, belief elicitation procedures (see Ajzen, Citation2015 or Peters, Citation2014) and quantitative determinant studies (see Crutzen, Peters, & Noijen, Citation2017). Each component must apply one or more behaviour change methods (see, for example, the list at http://osf.io/sqtuz/) to be able to change the relevant determinant (Crutzen & Peters, Citation2018).

shows a hypothetical application of these dynamics to health communication on tobacco packaging to promote smoking cessation. From right to left, cessation is determined by seven determinants, each consisting of sub-determinants, which are targeted by tobacco packaging messages, which each derive their effectiveness from a method for behaviour change.

Conclusion and the path towards further consensus

Based on this second route, as a scientific community of health psychologists, we owe it vulnerable populations to ask ourselves these questions:

  • What are the strongest determinants of the desirable behaviours (e.g., smoking cessation)?

  • Which methods are most effective to change those determinants?

  • Which of those methods can we successfully apply given health communication constraints (e.g., on tobacco packaging)?

We are deeply sceptical that the answer to the first question will be that ‘risk perception’ or ‘disgust’ are the strongest determinants of either initiation or successful cessation, and that the final outcome is ‘fear appeals’, but we concede that it is possible.

Regardless, we assume we are in agreement with the commentators that behaviour change must be approached systematically, using theory and strong empirical evidence to work from determinant to method to intervention, rather than retaining potentially suboptimal interventions (literally at all costs). We are confident that if henceforth we follow the two parallel routes we propose, this will ultimately yield optimal behaviour change interventions.

Supplemental material

RHPR_1454846_Supplemental_material.pdf

Download PDF (644.1 KB)

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Notes

1 This supplementary file is also hosted Open Access at the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/a2v46/.

2 This is obvious at the personal level, but at environmental levels (inter-individual, organizational, community, society), these same models can be used once the environmental agents have been identified that are responsible for changing each environmental condition, as well as additional theories (e.g. Glanz, Rimer, & Viswanath, Citation2015).

References

  • Abraham, C., & Michie, S. (2008). A taxonomy of behavior change techniques used in interventions. Health Psychology, 27, 379–387. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.3.379
  • Ajzen, I. (2015). The theory of planned behaviour is alive and well, and not ready to retire: A commentary on Sniehotta, Presseau, and Araújo-Soares. Health Psychology Review, 9, 131–137. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2014.883474
  • Bartholomew Eldredge, L. K., Markham, C. M., Ruiter, R. A. C., Fernández, M. E., Kok, G., & Parcel, G. S. (2016). Planning health promotion programs: An intervention mapping approach (4th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Bartholomew, L. K., Parcel, G. S., & Kok, G. (1998). Intervention mapping: A process for developing theory- and evidence-based health education programs. Health Education and Behavior, 25, 545–563. doi: 10.1177/109019819802500502
  • Bartlett, Y. K., Sheeran, P., & Hawley, M. S. (2014). Effective behaviour change techniques in smoking cessation interventions for people with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 181–203. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12071
  • Borland, R. (2018). Misinterpreting theory and ignoring evidence: Fear appeals can actually work: A comment on Kok et al. (2018). Health Psychology Review, 12(2), 126–128. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1445545
  • Brewer, N. T., Hall, M. G., & Noar, S. M. (2018). Pictorial cigarette pack warnings increase quitting: A comment on Kok et al. Health Psychology Review, 12(2), 129–132. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1445544
  • Brewer, N. T., Hall, M. G., Noar, S. M., Parada, H., Stein-Seroussi, A., Bach, L. E., … Ribisl, K. M. (2016). Effect of pictorial cigarette pack warnings on changes in smoking behavior. JAMA Internal Medicine, 176, 905. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.2621
  • Buller, D., Borland, R., & Burgoon, M. (1998). Impact of behavioural intention on effectiveness of message features: Evidence from the family sun safety project. Human Communication Research, 24, 433–453. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1998.tb00424.x
  • Chen, H., Cohen, P., & Chen, S. (2010). How big is a big odds ratio? Interpreting the magnitudes of odds ratios in epidemiological studies. Communications in Statistics: Simulation and Computation, 39, 860–864. doi: 10.1080/03610911003650383
  • Christley, R. M. (2010). Power and error: Increased risk of false positive results in underpowered studies. The Open Epidemiology Journal, 3, 16–19. doi: 10.2174/1874297101003010016
  • Conner, M., & Norman, P. (2015). Predicting and changing health behaviour: Research and practice with social cognition models (3rd ed.). London: Open University Press.
  • Crutzen, R., & Peters, G.-J. Y. (2018). Evolutionary learning processes as the foundation for behaviour change. Health Psychology Review, 12, 43–57. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2017.1362569
  • Crutzen, R., Peters, G.-J. Y., & Noijen, J. (2017). Using confidence interval-based estimation of relevance to select social-cognitive determinants for behavior change interventions. Frontiers in Public Health, 5, 1807. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00165
  • Gelman, A., & Loken, E. (2014). The garden of forking paths: Why multiple comparisons can be a problem, even when there is no “fishing expedition” or “p-hacking” and the research hypothesis was posited ahead of time. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1272–1280. doi: 10.1037/a0037714
  • Glanz, K., Rimer, B. K., & Viswanath, K. (2015). Health behavior: Theory, research, and practice (5th ed.). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
  • Green, D. P., Ha, S. E., & Bullock, J. G. (2010). Enough already about “black box” experiments: Studying mediation is more difficult than most scholars suppose. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 628, 200–208. doi: 10.1177/0002716209351526
  • Kleinot, M. C., & Rogers, R. W. (1982). Identifying effective components of alcohol misuse prevention programs. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 43, 802–811. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1982.43.802
  • Kok, G. (2014). A practical guide to effective behavior change: How to apply theory- and evidence-based behavior change methods in an intervention. European Health Psychologist, 16, 156–170.
  • Kok, G., Gottlieb, N. H., Peters, G. Y., Mullen, P. D., Parcel, G. S., Ruiter, R. A. C., … Bartholomew, L. K. (2016). A taxonomy of behavior change methods: An intervention mapping approach. Health Psychology Review, 10, 297–312. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2015.1077155
  • Kok, G., Peters, G.-J. Y., Kessels, L. T. E., ten Hoor, G. A., & Ruiter, R. A. C. (2018). Ignoring theory and misinterpreting evidence: The false belief in fear appeals. Health Psychology Review, 12(2), 111–125. doi:10.1080/17437199.2017.1415767
  • Kwasnicka, D., Dombrowski, S. U., White, M., & Sniehotta, F. (2016). Theoretical explanations for maintenance of behaviour change: A systematic review of behaviour theories. Health Psychology Review, 10, 277–296. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2016.1151372
  • Malouff, J. (2018). What constitutes evidence that fear appeals have positive effects on health behavior? Commentary on Kok, Peters, Kessels, ten Hoor, and Ruiter (2018). Health Psychology Review, 12(2), 133–135. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1445541
  • Masters, R., Anwar, E., Collins, B., Cookson, R., & Capewell, S. (2017). Return on investment of public health interventions: A systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 71, 827–834. doi: 10.1136/jech-2016-208141
  • McEachan, R. R. C., Conner, M., Taylor, N. J., & Lawton, R. J. (2011). Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review, 5, 97–144. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2010.521684
  • Michie, S., Richardson, M., Johnston, M., Abraham, C., Francis, J., Hardeman, W., … Wood, C. E. (2013). The Behavior Change Technique Taxonomy (v1) of 93 Hierarchically Clustered Techniques: Building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Annals of Behavioral Medicine: A Publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine 46, 81–95. doi: 10.1007/s12160-013-9486-6
  • Niederdeppe, J., & Kemp, D. (2018). Ignoring theory and evidence: Commentary on Kok et al. (2018). Health Psychology Review, 12(2), 136–139. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1445543
  • Ntoumanis, N., Healy, L. C., Sedikides, C., Smith, A. L., & Duda, J. L. (2014). Self-regulatory responses to unattainable goals: The role of goal motives. Self and Identity, 13, 594–612. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2014.889033
  • Ntoumanis, N., & Sedikides, C. (2018). Holding on (to the goal) or letting it go and move on? A tripartite model of goal striving. Current Directions in Psychological Science. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323676131_Holding_on_to_the_goal_or_letting_it_go_and_move_on_A_tripartite_model_of_goal_striving
  • Ogden, J. (2016). Celebrating variability and a call to limit systematisation: The example of the behaviour change technique taxonomy and the behaviour change wheel. Health Psychology Review, 10, 245–250. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2016.1190291
  • Peters, E., & Shoots-Reinhard, B. (2018). Don’t throw the baby out with the bath water: Commentary on Kok, Peters, Kessels, ten Hoor, and Ruiter (2018). Health Psychology Review, 12(2), 140–143. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1445542
  • Peters, G.-J. Y. (2014). A practical guide to effective behavior change: How to identify what to change in the first place. European Health Psychologist, 16, 142–155.
  • Peters, G.-J. Y., Abraham, C., & Crutzen, R. (2012). Full disclosure: Doing behavioural science necessitates sharing. The European Health Psychologist, 14, 77–84.
  • Peters, G.-J. Y., & Crutzen, R. (2017). Pragmatic nihilism: How a theory of nothing can help health psychology progress. Health Psychology Review, 11, 103–121. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2017.1284015
  • Peters, G.-J. Y., de Bruin, M., & Crutzen, R. (2015). Everything should be as simple as possible, but no simpler: Towards a protocol for accumulating evidence regarding the active content of health behaviour change interventions. Health Psychology Review, 9, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2013.848409
  • Peters, G.-J. Y., & Kok, G. (2016). All models are wrong, but some are useful: A comment on Ogden (2016). Health Psychology Review, 10(3), 265–268. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2016.1190658
  • Peters, G.-J. Y., Ruiter, R. A. C., & Kok, G. (2014). Threatening communication: A qualitative study of fear appeal effectiveness beliefs among intervention developers, policy makers, politicians, scientists, and advertising professionals. International Journal of Psychology, 49(2), 71–79. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12000
  • Peters, G.-J. Y., Ruiter, R. A. C., Verboon, P., & Kok, G. (2017, April 30). Threatening communication: Diffusing the scientific evidence on fear appeal effectiveness among intervention developers and other groups of key actors. PsyArXiv. Retrieved from https://psyarxiv.com/vehtx
  • Roberto, A. J., Mongeau, P. A., & Liu, Y. (2018). A (re)defining moment for fear appeals: A comment on Kok et al. (2018). Health Psychology Review, 12(2), 144–146. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1445546
  • Roe, R. A. (2012). What is wrong with mediators and moderators? The European Health Psychologist, 14, 4–10.
  • Rogers, R. W., & Mewborn, C. R. (1976). Fear appeals and attitude change: Effects of a threat’s noxiousness, probability of occurrence, and the efficacy of coping responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 54–61. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.34.1.54
  • Rosenthal, J. A. (1996). Qualitative descriptors of strength of association and effect size. Journal of Social Service Research, 21, 37–59. doi: 10.1300/J079v21n04_02
  • Ruiter, R. A. C., Abraham, C., & Kok, G. (2001). Scary warnings and rational precautions: A review of the psychology of fear appeals. Psychology & Health, 16, 613–630. doi: 10.1080/08870440108405863
  • Schwarzer, R. K., & Luszczynska, A., (2015). Health action process approach. In M. Conner, & P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting and changing health behaviour: Research and practice with social cognition models (3rd ed., pp. 252–278). London: Open University Press.
  • Sheeran, P., & Webb, T. L. (2016). The intention–behavior Gap. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(9), 503–518. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12265
  • Shtulman, A., & Valcarcel, J. (2012). Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant earlier intuitions. Cognition, 124, 209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.005
  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22, 1359–1366. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417632
  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2012). A 21 word solution. SSRN Electronic Journal, 1–4. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.2160588
  • Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 845–851. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.845
  • ten Hoor, G. A., Peters, G.-J. Y., Kalagi, J., de Groot, L., Grootjans, K., Huschens, A., … Kok, G. (2012). Reactions to threatening health messages. BMC Public Health, 12, 856. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-1011
  • Thrasher, J. F., Osman, A., Abad-Vivero, E. N., Hammond, D., Bansal-Travers, M., Michael Cummings, K., … Moodie, C. (2015). The use of cigarette package inserts to supplement pictorial health warnings: An evaluation of the Canadian policy. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 17, 870–875. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu246
  • van ‘t Riet, J., & Ruiter, R. A. C. (2013). Defensive reactions to health-promoting information: An overview and implications for future research. Health Psychology Review, 7, S104–S136. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2011.606782
  • Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249–268. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249
  • White, B., & Albarracín, D. (2018). Investigating belief falsehood. Fear appeals do change behavior in experimental laboratory studies. A commentary on Kok et al. (2018). Health Psychology Review, 12(2), 147–150. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1448292
  • Whittingham, J. R. D., Ruiter, R. A. C., Bolier, L., Lemmers, L., Hasselt, N. V., & Kok, G. (2009). Avoiding counterproductive results: An experimental pretest of a harm reduction intervention on attitude toward party drugs among users and nonusers. Substance Use & Misuse, 44, 532–547. doi: 10.1080/10826080802347685
  • Wilson, T. D., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2008). Psychology: The unseen mind. Science, 321, 1046–1047. doi: 10.1126/science.1163029
  • Wilson, T. D., & Dunn, E. W. (2004). Self-knowledge: Its limits, value, and potential for improvement. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 493–518. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141954
  • Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Communication Monographs, 59, 329–349. doi:10.1080/03637759209376276.