136,096
Views
149
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Effects of music interventions on stress-related outcomes: a systematic review and two meta-analyses

, , , &
Pages 294-324 | Received 18 Sep 2018, Accepted 03 Jun 2019, Published online: 15 Jul 2019

ABSTRACT

Music interventions are used for stress reduction in a variety of settings because of the positive effects of music listening on both physiological arousal (e.g., heart rate, blood pressure, and hormonal levels) and psychological stress experiences (e.g., restlessness, anxiety, and nervousness). To summarize the growing body of empirical research, two multilevel meta-analyses of 104 RCTs, containing 327 effect sizes and 9,617 participants, were performed to assess the strength of the effects of music interventions on both physiological and psychological stress-related outcomes, and to test the potential moderators of the intervention effects. Results showed that music interventions had an overall significant effect on stress reduction in both physiological (d = .380) and psychological (d = .545) outcomes. Further, moderator analyses showed that the type of outcome assessment moderated the effects of music interventions on stress-related outcomes. Larger effects were found on heart rate (d = .456), compared to blood pressure (d = .343) and hormone levels (d = .349). Implications for stress-reducing music interventions are discussed.

Introduction

Stress is believed to be one of the major factors negatively affecting our health. High stress levels have shown to be strongly associated with many physical and emotional problems, such as cardiovascular disease, chronic pain, anxiety disorders, depression, burnout, and addictions (American Psychological Association [APA], Citation2017; Australian Psychological Society [APS], Citation2015; Casey, Citation2017; Howe, Chang, & Johnson, Citation2013; McEwen & Gianaros, Citation2010). Furthermore, there is a strong relationship between these stress-related health problems and higher absenteeism at work (UK Health and Safety Executive, Citation2016). To cope with stressors, millions of people around the world use tranquilising medication, which is associated with numerous contraindications and negative side effects (e.g., Bandelow et al., Citation2015; Olfson, King, & Schoenbaum, Citation2015; Puetz, Youngstedt, & Herring, Citation2015). Because of the difficulty of reducing or preventing stress without any professional support and the great demand for nonpharmacological stress reduction interventions, the relevance of the development of cost-effective interventions for stress reduction is high (Casey, Citation2017; Holahan, Moos, Holahan, Brennan, & Schutte, Citation2005; Howe et al., Citation2013; McEwen & Gianaros, Citation2010; World Health Organization [WHO], Citation2010).

Music listening and music making have been associated with a broad range of positive outcomes in the domains of health and well-being (Juslin & Västfjäll, Citation2008; Koelsch, Citation2012, Citation2015; Thaut & Hoemberg, Citation2014; Zatorre, Citation2015). The most widely studied effects of music are the calming and stress reducing effects (Chanda & Levitin, Citation2013; Gillen, Biley, & Allen, Citation2008; Juslin & Västfjäll, Citation2008; Koelsch, Citation2015). For decades, music has been used as an intervention for stress reduction, such as music activities (like singing or music making), music listening for a certain patient group (‘music as medicine’), and live music therapy offered by music therapists (Bradt, Dileo, & Shim, Citation2013b; Gold et al., Citation2011).

In order to integrate the available knowledge on the effects of music interventions on stress, the present study is a systematic review and meta-analysis of experimental studies testing the effects of music interventions on both physiological and psychological stress-related outcomes in clinical, medical and work- or study-related settings.

The stress system

The stress system can be considered as a highly important and preserved system in human beings. In physiology and medicine, the general definition of stress is introduced by Selye (Citation1956): ‘Stress is a general activation reaction to a stimulus that could mean both a challenge (in a positive way) and a threat (in a negative sense)’ (p. 32). Aldwin (Citation2007) emphasized the negative part and defined stress as the quality of an experience, produced through a person-environment transaction that, through either overarousal or underarousal, results in psychological or physiological distress (Aldwin, Citation2007; Riley & Park, Citation2015). The responses to stress can be categorised as physiological arousal and emotional responses (e.g., Aldwin, Citation2007; Li & Goldsmith, Citation2012; Pelletier, Citation2004). Together, the underlying systems of these responses regulate stress and affect each other during stress (e.g., Linnemann, Strahler, & Nater, Citation2017; McEwen & Gianaros, Citation2010).

The physiological response to stress implies the activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis and, because of a release of adrenalin and noradrenalin, increased activity of the sympathetic nervous system resulting in increased physiological arousal, such as heartrate, blood pressure, and cardiac output (Bally, Campbell, Chesnick, & Tranmer, Citation2003; McCance,Forshee, & Shelby, Citation2006; Pfaff, Martin, & Ribeiro, Citation2007). In a parallel process, involving the hypothalamus and the adrenal glands, cortisol is released. The emotional response to stress can be described as emotional states of subjective worry, such as state anxiety, restlessness or nervousness (Akin & Iskender, Citation2011; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, Citation1983; Pittman & Kridli, Citation2011; Pritchard, Citation2009). State anxiety has been defined as an emotional response to an individual’s perception of a stressful experience (e.g., Hook, Songwathana, & Petpichetchian, Citation2008; Koelsch, Fuermetz, et al., Citation2011a; Ng et al., Citation2016; Zhang et al., Citation2014). Therefore, state anxiety was operationalised for several decades as one of the psychological stress-related outcomes (Koelsch, Fuermetz, et al., Citation2011a; Lazarus, Citation1966; Pelletier, Citation2004). It is assumed that both the physiological and the emotional responses of stress may be reduced by music (Bradt & Dileo, Citation2014; Dileo & Bradt, Citation2007; Pelletier, Citation2004).

Effects of music on stress

Recent neuroscientific studies provide insights into how music interventions may lead to stress reduction and increased well-being. Firstly, music seems to be able to decrease physiological arousal, which is increased during stress. Music listening, and music making/singing, have been associated with decreases of physiological arousal, shown by reduction of cortisol levels or decrease in heart rate and blood pressure (Hodges, Citation2011; Koelsch et al., Citation2016; Kreutz, Murcia, & Bongard, Citation2012; Leardi et al., Citation2007; Linnemann, Ditzen, Strahler, Doerr, & Nater, Citation2015; Nilsson, Citation2009; Sokhadze, Citation2007). These three outcomes have been identified in neurobiology as distinct stress biomarkers (Cacioppo, Tassinary, & Berntson, Citation2007; Pfaff et al., Citation2007).

Music may also affect stress-related emotional states, such as subjective worry, anxiety, restlessness or nervousness (Akin & Iskender, Citation2011; Cohen et al., Citation1983; Pittman & Kridli, Citation2011; Pritchard, Citation2009). This is because music can modulate activity in brain structures that are known to be crucially involved in emotional processes. Recent neuroimaging studies on music and emotion showed that music may strongly influence the amygdala, a part of the limbic system, which is a section of the brain that plays a crucial role in the regulation of emotional processes by releasing endorphins. These neurotransmitters play an important role in enhancing a sense of well-being (Blood & Zatorre, Citation2001; Hodges, Citation2011; Koelsch, Citation2015; Koelsch, Siebel, & Fritz, Citation2011b; Levitin, Citation2009; Moore, Citation2013; Thaut & Wheeler, Citation2010; Uhlig, Jaschke, & Scherder, Citation2013; Zatorre, Citation2015).

The systematic review of Moore (Citation2013) on the neurological effects of music on emotional processes indicated that musical improvisation and music listening could deactivate the amygdala, which may decrease the intensity of stress-related emotional states and psychophysiological arousal. This in turn has been shown to evoke feelings of pleasure and happiness (Blood & Zatorre, Citation2001; Koelsch et al., Citation2016; Koelsch, Offermanns, & Franzke, Citation2010; Limb & Braun, Citation2008). The (cognitive) behavioural framework is consistent with this and takes into account that music can serve as a distractor, diverting attention from a stressful event to something more pleasant, which reduces stress levels (Sendelbach, Halm, Doran, Miller, & Gaillard, Citation2006; Vaajoki, Kankkunen, Pietilä, & Vehviläinen-Julkunen, Citation2011).

Many studies on the effects of music considered state anxiety to be a stress-related emotional state, examining relationships between state anxiety outcomes and physiological stress-related outcomes (e.g., Hook et al., Citation2008; Koelsch, Fuermetz, et al., Citation2011a; Ng et al., Citation2016; Zhang et al., Citation2014). Although the terms state anxiety and stress are used interchangeably in the psychology literature, different self-reporting questionnaires are used. It is therefore necessary to examine whether music has the same effects on stress and state anxiety.

Music interventions

Music interventions can be regarded as purposeful musical exercises or methods in which music listening, music making, or singing is central. In both literature and practice there is a distinction between music interventions offered by a music therapist and music interventions offered by other healthcare professionals or without any support. First, music interventions can be defined as purposeful music activities if they concern listening to prerecorded music offered by medical or healthcare professionals, if the intervention is self-administered by the patient (‘music as medicine’), or if it concerns music making or singing without the involvement of a music therapist or a therapeutic context (American Music Therapy Association, Citation2019). Second, music interventions as a part of music therapy are offered by trained music therapists and are characterised by the presence of a therapeutic process and the use of personal music experiences (Bradt & Dileo, Citation2014; Bradt et al., Citation2013b; Dileo, Citation2006; Gold et al., Citation2011; Kamioka et al., Citation2014). Music interventions in the practice of music therapy may concern music listening or music playing, but may also include composing, songwriting, or interacting with music (Leubner & Hinterberger, Citation2017).

It is assumed that specific characteristics of the music may have an impact on the stress-reducing effects of music interventions (e.g., Bradt & Dileo, Citation2014; Bradt, Dileo, & Potvin, Citation2013a; Pittman & Kridli, Citation2011). Music tempo can be considered as one of the most important moderators of music-related arousal and relaxation. Music with a slow tempo (60–80 bpm), for example meditative music, has often been associated with reductions in heart rate, resulting in greater relaxation (e.g., Bernardi, Porta, & Sleight, Citation2006; Bringman, Giesecke, Thörne, & Bringman, Citation2009; Chlan, Citation2000; Hilz et al., Citation2014; Nomura, Yoshimura, & Kurosawa, Citation2013). The use of instrumental music, instead of music with lyrics, would often lead to greater effects of music interventions on stress reduction. Several studies reported that music containing lyrics may be more distracting and activating instead of calming (Good et al., Citation2000; Halpern & Savary, Citation1985). However, Koelsch et al. (Citation2011a) reported that the use of music with lyrics may reinforce the positive effects of music interventions on stress reduction through the possible comforting effects of the lyrics. Another component of music interventions for stress reduction is the way the music is played (live music or prerecorded music). Music therapy consists mainly of live music interventions, which are assumed be more effective than ‘music as medicine’ interventions because music therapists individualise their interventions to meet patients’ specific needs (Bradt & Dileo, Citation2014; Dileo, Citation1999, Citation2006). Notably, some studies measured differences in stress responses between participants receiving live music and those receiving prerecorded music, with live music appearing to be the most stress-reducing (e.g., Arnon et al., Citation2006; Bailey, Citation1983; Baker, Citation2001).

Many studies examining the effects of music interventions on stress-related outcomes in specific patient groups or settings have been published, such as cancer patients (Bradt, Dileo, Grocke, & Magill, Citation2011), coronary heart disease patients (Bradt et al., Citation2013a), and patients undergoing endoscopic procedures (Rudin, Kiss, Wetz, & Sottile, Citation2007). Several studies reported positive effects of music-listening on stress-related outcomes. In medical settings, listening to tranquilising music before, during, and after medical procedures has been reported to correlate with lower cortisol levels, associated with the reduction of stress and/or anxiety (e.g., Chanda & Levitin, Citation2013; Kamioka et al., Citation2014; Koelsch et al., Citation2016; Linnemann et al., Citation2015; Nilsson, Citation2008). However, the strength of the effect differs both within and between studies, and the particular impact of potential moderators – such as patient/client, setting, measurement and intervention characteristics – is largely unknown.

Rationale for the present study

The present study consists of two multilevel meta-analyses on the effects of music interventions on both physiological stress-related arousal (e.g., blood pressure, heart rate, hormone levels) and psychological stress-related experiences (e.g., state anxiety, restlessness or nervousness) in various populations and settings, and is a replication of the meta-analysis by Pelletier (Citation2004), who reviewed 22 quantitative studies examining the effects of music interventions on stress reduction. Pelletier’s (Citation2004) meta-analysis showed that music alone and music-assisted relaxation significantly reduced stress-related arousal, with an overall medium-to-large effect size of d = .67, moderated by study characteristics, such as age, type of stress, musical preference and type of intervention. An important difference with the present study is that Pelletier included only studies in which the intervention consisted of listening to prerecorded music, often combined with relaxation techniques, whereas studies with live music interventions were excluded. Besides, studies examining music therapy for stress reduction were not included. The quality of the included studies was not assessed. Therefore, it is not clear whether the methodology of the studies did influence Pelletier’s overall effects. In addition, we assume that the study quality of comparable studies has increased over the last 15 years.

In the last decade, music interventions were increasingly developed and used to reduce stress in a variety of settings (Chanda & Levitin, Citation2013; Heiderscheit, Chlan, & Donley, Citation2011; Koelsch, Citation2015; Uhlig et al., Citation2013), and to support physical and psychological health by creating an environment that stimulates relaxation and stress reduction (Bradt & Dileo, Citation2014; Kamioka et al., Citation2014; Koelsch & Stegemann, Citation2012; Nilsson, Citation2008). Therefore, it is timely to replicate the meta-analysis by Pelletier (Citation2004) by using new multilevel meta-analytic techniques that enable moderator analyses of both within and between study differences in outcomes, thus preventing loss of information and increasing statistical power (Assink & Wibbelink, Citation2016).

The present meta-analytic study includes all randomized controlled trials (RCTs) on the effects of music interventions on stress-related outcomes in adults, who are not suffering from dementia, that have been published. The first aim of this study is to examine whether, and the degree to which, music interventions are effective in reducing stress. The second aim is to examine possible moderator effects of study, sample and intervention characteristics, which may influence the strength of the effects of music interventions on stress-related outcomes. The results of this meta-analysis may be used to increase the effects of music interventions on stress by examining the conditions under which music interventions have the largest effects on both physiological and psychosocial dimensions of coping and stress.

Methods

Inclusion criteria

For the current meta-analysis, multiple inclusion criteria were applied. Firstly, only RCTs that examined the effect of music interventions on the experience of stress and/or (state) anxiety were included. Outcome measures related to quality of life (QoL) or pain were excluded, because these outcomes can be a response to stress, but are not measuring the primary stress-related outcome measures, upon which the present study is focused. The physiological effects of stress had to be measured by heart rate, blood pressure and hormone levels. Psychological effects of stress had to be measured by self-report instruments aiming at ‘stress’ or ‘state anxiety’. Secondly, studies including participants younger than 18 years of age or examining people with dementia were excluded. Although many studies reported cognitive and emotional benefits in dementia patients when they were singing or when they were listening to familiar songs (Särkämö et al., Citation2008, Citation2014), such findings are not directly related to ‘stress reduction’. Moreover, the regular stress measurement instruments, which are also used in the included RCTs of the present study, are not used in studies in which people with dementia are being examined.

Selection of studies

We conducted a computer-based search of the psychological and medical electronic literature databases, including Medline, Academic Search Complete, Cochrane Library, Web of Science, Wiley Online Library, SpringerLink, PubMed, PiCarta, Academic Search Premier, ScienceDirect, PsychINFO and Google Scholar. Pelletier’s meta-analysis (Citation2004) can be seen as the starting point of current meta-analysis because Pelletier also included all kinds of settings and patient groups. All RCTs available until November 2017 that were in line with the inclusion criteria were included in this meta-analysis. The electronic databases were searched using the following English search string: (music*) AND (stress* OR anxiety* OR arousal) AND (‘randomized controlled trial’ OR ‘randomised controlled trial’ OR RCT). Furthermore, reference sections of review- and meta-analytic articles about the effect of music interventions on stress-related outcomes were inspected for qualifying studies. The initial search resulted in 2679 studies. Finally, 104 studies met all the inclusion criteria (). An overview of the included studies and their main characteristics are presented in (See ).

Figure 1. Flow chart of the search results.

Figure 1. Flow chart of the search results.

Coding and moderators

The included studies were coded by the first author using a coding sheet according to the guidelines of Lipsey and Wilson (Citation2001). Stress can be considered as the dependent variable and was coded into physiological or psychological stress-related outcomes, resulting in two meta-analyses. For each meta-analysis, various factors with a potential moderating effect on the relation between music interventions and stress were identified. These moderators were divided into outcome-, study-, sample-, and intervention characteristics.

Regarding the stress-related outcomes, three different physiological outcome measures were coded: heart rate, blood pressure and hormone levels. These are three different biomarkers and each biomarker must be measured differently (Cacioppo et al., Citation2007; Pfaff et al., Citation2007). Regarding the psychological stress-related outcomes, it was coded whether the outcomes were assessed by means of questionnaires measuring stress or state anxiety. For measuring stress, the self-report questionnaires Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; Cohen et al., Citation1983) and the Visual Analogue Scale Stress (VAS-S) are widely used and 19% of the included studies measuring psychological stress-related outcomes have applied them. State anxiety is measured in multiple studies by the state version of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S; Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, Citation1983), and is used in 59% of the included studies assessing psychological stress-related outcomes (k = 76). Notably, despite the positive psychometric features of the STAI-S, the measures have been criticised for their inability to adequately discriminate between the symptoms of anxiety and depression (Caci, Bayle, Dossios, Robert, & Boyer, Citation2003; Grös, Antony, Simms, & McCabe, Citation2007).

We coded whether the study quality was strong, moderate or weak with the ‘Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies’ (Effective Public Health Practice Project [EPHPP], Citation2009). This tool assesses the quality of a study by providing a comprehensive and structured assessment of the concept of study quality (Armijo-Olivo, Stiles, Hagen, Biondo, & Cummings, Citation2012). The EPHPP has been reported to have content and construct validity (Jackson & Waters, Citation2005; Thomas, Ciliska, Dobbins, & Micucci, Citation2004). Low-quality studies negatively affect the internal (causal conclusion) validity, which can lead to a biased estimation of the overall effect estimate (Higgins & Green, Citation2011; Zeng et al., Citation2015). Regarding the setting in which the study was conducted, we coded whether the study was conducted in a polyclinic medical setting, pre-, during or after medical surgery, or whether it was a so-called nonmedical setting (e.g., work-related settings, research among students at universities or musical activities to improve health in chronical patients). Further, we coded whether the study was conducted in a Western country (European countries, Australia, USA, Canada, New Zealand) or in a non-Western country (mainly Asiatic countries). The cultural environment has been shown to influence the way people respond to and cope with stress (Lonner, Citation2007; Tweed, White, & Lehman, Citation2004), which could influence the effect of music on stress. The type of control condition was coded as care as usual (CAU) or another intervention. Different control conditions can lead to different effect sizes and recognising this is crucial for drawing accurate conclusions about treatment efficacy (Finney, Citation2000; Karlsson & Bergmark, Citation2015).

Several sample characteristics were also coded, such as the percentage of men in each study. There are indications men and women tend to react differently to stress, both psychologically and physiologically, leading to substantiated gender differences in measured stress levels (Galanakis, Stalikas, Kallia, Karagianni, & Karela, Citation2009; Kajantie & Phillips, Citation2006; Verma, Balhara, & Gupta, Citation2011). We also coded the average age of the samples per study because research on occupational stress shows the existence of several significant differences in stress levels between different ages (Galanakis et al., Citation2009).

Finally, we coded six music intervention characteristics. First, according to Bradt and Dileo (Citation2014), we coded whether the music intervention was offered in the context of music therapy by a trained music therapist, or whether the music intervention was offered by a healthcare professional, the researcher, or by the patient himself, as a music activity (Dileo, Citation2006; Dileo & Bradt, Citation2007; Kamioka et al., Citation2014; Leubner & Hinterberger, Citation2017). Second, we coded whether the music intervention involved live music or prerecorded music, because differences in effects on stress-related outcomes were found (e.g., Arnon et al., Citation2006; Bailey, Citation1983; Baker, Citation2001). Third, regarding the selection of music, it was coded whether the music was self-selected by the participants and based on own preference (bringing their own music) or whether the music was pre-selected by the researcher or the (music) therapist. Some researchers have advised to allow participants choose the music themselves, because this may have a greater stress reducing impact ( Juslin, Liljeström, Västfjäll, Barradas, & Silva, Citation2008), but this does not always mean that participants could bring their own music. On the other hand, the pre-selected music (by the researchers) is mostly based on the musical characteristics related to classical and soothing music, which are assumed to positively affect relaxation and stress reduction (Burns et al., Citation2002; Labbé, Schmidt, Babin, & Pharr, Citation2007). Fourth, we coded whether the music intervention concerned the music tempo of 60–80 bpm, or whether no specific music tempo was mentioned in the study. Fifth, it was coded whether the music contained lyrics or whether the music was purely instrumental. Lastly, we coded the number of music intervention sessions. The number of interventions is positively correlated to improvements in many outcomes including the regulation of stress and anxiety (Cassileth, Vickers, & Magill, Citation2003; Gold, Solli, Krüger, & Lie, Citation2009; Robb, Carpenter, & Burns, Citation2011).

Calculation and analyses

Data analysis was performed by the first and second author. The effect sizes were transformed into Cohen’s d by use of Wilson’s (Citation2013) calculator and Lipsey and Wilson’s (Citation2001) formulae. Negative effect sizes indicate that music interventions had a negative effect on stress-related outcomes. Most d-values were calculated based on reported means and standard deviations. To correct for pretreatment differences, pretest effects were subtracted from posttest effects. The effect size was coded as zero when a study mentioned that an effect was not significant without providing any statistics (Lipsey & Wilson, Citation2001). For both meta-analyses, the continuous moderators (age of the participants, gender of the participants, duration of the music intervention and frequency of the music intervention) were centred on their means. For categorical variables, dichotomous dummy variables were created. Extreme outliers in effect sizes were identified using box plots (Tabachnik & Fidell, Citation2013), and were winsorized (i.e., replaced by the highest or lowest acceptable score falling within the normal range) for both meta-analyses. Standard errors were estimated using Lipsey and Wilson’s (Citation2001) formulae.

In almost all the studies it was possible to calculate more than one effect size as most studies reported on multiple stress-related outcome variables, multiple scales or measurement instruments. It is possible that the effect sizes from the same study are more alike than effect sizes from other studies. The assumption of independent effect sizes underlying traditional meta-analytic methods was therefore violated (Hox, Citation2010; Lipsey & Wilson, Citation2001). In line with recently conducted meta-analyses, we applied a multilevel approach in both meta-analyses in order to deal with the interdependency of effect sizes (Assink et al., Citation2015; Cheung, Citation2014; Houben, Van Den Noortgate, & Kuppens, Citation2015; Spruit, Assink, van Vugt, van der Put, & Stams, Citation2016; ter Beek et al., Citation2018).

A three-level meta-analytic model was used to calculate the combined effect sizes and to perform the moderator analyses. Three sources of variance were modelled, including the sampling variance for each effect sizes (level-one), the variance between effect sizes within studies (level-two), and the variance between studies (level-three) (Assink & Wibbelink, Citation2016). The meta-analysis was conducted in R (version 3.4.3) with the metafor package, employing a multilevel random effects model (Houben et al., Citation2015; Van den Bussche, Van den Noortgate, & Reynvoet, Citation2009; Viechtbauer, Citation2010). This model is adequate and often used for multilevel meta-analyses and is, in general, superior to the fixed-effects approaches used in traditional meta-analyses (Van Den Noortgate & Onghena, Citation2003). We used likelihood ratio tests to compare the deviance scores of the full model and the models without variance parameters on level two or three to determine if the level-two and -three variances were significant, indicating heterogeneity of effect sizes. A heterogeneous effect size distribution indicates that the effect sizes cannot be treated as estimates of a common overall effect size. In that case, we conducted moderator analyses, because the differences among effect sizes may be explained by outcome, study, sample, and/or intervention characteristics.

Publication bias

A common problem in conducting a meta-analysis is that studies with nonsignificant or negative results are less likely to be published than studies with positive and significant results. The studies included in this meta-analysis may therefore not be an adequate representation of all studies that have been conducted. This phenomenon is called the ‘file drawer problem’ (Rosenthal, Citation1995).

In order to check the presence of publication bias in the current meta-analysis, a trim and fill procedure was performed (Duval & Tweedie, Citation2000a, Citation2000b) by testing the asymmetry of the funnel plot according to Egger’s method (Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider, & Minder, Citation1997). In case of publication bias, the funnel plot of the distribution of effect sizes is asymmetric, resulting in a significant Egger’s test. If Egger’s method indicates publication bias, a trim and fill procedure is required. We tested if effect sizes were missing on the left side of the distribution, since publication bias would only be likely to occur in the case of nonsignificant or unfavourable (i.e., negative) results. In case of left-sided funnel plot asymmetry, we imputed estimations of effect sizes of missing studies through trim and fill analyses, and subsequently computed an overall effect size that would take the influence of publication bias into account (Duval & Tweedie, Citation2000b), providing an estimate of the degree to which publication bias might have affected the overall mean effect size.

Results

This meta-analytic review included 104 RCTs (all non-overlapping samples) with a total of N = 9.617 participants of whom n = 4.838 participated in a music intervention group or music therapy group, and n = 4.779 constituting the comparison group. shows an overview of the most important characteristics of the included studies (see ). shows the overall effects of music interventions on both physiological stress-related outcomes and psychological stress-related outcomes.

Table 1. Overall effects of music interventions on physiological and psychological stress-related outcomes.

Effect of music interventions on physiological stress-related outcomes

The meta-analysis on the effect of music interventions on physiological stress-related outcomes contained 61 independent studies (s), from which 197 effect sizes (k) were denied, and a total sample of N = 3.188 participants, of which n = 1.624 participants in the music intervention groups, and n = 1.564 participants in the comparison groups.

Overall effect on physiological stress-related outcomes

A significant small-to-medium effect (d = 0.380, p ≤ .001) of music interventions on physiological stress-related outcomes (heart rate, blood pressure, stress-related hormones) was found, indicating that music interventions reduced physiological stress symptoms. According to the trim-and-fill plot, the presence of publication bias was unlikely, as there were no imputed effect sizes on the left side of the funnel (see , ). The likelihood ratio test showed that significant variance was present at the between-study level (level 3) and the within studies level (level 2). In cases of heterogeneous effect size distributions, moderator analyses are advised to assess whether the variance between the effect sizes can be explained by moderators. Therefore, we conducted moderator analyses on type of outcome, study, sample, and music intervention characteristics to examine the effect of music intervention on physiological stress-related outcomes.

Results of moderator analyses on physiological stress-related outcomes

The moderator analyses of the physiological stress-related outcomes were justified by significant overall heterogeneity for all moderator variables, including the variables with missing values. The results of these moderator analyses are presented in .

Table 2. Moderator effects of music interventions on physiological stress-related outcomes.

Outcome characteristics

We found a significant moderating effect (p ≤ .05) on the different types of physiological stress-related outcomes (heart rate, blood pressure or stress-related hormones). The strongest effects of music interventions on the physiological stress-related outcomes were measured by heart rate (d = 0.456) compared to blood pressure (d = 0.343) and hormone levels (d = 0.349).

Study characteristics

No significant moderating effects of study characteristics were found. More specifically, the type of setting (nonmedical settings, pre- or postmedical surgery or during medical procedures), the continent where the study was conducted (Western or non-Western countries), and the quality of the study (weak, moderate or strong) showed no significant moderating effects. The continent where the study had been carried out showed a trend (p = 0.089), indicating that non-Western studies yielded larger effects on physiological stress-related outcomes than studies conducted in Western countries.

Sample characteristics

The age and gender of the samples did not have a moderating effect on the physiological stress symptoms.

Intervention characteristics

No significant moderating effect was found in the type of intervention (music therapy or music intervention), type of control condition (CAU or another intervention), music induction (prerecorded music or live music), music style (predetermined relaxing music or patient preferred music), music tempo (60–80 bpm or another tempo), or the use of music with lyrics in contrast to the use of purely instrumental music.

Effect of music interventions on psychological stress-related outcomes

The meta-analysis on the effect of music interventions on physiological stress-related outcomes contained 79 independent studies (s), from which 130 effect sizes (k) were denied, and a total sample of N = 6.800 participants, of which n = 3.373 participants in the music intervention groups and n = 3.427 participants in the comparison groups.

Overall effect on psychological stress-related outcomes

A significant medium-to-large effect (d = 0.545, p ≤ .001) of music interventions on psychological stress-related outcomes was found, indicating that music interventions reduced psychological stress-related symptoms (state anxiety, nervousness, restlessness, and feelings of worry). The trim-and-fill plot did not show lack of effect sizes on the left side of the funnel (see , ), therefore publication bias was unlikely. The likelihood ratio test showed that significant variance was present at the between (level 3) and within (level 2) study level. Moderator analyses on type of outcome, study, sample, and music intervention characteristics were conducted to examine the effect of music intervention on psychological stress-related outcomes.

Results of moderator analyses on psychological stress-related outcomes

The moderator analyses of the psychological stress-related outcomes were justified by significant overall heterogeneity for all moderator variables, including the variables with missing values. The results of these moderator analyses are presented in .

Table 3. Moderator effects of music interventions on psychological stress-related outcome.

Outcome characteristic

The type of psychological outcome (stress- or state anxiety) did not moderate the effect of music interventions on psychological stress-related outcomes.

Study characteristics

No significant moderating effects were found in the study characteristics. More specifically, the continent where the study was conducted, type of setting, and quality of the study, did not moderate the effect of the music intervention on physiological stress-related outcomes.

Sample characteristics

No moderating effects of the age and gender composition of the samples were found.

Intervention characteristics

We found no significant moderating effects of the intervention characteristics, which included the type of intervention (music therapy or music intervention), music induction (prerecorded music or live music), music style (predetermined relaxing music or self-selected music by the patient), and the use of music with lyrics in contrast to the use of purely instrumental music. The music tempo showed a trend (p = .064), indicating that music with 60–80 bpm yielded larger effects than music with another or unspecified tempo. Another trend concerned the type of control condition (p = .097). If a non-established intervention aimed at stress reduction was used as a control condition, effects were somewhat larger than if the control condition concerned CAU (i.e., regular medical care). Finally, the number of interventions showed a trend (p = .078) indicating that one single session generated larger effects than two or more sessions.

Discussion

Overall effects

By conducting two separate multilevel meta-analyses, the current study aimed to assess the strength of the effect of music interventions on both physiological and psychological stress-related outcomes. Furthermore, the study aimed to examine which outcome, study, sample or intervention characteristics moderated the strength of the effect on physiological and psychological stress-related outcomes. Overall, we found a significant small-to-medium effect of music interventions on physiological stress-related outcomes (d = .380) and a medium effect of music interventions on psychological stress-related outcomes (d = .545), indicating that music intervention groups benefited more than the comparison groups. We conclude that music interventions are effective in reducing physiological and psychological stress-related symptoms in different kinds of settings (mental healthcare, polyclinic medical settings, during medical surgery and in daily life situations). There were no indications of publication bias.

The overall findings of the current study are consistent with findings of previous meta-analyses (Bradt & Dileo, Citation2014; Bradt et al., Citation2013a; Bradt et al., Citation2013b; Bradt et al., Citation2011; Gillen et al., Citation2008; Kim, Evangelista, & Park, Citation2015; Pelletier, Citation2004; Rudin et al., Citation2007). There is a growing body of evidence that music interventions yield positive, moderate effects on stress reduction. Considering the demands of today’s society, the need for stress reduction interventions are large. Millions of people around the world use tranquilising medications, such as tricyclic antidepressants and benzodiazepines, to cope with life stressors or anxiety (e.g., Bandelow et al., Citation2015; Olfson et al., Citation2015; Puetz et al., Citation2015). Not only do these types of medication have considerable negative side effects, including substance dependence and abuse, research also indicates that the effects of pharmacological treatment on stress-related problems are not much larger than the effects of music interventions found in the current meta-analytic study (Olfson et al., Citation2015). Besides, a common argument for starting pharmacological treatment instead of psychological treatment for stress reduction is that its effects occur immediately or faster (Bandelow et al., Citation2015; Fedoroff & Taylor, Citation2001). However, the results of this meta-analysis were based on mainly short-term music interventions, most of the time on a single occasion, having a direct stress reducing effect in various contexts. The current study therefore indicates the relevance of brief music interventions for stress reduction in all kinds of settings. Also, our search revealed that the effects of long-term music interventions have been seldom examined.

Psychological effects of music

Results of our meta-analysis show a medium effect of music interventions on psychological stress-related outcomes, including emotional states of subjective worry, state anxiety, restlessness and nervousness. Music not only reduces physiological arousal, but also affects emotional states. This may be attributed to the effect of music on brain areas, such as the amygdala, which are responsible for emotional processes. A related explanation for the positive effects of music interventions on psychological stress-related outcomes concerns the positive influence of listening to pleasant music on emotional valence, which can be explained by the degree of attraction that an individual feels towards a specific object or event (Jäncke, Citation2008; Juslin & Västfjäll, Citation2008). Music experienced as pleasant increases the intensity of emotional valence (the felt happiness), which has a stress-reducing effect (Jiang, Rickson, & Jiang, Citation2016; Rohner & Miller, Citation1980; Sandstrom & Russo, Citation2010; Witvliet & Vrana, Citation2007). An increased dopamine activity in the mesolimbic reward brain system has been shown to be associated with these feelings of happiness in response to high-valence music (e.g., Blood & Zatorre, Citation2001; Salimpoor et al., Citation2013; Salimpoor, Benovoy, Larcher, Dagher, & Zatorre, Citation2011; Zatorre, Citation2015).

Another explanation for the positive effect of music interventions on psychological stress-related outcomes may be that listening to music can provide ‘distraction’ from stress-increasing thoughts or feelings (Bernatzky, Presch, Anderson, & Panksepp, Citation2011; Chanda & Levitin, Citation2013). Indeed, the beneficial property of music to distract people from aversive states has been supported by short-term music interventions for acute stress reduction (Fancourt, Ockelford, & Belai, Citation2014; Linnemann et al., Citation2015). These findings are therefore in line with the current meta-analysis, which primarily included studies involving short-term music interventions.

Music listening in the presence of others may strengthen the stress-reducing effect of the music intervention, which is believed to be caused by increased emotional well-being (Juslin et al., Citation2008), and increased feelings of social cohesion (Boer & Abubakar, Citation2014; Linnemann, Strahler, & Nater, Citation2016; Pearce, Launay, & Dunbar, Citation2015). There is also empirical evidence showing that people synchronise in movement (auditory-motor synchronisation) with each other when engaging in music therapy group interventions, which evokes positive feelings of togetherness and bonding, and decreases stress levels (Linnemann et al., Citation2016; Tarr, Launay, & Dunbar, Citation2014). Group music-making or singing together may result in social bonding, which may be explained by the release of the neurotransmitters endorphin and oxytocin (e.g., Dunbar, Kaskatis, MacDonald, & Barra, Citation2012; Freeman, Citation2000; Tarr et al., Citation2014; Weinstein, Launay, Pearce, Dunbar, & Stewart, Citation2016). These neurotransmitters play a role in the defensive response to stress (Amir, Brown, & Amit, Citation1980; Dief, Sivukhina, & Jirikowski, Citation2018; Myint, Jayakumar, Hoe, Kanthimathi, & Lam, Citation2017). It should be noted that nearly all studies included in our meta-analysis examined participants listening to music alone (e.g., by using headphones), because most studies examined ‘music as medicine’ instead of music therapy, which is applied by a music therapist, and can be delivered both in a group and individual format.

Effect moderating variables

Results indicate that moderating variables may explain differences in the strength of the effect sizes. Significant stronger moderating effects were found in studies in which physiological arousal, as a result of stress, was measured by heart rate, compared to studies in which physiological arousal was measured by blood pressure or stress-related hormone levels. These results are consistent with the large body of knowledge concerning the immediate effects of psychological stress on the sympathetic responses in the autonomic nervous system, meaning increases of heart rate and decreases of heart rate variability (Chandola, Heraclides, & Kumari, Citation2010; Föhr et al., Citation2017). The results are also in line with the assumption that music with a slow steady rhythm provides stress reduction by altering inherent body rhythms, such as heart rate (Thaut & Hoemberg, Citation2014; Thaut, Kenyon, Schauer, & McIntosh, Citation1999).

None of the other possible moderators of interventions effects proved to be significant, but results showed some noteworthy trends of the intervention characteristics. First, the music tempo seemed to influence the strength of the effect of music interventions on the psychological stress-related outcomes. Larger effect sizes were found in music with a tempo of 60–80 bpm, where tempo represents slow and soothing music. This corresponds with previous research, which suggests that music with a slow tempo can be considered to be one of the most significant determinants of audio-related effects on stress reduction (Bernardi et al., Citation2006; Björkman, Karlsson, Lundberg, & Frisman, Citation2013; Iwanaga, Kobayashi, & Kawasaki, Citation2005; Jiang et al., Citation2016; Nilsson, Citation2008; ). Second, the frequency of the music intervention sessions did moderate the effect of music interventions on the psychological stress-related outcomes, indicating that only one session is needed for achieving effects on stress reduction. This is in line with previous research, which implies that music has an immediate positive effect on stress reduction (e.g., Koelsch, Citation2015; Zatorre, Citation2015). However, Leubner and Hinterberger’s (Citation2017) review of the effectiveness of music interventions on depression showed that more than one session yielded larger effects (at least, within the first 6 weeks of treatment).

Contrary to our expectations, we did not find a significant moderating effect of some intervention characteristics. Firstly, type of intervention did not moderate the effects on physiological stress-related outcomes. Studies in which music therapy is offered (by a trained therapist) did not yield significantly larger effect sizes than studies in which music interventions were offered as so-called purposeful music activities (by the researcher, healthcare professionals or self-administered by the patient). However, Dileo (Citation2006) stated that music therapy is more effective than ‘music as medicine’ interventions and attributed this difference to the fact that music therapists individualise their interventions to meet patients’ specific needs (Bradt & Dileo, Citation2014; Dileo, Citation1999, Citation2006). A possible explanation for different findings of Dileo’s review (Citation2006) compared to the current meta-analytic review is that, in the past 14 years, the number of RCTs examining the effects of ‘music as medicine’ interventions has grown substantially, and with that, both the associated research methods and intervention protocols were improved. However, experimental research on the effects of music therapy is still in its infancy, while considerable diversity is evident in the interventions that have been examined so far. Secondly, results show that the way the music was selected did not influence the effect of music interventions on stress-related outcomes, while many previous studies report that self-selected music is the most effective in terms of stress reduction (e.g., Jiang et al., Citation2016; Jiang, Zhou, Rickson, & Jiang, Citation2013; Juslin et al., Citation2008). A possible explanation is that the term ‘self-selected music’ is used differently in studies included in this meta-analytic review. The term ‘self-selected music’ was both used when the patient brings their own favourite music and when the patient could choose the music from a pre-selected list of musical styles (e.g., Bradt & Dileo, Citation2014; Cepeda, Carr, Lau, & Alvarez, Citation2006; Helsing, Västfjäll, Bjälkebring, Juslin, & Hartig, Citation2016; Lee, Chung, Chan, & Chan, Citation2005; Nilsson, Citation2008). Notably, where self-selected music also means that patients have to choose their music from a preselected list, the researcher can preselect only music with specific characteristics (nonlyrical music with a tempo of 60–80 bpm and a sound intensity level of 60 dB) which contributes to a stronger effect on physiological stress-related outcomes (Bernardi et al., Citation2006; Björkman et al., Citation2013; Dileo, 2007; Gan, Lim, & Haw, Citation2016; Good et al., Citation2000). Besides, previous research shows that listening to soothing music, pre-selected by the researcher, lowers the stress-levels significantly in contrast to energetic up-tempo music (Iwanaga et al., Citation2005; Jiang et al., Citation2013; Sandstrom & Russo, Citation2010). This differs from the findings of many studies, which attribute these positive effects to the ability of participants to self-select music (Juslin et al., Citation2008). However, this often means that the participants can choose one of the researcher’s pre-selected playlists instead of actually bringing their own music. Because of the uncertainty about what the effect actually causes, in the present study we coded ‘own preference’ when patients could bring their own music, and ‘selection by researcher/therapist’ when patients had to choose the music what was pre-selected by the researcher or therapist.

Results showed no moderating effect for the settings in which the music intervention was conducted (nonmedical, surgery, polyclinic procedures), which might indicate that the effects of music interventions do not depend on the type of setting. This is in line with the assumption that stress is a general activation response to any stimulus that could mean both a threat and a challenge, resulting in heightened arousal of the autonomic nervous system (Pfaff et al., Citation2007; Selye, Citation1976, Citation1973). Also, neurological evidence provides insight in to the positive effects of music on the stress response with respect to arousal regulation, and these effects appear to be independent of context or setting (e.g., Casey, Citation2017; Koelsch, Citation2015; Koelsch et al., Citation2016; Linnemann et al., Citation2015; Thaut & Hoemberg, Citation2014).

Limitations of the present study

The current study has some limitations that need to be mentioned. Firstly, we operationalised the concept of stress in terms of physiological and psychological outcomes, which resulted in two meta-analyses. Because the moderator analyses were performed on both psychological and physiological data, the impression can be that it concerns two independent outcomes, while the outcomes always affect and strengthen each other during stress. This is because the underlying systems of the physiological and psychological responses are both, and mostly at the same time, responsible for the experience of stress (e.g., Linnemann et al., Citation2017; McEwen & Gianaros, Citation2010). Secondly, some categories of relevant variables included in the moderator analyses contained only a few effect sizes, which reduces statistical power and sets limits to the generalizability of the study findings. This especially may be the case for the moderators music induction (prerecorded music vs. live music) and music style (relaxation vs. own choice) in the meta-analysis of the physiological stress-related outcomes and the moderators music tempo (60–80 bpm vs. another tempo), and the frequency of the intervention (a single session vs. more than one session) in the meta-analysis of the psychological stress-related outcomes.

Implications for research and practice

Despite the limitations, this study has important implications for future research and the practical use of music interventions in stress reduction. First and foremost, this meta-analytic study indicates that music interventions can be effective in the reduction of stress. Many people suffer from stress-related symptoms, both in their daily lives and in specific settings (e.g., medical settings, mental health care settings, work-related settings). Considering the fact that music interventions are very easy and inexpensive to integrate in both daily lives and in medical settings, it is important to recognise the effects of music interventions. Another implication is that future research should focus on the specific characteristics of the music intervention on stress reduction, for example, the music tempo, the style of the music, the use of live music or prerecorded music, the way the music is selected, or the frequency of the music intervention sessions.

The vast body of neurological evidence regarding the influence of music on arousal, stress and emotional processes is still growing, but the specific practical implications have not yet been sufficiently investigated. In included RCTs, music interventions are still considered like one and the same, while the specific characteristics of music show different effects, such as the music tempo (60–80 bpm). To identify the most effective music intervention for stress reduction, it is important that the possible moderators of the music interventions be better tested. Therefore, it is recommended for future trials to describe all aspects of the music intervention, with both the specific characteristics involved in these two meta-analyses as well as the musical characteristics, such as timbre, melodic and harmonic aspects, and rhythmic accentuation. These characteristics were not reported in the included RCTs, but can nonetheless moderate the effects of music interventions on stress reduction (e.g., Leubner & Hinterberger, Citation2017; Moore, Citation2013; Thaut & Hoemberg, Citation2014). There are many RCTs examining the effect of music as medicine interventions, but there is definitely a lack of RCTs examining the effect of music therapy interventions on stress-related outcomes, and it is therefore impossible to establish at this time whether these interventions are more effective than the ‘music as medicine’ interventions. Although the use of prerecorded music may be preferred by researchers as a standardised stimulus, it is recommended to also develop specific music therapy protocols that will adhere to the research standards of RCTs.

The relationship between the frequency/duration of the music intervention and the effects on stress reduction is unclear. Future research should examine this topic, because apart from the musical characteristics, such as the music style and music tempo, these general characteristics of the music intervention may also moderate the effects on stress reduction. It seems that the influence of the music selection method on the effects of the music intervention is interpreted differently. In new trials, we strongly recommend reporting clearly if the researcher/therapist did choose the music of the intervention, or if the participants had to choose one of the researchers’ or therapists’ pre-selected music playlists, or if the participants brought their own music. Finally, it is recommended to compare the effects of music (therapy) interventions on stress-related outcomes with pharmacological treatment, but also with other experiential interventions, such as yoga or mindfulness.

Summary

The current meta-analytic review provides high-level evidence that music interventions can be effective in reducing stress and provides justification for the increasing use of music interventions for stress reduction in both medical and mental health care practice. Considering the low costs and lack of side effects of music interventions, the moderate tranquilising effects of music are very significant for the prevention and treatment of stress-related problems. However, the development of music (therapy) intervention protocols are necessary to set up more robust research into the effects of music interventions, and to gain more insight into the effect moderating characteristics of music intervention for stress reduction.

Studies included in the meta-analyses

Aba, Y. A., Avci, D., Guzel, Y., Ozcelik, S. K., & Gurtekin, B. (2017). Effect of music therapy on the anxiety levels and pregnancy rate of women undergoing in vitro fertilization-embryo transfer: A randomized controlled trial. Applied Nursing Research, 36, 19–24. doi:10.1016/j.apnr.2017.05.005

Alam, M., Roongpisuthipong, W., Kim, N. A., Goyal, A., Swary, J. H., Brindise, R. T., … Yoo, S. (2016). Utility of recorded guided imagery and relaxing music in reducing patient pain and anxiety, and surgeon anxiety, during cutaneous surgical procedures: A single-blinded randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 75, 585–589. doi:10.1016/j.jaad.2016.02

Ames, N., Shuford, R., Yang, L., Moriyama, B., Frey, M., Wilson, F., … Wallen, G. R. (2017). Music listening among postoperative patients in the intensive care unit: A randomized controlled trial with mixed-methods analysis. Integrative Medicine Insights, 12, 1–13. doi:10.1177/1178633717716455

Angioli, R., Nardone, C. D. C., Plotti, F., Cafà, E. V., Dugo, N., Damiani, P., … Terranova, C. (2014). Use of music to reduce anxiety during office hysteroscopy: Prospective randomized trial. Journal of Minimally Invasive Gynecology, 21, 454–459. doi:j.jmig.2013.07.020

Bally, K., Campbell, D., Chesnick, K., & Tranmer, J. (2003). Effects of patient-controlled music therapy during coronary angiography on procedural pain and anxiety distress syndrome. Critical Care Nurse, 23, 50–58.

Bauer, C. L., Victorson, D., Rosenbloom S., Barocas, J., & Silver, R. K. (2010). Alleviating distress during antepartum hospitalization: A randomized controlled trial of music and recreation therapy. Journal of Women’s Health, 19, 523–531. doi:10.1089/jwh.2008.1344

Beck, B. D., Hansen, Å. M., & Gold, C. (2015). Coping with work-related stress through guided imagery and music (GIM): Randomized controlled trial. Journal of Music Therapy, 52, 323–352. doi:10.1093/jmt/thv011

Bekiroğlu, T., Ovayolu, N., Ergün, Y., & Ekerbiçer, H. Ç. (2013). Effect of Turkish classical music on blood pressure: A randomized controlled trial in hypertensive elderly patients. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 21, 147–154. doi:10.1016/j.ctim.2013.03.005

Bringman, H., Giesecke, K., Thörne, A., & Bringman, S. (2009). Relaxing music as pre-medication before surgery: A randomised controlled trial. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica, 53, 759–764. doi:10.1111/j.1399–6576.2009.01969.x

Buffum, M. D., Sasso, C., Sands, L. P., Lanier, E., Yellen, M., & Hayes, A. (2006). A music intervention to reduce anxiety before vascular angiography procedures. Journal of Vascular Nursing, 24, 68–73. doi:10.1016/j.jvn.2006.04.001

Bulfone, T., Quattrin, R., Zanotti, R., Regattin, L., & Brusaferro, S. (2009). Effectiveness of music therapy for anxiety reduction in women with breast cancer in chemotherapy treatment. Holistic Nursing Practice, 23, 238–242. doi:10.1097/HNP.0b013e3181aeceee

Camara, J. G., Ruszkowski, J. M., & Worak, S. R. (2008). The effect of live classical piano music on the vital signs of patients undergoing ophthalmic surgery. The Medscape Journal of Medicine, 10, 149. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2491669/

Chan, M. F., Wong, O. C., Chan, H. L., Fong, M. C., Lai, S. Y., Lo, C. W., … Leung, S. K. (2006). Effects of music on patients undergoing a C-clamp procedure after percutaneous coronary interventions. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 53, 669–679. doi:10.1111/j.1365–2648.2006.03773.x

Chen, X., Seth, R. K., Rao, V. S., Huang, J. J., & Adelman, R. A. (2012). Effects of music therapy on intravitreal injections: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Ocular Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 28, 414–419. doi:10.1089/jop.2011.0257

Cutshall, S. M., Anderson, P. G., Prinsen, S. K., Wentworth, L. J., Brekke, K. M., Li, Z., … Bauer, B. A. (2011). Effect of the combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac surgical patients: A randomized study. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 17, 16–23.

Chang, Y. H., Oh, T. H., Lee, J. W., Park, S. C., Seo, I. Y., Jeong, H. J., & Kwon, W. A. (2015). Listening to music during transrectal ultrasound-guided prostate biopsy decreases anxiety, pain and dissatisfaction in patients: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Urologia Internationalis, 94, 337–341. doi:10.1159/000368420

Chang, H. C., Yu, C. H., Chen, S. Y., & Chen, C. H. (2015). The effects of music listening on psychosocial stress and maternal-fetal attachment during pregnancy. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 23, 509–515. doi:10.1016/j.ctim.2015.05.002

Chang, M. Y., Chen, C. H., & Huang, K. F. (2008). Effects of music therapy on psychological health of women during pregnancy. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 17, 2580–2587. doi:10.1111/j.1365–2702.2007.02064.x

de la Torre-Luque, A., Caparros-Gonzalez, R. A., Bastard, T., Vico, F. J., & Buela-Casal, G. (2017a). Acute stress recovery through listening to Melomics relaxing music: A randomized controlled trial. Nordic Journal of Music Therapy, 26, 124–141. doi:10.1080/08098131.2015.1131186

de la Torre-Luque, A., Díaz-Piedra, C., & Buela-Casal, G. (2017b). Effects of preferred relaxing music after acute stress exposure: A randomized controlled trial. Psychology of Music, 6, 795–813. doi:10.1177/0305735617689953

DeMarco, J., Alexander, J. L., Nehrenz, G., & Gallagher, L. (2012). The benefit of music for the reduction of stress and anxiety in patients undergoing elective cosmetic surgery. Music and Medicine, 4, 44–48. doi:10.1177/1943862111424416

Di Nasso, L., Nizzardo, A., Pace, R., Pierleoni, F., Pagavino, G., & Giuliani, V. (2016). Clinical research: Influences of 432 hz music on the perception of anxiety during endodontic treatment: A randomized controlled clinical trial. Journal of Endodontics, 42, 1338–1343. doi:10.1016/j.joen.2016.05.015

Doğan, M. V., & Şenturan, L. (2012). The effect of music therapy on the level of anxiety in the patients undergoing coronary angiography. Open Journal of Nursing, 2, 165–169. doi:10.4236/ojn.2012.23025

Doro, C. A., Neto, J. Z., Doro, M. P., & Cunha, R. (2017). Music therapy improves the mood of patients undergoing hematopoietic stem cells transplantation (controlled randomized study). Supportive Care in Cancer, 25, 1013–1018. doi:10.1007/s00520–016–3529–z

Drzymalski, D. M., Tsen, L. C., Palanisamy, A., Zhou, J., Huang, C. C., & Kodali, B. S. (2017). A randomized controlled trial of music use during epidural catheter placement on laboring parturient anxiety, pain, and satisfaction. Anesthesia & Analgesia, 124, 542–547. doi:10.1213/ANE.0000000000001656

El-Hassan, H., McKeown, K., & Muller, A. F. (2009). Clinical trial: Music reduces anxiety levels in patients attending for endoscopy. Alimentary Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 30, 718–724. doi:10.1111/j.1365–2036.2009.04091.x

Ghetti, C. M. (2013). Effect of music therapy with emotional-approach coping on preprocedural anxiety in cardiac catheterization: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Music Therapy, 50, 93–122. doi:10.1093/jmt/50.2.93

Ghezeljeh, T. N., Ardebili, F. M., Rafii, F., & Haghani, H. (2017). The effects of patient-preferred music on anticipatory anxiety, post-procedural burn pain and relaxation level. European Journal of Integrative Medicine, 9, 141–147. doi:10.1016/j.eujim.2016.12.004

Gómez-Urquiza, J. L., Hueso-Montoro, C., Urquiza-Olmo, J., Ibarrondo-Crespo, R., González-Jiménez, E., & Schmidt-Riovalle, J. (2016). A randomized controlled trial of the effect of a photographic display with and without music on pre-operative anxiety. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 72, 1666. doi:10.1111/jan.12937

Graversen, M., & Sommer, T. (2013). Perioperative music may reduce pain and fatigue in patients undergoing laparoscopic cholecystectomy. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica, 57, 1010–1016. doi:10.1111/aas.12100

Groener, J. B., Neus, I., Kopf, S., Hartmann, M., Schanz, J., Kliemank, E., … Nawroth, P. P. (2015). Group singing as a therapy during diabetes training: A randomized controlled pilot study. Experimental and Clinical Endocrinology & Diabetes, 123, 617–621. doi:10.1055/s-0035-1555941

Gupta, U., & Gupta, B. S. (2015). Psychophysiological reactions to music in male coronary patients and healthy controls. Psychology of Music, 43, 736–755. doi:10.1177/0305735614536754

Hamel, W. J. (2001). The effects of music intervention on anxiety in the patient waiting for cardiac catheterization. Intensive and Critical Care Nursing, 17, 279–285. doi:10.1054/iccn.2001.1594

Hamidi, N., & Ozturk, E. (2017). The effect of listening to music during percutaneous nephrostomy tube placement on pain, anxiety, and success rate of procedure: A randomized prospective study. Journal of Endourology, 31, 457–460. doi:10.1089/end.2016.0843

Hammer, S. H. (1996). The effects of guided imagery through music on state and trait anxiety. Journal of Music Therapy, 33, 47–70. doi:10.1093/jmt/33.1.47

Han, L., Li, J. P., Sit, J. W., Chung, L., Jiao, Z. Y., & Ma, W. G. (2010). Effects of music intervention on physiological stress response and anxiety level of mechanically ventilated patients in China: A randomised controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 19, 978–987. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2702.2009.02845.x

Hayes, A., Buffum, M., Lanier, E., Rodahl, E., & Sasso, C. (2003). A music intervention to reduce anxiety prior to gastrointestinal procedures. Gastroenterology Nursing, 26, 145–149. doi:10.1097/00001610-200307000-00002

Horne-Thomson, A., & Grocke, D. (2008). The effect of music therapy on anxiety in patients who are terminally ill. Journal of Palliative Medicine, 11, 582–590. doi:10.1089/jpm.2007.0193

Hook, L., Songwathana, P., & Petpichetchian, W. (2008). Music therapy with female surgical patients: Effect on anxiety and pain. Pacific Rim International Journal of Nursing Research, 12, 259–271. Retrieved from https://scinapse.io/papers/1564375600

Hsu, K. C., Chen, L. F., & Hsiep, P. H. (2016). Effect of music intervention on burn patients’ pain and anxiety during dressing changes. Burns, 42, 1789–1796. doi:10.1016/j.burns.2016.05.006

Jeppesen, E., Pedersen, C. M., Larsen, K. R., Rehl, A., Bartholdy, K., Walsted, E. S., & Backer, V. (2016). Music does not alter anxiety in patients with suspected lung cancer undergoing bronchoscopy: A randomised controlled trial. European Clinical Respiratory Journal, 3, 33472. doi:10.1038/eye.2016.16010.3402/ecrj.v3.33472

Jiménez-Jiménez, M., García-Escalona, A., Martín-López, A., De Vera-Vera, R., & De Haro, J. (2013). Intraoperative stress and anxiety reduction with music therapy: A controlled randomized clinical trial of efficacy and safety. Journal of Vascular Nursing, 31, 101–106. doi:10.1016/j.jvn.2012.10.002

Johnson, B., Raymond, S., & Goss, J. (2012). Perioperative music or headsets to decrease anxiety. Journal of PeriAnesthesia Nursing, 27, 146–154. doi:10.1016/j.jopan.2012.03.001

Kahloul, M., Mhamdi, S., Nakhli, M. S., Sfeyhi, A. N., Azzaza, M., Chaouch, A., & Naija, W. (2017). Effects of music therapy under general anesthesia in patients undergoing abdominal surgery. Libyan Journal of Medicine, 12, 1260886. doi:10.1080/19932820.2017.1260886

Kar, S. K., Ganguly, T., Roy, S. S., & Goswami, A. (2015). Effect of Indian classical music (Raga therapy) on fentanyl, vecuronium, propofol requirements and cortisol levels in cardiopulmonary bypass. Journal of Anesthesia & Critical Care, 2, 00047. doi:10.15406/jaccoa.2015.02.00047

Kahloul, M., Mhamdi, S., Nakhli, M. S., Sfeyhi, A. N., Azzaza, M., Chaouch, A., & Naija, W. (2017). Effects of music therapy under general anesthesia in patients undergoing abdominal surgery. Libyan Journal of Medicine, 12, 1260886. doi:10.1080/19932820.2017.1260886

Ko, C. H., Chen, Y. Y., Wu, K. T., Wang, S. C., Yang, J. F., Lin, Y. Y., … Hsieh, M. H. (2017). Effect of music on level of anxiety in patients undergoing colonoscopy without sedation. Journal of the Chinese Medical Association, 80, 154–160. doi:10.1016/j.jcma.2016.08.010

Kunikullaya, K. U., Goturu, J., Muradi, V., Hukkeri, P. A., Kunnavil, R., Doreswamy, V., … Murthy, N. S. (2015). Music versus lifestyle on the autonomic nervous system of prehypertensives and hypertensives: A randomized control trial. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 23, 733–740. doi:10.1016/j.ctim.2015.08.003

Kushnir, J., Friedman, A., Ehrenfeld, M., & Kushnir, T. (2012). Coping with preoperative anxiety in cesarean section: Physiological, cognitive, and emotional effects of listening to favorite music. Birth, 39, 121–127. doi:10.1111/j.1523–536X.2012.00532.x

Lai, H. L., & Li, Y. M. (2011). The effect of music on biochemical markers and self-perceived stress among first-line nurses: A randomized controlled crossover trial. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 67, 2414–2424. doi:10.1111/j.1365–2648.2011.05670.x

Lai, H. L., Hwang, M. J., Chen, C. J., Chang, K. F., Peng, T. C., & Chang, F. M. (2008). Randomised controlled trial of music on state anxiety and physiological indices in patients undergoing root canal treatment. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 17, 2654–2660. doi:10.1111/j.1365–2702.2008.02350.x

Lai, H. L., Liao, K. W., Huang, C. Y., Chen, P. W., & Peng, T. C. (2013). Effects of music on immunity and physiological responses in healthcare workers: A randomized controlled trial. Stress and Health, 29, 91–98. doi:10.1002/smi.2429

Latha, R., Srikanth, S., Sairaman, H., & Dity, N. R. E. (2014). Effect of music on heart rate variability and stress in medical students. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Physiology, 1, 131–134. doi:10.4103/2348–8093.137409

Leardi, S., Pietroletti, R., Angeloni, G., Necozione, S., Ranalletta, G., & Del Gusto, B. (2007). Randomized clinical trial examining the effect of music therapy in stress response to day surgery. British Journal of Surgery, 94, 943–947. doi:10.1002/bjs.5914

Lee, K. C., Chao, Y. H., Yiin, J. J., Chiang, P. Y., & Chao, Y. F. (2011). Effectiveness of different music-playing devices for reducing preoperative anxiety: A clinical control study. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 48, 1180–1187. doi:10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2011.04.001

Lee, K. S., Jeong, H. C., Yim, J. E., & Jeon, M. Y. (2016). Effects of music therapy on the cardiovascular and autonomic nervous system in stress-induced university students: A randomized controlled trial. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 22, 59–65. doi:10.1089/acm.2015.0079

Lee, C. H., Lee, C. Y., Hsu, M. Y., Lai, C. L., Sung, Y. H., Lin, C. Y., & Lin, L. Y. (2017). Effects of music intervention on state anxiety and physiological indices in patients undergoing mechanical ventilation in the intensive care unit: A randomized controlled trial. Biological Research for Nursing, 19, 137–144. doi:10.1177/1099800416669601

Lee, W. L., Sung, H. C., Liu, S. H., & Chang, S. M. (2017). Meditative music listening to reduce state anxiety in patients during the uptake phase before positron emission tomography (PET) scans. The British Journal of Radiology, 90, 20160466. doi:10.1259/bjr.20160466

Lee, W., Wu, P., Lee, M., Ho, L., & Shih, W. (2017). Music listening alleviates anxiety and physiological responses in patients receiving spinal anesthesia. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 31, 8–13. doi:10.1016/j.ctim.2016.12.006

Lin, M. F., Hsieh, Y. J., Hsu, Y. Y., Fetzer, S., & Hsu, M. C. (2011). A randomised controlled trial of the effect of music therapy and verbal relaxation on chemotherapy-induced anxiety. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 20, 988–999. doi:10.1111/j.1365–2702.2010.03525.x

Lin, Y. J., Lu, K. C., Chen, C. M., & Chang, C. C. (2012). The effects of music as therapy on the overall well-being of elderly patients on maintenance hemodialysis. Biological Research for Nursing, 14, 277–285. doi:10.1177/1099800411413259

López-Cepero Andrada, J. M., Amaya Vidal, A., Castro Aguilar-Tablada, T., García Reina, I., Silva, L., Ruiz Guinaldo, A., … Benítez Roldán, A. (2004). Anxiety during the performance of colonoscopies: Modification using music therapy. European Journal of Gastroenterology & Hepatology, 16, 1381–1386. doi:10.1097/00042737-200412000-00024

Mahdipour Raberi, R., & Nematollahi, M. (2012). The effect of the music listening and the intensive care unit visit program on the anxiety, stress and depression levels of the heart surgery patients candidates. Journal of Critical Care Nursing, 5, e7060. Retrieved from http://www.sid.ir/En/Journal/ViewPaper.aspx?ID=286132

Mandel, S. E., Hanser, S. B., Secic, M., & Davis, B. A. (2007). Effects of music therapy on health-related outcomes in cardiac rehabilitation: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Music Therapy, 44, 176–197. doi:10.1093/jmt/44.3.176

Menegazzi, J. J., Paris, P. M., Kersteen, C. H., Flynn, B., & Trautman, D. E. (1991). A randomized, controlled trial of the use of music during laceration repair. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 20, 348–350. doi:10.1016/S0196-0644(05)81652-X

Miyata, K., Odanaka, H., Nitta, Y., Shimoji, S., Kanehira, T., Kawanami, M., & Fujisawa, T. (2016). Music before dental surgery suppresses sympathetic activity derived from preoperative anxiety: A randomized controlled trial. JDR Clinical & Translational Research, 1, 153–162. doi:10.1177/2380084416650613

Ng, M. Y., Karimzad, Y., Menezes, R. J., Wintersperger, B. J., Li, Q., Forero, J., … Nguyen, E. T. (2016). Randomized controlled trial of relaxation music to reduce heart rate in patients undergoing cardiac CT. European Radiology, 26, 3635–3642. doi:10.1007/s00330-016-4215-8

Ni, C. H., Tsai, W. H., Lee, L. M., Kao, C. C., & Chen, Y. C. (2012). Minimising preoperative anxiety with music for day surgery patients: A randomised clinical trial. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 21, 620–625. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2702.2010.03466.x

Nilsson, U. (2009). The effect of music intervention in stress response to cardiac surgery in a randomized clinical trial. Heart & Lung, 38, 201–207. doi:10.1016/j.hrtlng.2008.07.008

Nilsson, U., Unosson, M., & Rawal, N. (2005). Stress reduction and analgesia in patients exposed to calming music postoperatively: A randomized controlled trial. European Journal of Anaesthesiology, 22, 96–102. doi:10.1017/S0265021505000189

O’Callaghan, C., Sproston, M., Wilkinson, K., Willis, D., Milner, A., Grocke, D., & Wheeler, G. (2012). Effect of self-selected music on adults’ anxiety and subjective experiences during initial radiotherapy treatment: A randomised controlled trial and qualitative research. Journal of Medical Imaging and Radiation Oncology, 56, 473–477. doi:10.1111/j.1754–9485.2012.02395.x

Padam, A., Sharma, N., Sastri, O. S., Mahajan, S., Sharma, R., & Sharma, D. (2017). Effect of listening to Vedic chants and Indian classical instrumental music on patients undergoing upper gastrointestinal endoscopy: A randomized control trial. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 59, 214–218. doi:10.4103/psychiatry.IndianJPsychiatry_314_16

Pothoulaki, M., MacDonald, R. A. R., Flowers, P., Stamataki, E., Filiopoulos, V., Stamatiadis, D., & Stathakis, C. P. (2008). An investigation of the effects of music on anxiety and pain perception in patients undergoing haemodialysis treatment. Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 912–920. doi:10.1177/1359105308095065

Radstaak, M., Geurts, S. A., Brosschot, J. F., & Kompier, M. A. (2014). Music and psychophysiological recovery from stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 76, 529–537. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000094

Romito, F., Lagattolla, F., Costanzo, C., Giotta, F., & Mattioli, V. (2013). Music therapy and emotional expression during chemotherapy: How do breast cancer patients feel? European Journal of Integrative Medicine, 5, 438–442. doi:10.1016/j.eujim.2013.04.001

Ripley, L., Christopoulos, G., Michael, T. T., Alomar, M., Rangan, B. V., Roesle, M., … Brilakis, E. S. (2014). Randomized controlled trial on the impact of music therapy during cardiac catheterization on reactive hyperemia index and patient satisfaction: The Functional change in Endothelium after cardiac Catheterization, with and without Music Therapy (FEAT) study. The Journal of Invasive Cardiology, 26, 437–442. Retrieved from https://www.invasivecardiology.com/articles/randomized-controlled-trial-impact-music-therapy-during-cardiac-catheterization-reactive

Salehi, B., Salehi, M., Nsirnia, K., Soltani, P., Adalatnaghad, M., Kalantari, N., … Moghaddam, S. (2016). The effects of selected relaxing music on anxiety and depression during hemodialysis: A randomized crossover controlled clinical trial study. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 48, 76–80. doi:10.1016/j.aip.2016.03.003

Sanal, A. M., & Gorsev, S. (2014). Psychological and physiological effects of singing in a choir. Psychology of Music, 42, 420–429. doi:10.1177/0305735613477181

Sendelbach, S. E., Halm, M. A., Doran, K. A., Miller, E. H., & Gaillard, P. (2006). Effects of music therapy on physiological and psychological outcomes for patients undergoing cardiac surgery. Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing, 21, 194–200. doi:10.1097/00005082-200605000-00007

Shabanloei, R., Golchin, M., Esfahani, A., Dolatkhah, R., & Rasoulian, M. (2010). Effects of music therapy on pain and anxiety in patients undergoing bone marrow biopsy and aspiration. AORN Journal, 91, 746–751. doi:10.1016/j.aorn.2010.04.001

Shin, H. S., & Kim, J. H. (2011). Music therapy on anxiety, stress and maternal-fetal attachment in pregnant women during transvaginal ultrasound. Asian Nursing Research, 5, 19–27. doi:10.1016/S1976-1317(11)60010-8

Simavli, S., Kaygusuz, I., Gumus, I., Usluogulları, B., Yildirim, M., & Kafali, H. (2014). Effect of music therapy during vaginal delivery on postpartum pain relief and mental health. Journal of Affective Disorders, 156, 194–199. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.12.027

Smith, M. (2008). The effects of a single music relaxation session on state anxiety levels of adults in a workplace environment. The Australian Journal of Music Therapy, 19, 45–66. Retrieved from https://www.austmta.org.au/journal/article/effects-single-music-relaxation-session-state-anxiety-levels-adults-workplace

Solomon, E. R., & Ridgeway, B. (2016). Interventions to decrease pain and anxiety in patients undergoing urodynamic testing: A randomized controlled trial. Neurourology & Urodynamics, 35, 975. doi:10.1002/nau.22840

Soo, M. S., Jarosz, J. A., Wren, A. A., Soo, A. E., Mowery, Y. M., Johnson, K. S., … Shelby, R. A. (2016). Imaging-guided core-needle breast biopsy: Impact of meditation and music interventions on patient anxiety, pain, and fatigue. Journal of the American College of Radiology, 13, 526–534. doi:10.1016/j.jacr.2015.12.004

Stein, T. R., Olivo, E. L., Grand, S. H., Namerow, P. B., Costa, J., & Oz, M. C. (2010). A pilot study to assess the effects of a guided imagery audiotape intervention on psychological outcomes in patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Holistic Nursing Practice, 24, 213–222. doi:10.1097/HNP.0b013e3181e90303

Su, C. P., Lai, H. L., Chang, E. T., Yiin, L. M., Perng, S. J., & Chen, P. W. (2013). A randomized controlled trial of the effects of listening to non-commercial music on quality of nocturnal sleep and relaxation indices in patients in medical intensive care unit. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 69, 1377–1389. doi:10.1111/j.1365–2648.2012.06130.x

Tabrizi, E. M., Sahraei, H., Rad, S. M., Hajizadeh, E., & Lak, M. (2012). The effect of music on the level of cortisol, blood glucose and physiological variables in patients undergoing spinal anesthesia. EXCLI Journal, 11, 556–565. doi:10.17877/DE290R-10357

Tan, Y. Z., Ozdemir, S., Temiz, A., & Celik, F. (2015). The effect of relaxing music on heart rate and heart rate variability during ECG GATED-myocardial perfusion scintigraphy. Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice, 21, 137–140. doi:10.1016/j.ctcp.2014.12.003

Thoma, M. V., Zemp, M., Kreienbühl, L., Hofer, D., Schmidlin, P. R., Attin, T., … Nater, U. M. (2015). Effects of music listening on pre-treatment anxiety and stress levels in a dental hygiene recall population. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 22, 498–505. doi:10.1007/s12529-014-9439-x

Toker, E., & Kömürcü, N. (2017). Effect of Turkish classical music on prenatal anxiety and satisfaction: A randomized controlled trial in pregnant women with pre-eclampsia. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 30, 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.ctim.2016.11.005

Triller, N., Eržen, D., Duh, Š., Primožič, M. P., & Košnik, M. (2006). Music during bronchoscopic examination: The physiological effects. Respiration, 73, 95–99. doi:10.1159/000089818

Uedo, N., Ishikawa, H., Morimoto, K., Ishihara, R., Narahara, H., Akedo, I., … Fukuda, S. (2004). Reduction in salivary cortisol level by music therapy during colonoscopic examination. Hepato-gastroenterology, 51, 451–453. Retrieved from https://europepmc.org/abstract/med/15086180

Vachiramon, V., Sobanko, J. F., Rattanaumpawan, P., & Miller, C. J. (2013). Music reduces patient anxiety during Mohs surgery: An open-label randomized controlled trial. Dermatologic Surgery, 39, 298–305. doi:10.1111/dsu.12047

Vickers, A. (2003). Music therapy for mood disturbance during hospitalization for autologous stem cell transplantation: A randomized controlled trial. Cancer, 98, 2723–2729. doi:10.1097/10.1002/cncr.11842

Voss, J. A., Good, M., Yates, B., Baun, M. M., Thompson, A., & Hertzog, M. (2004). Sedative music reduces anxiety and pain during chair rest after open-heart surgery. Pain, 112, 197–203. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2004.08.020

Wang, Y., Tang, H., Guo, Q., Liu, J., Liu, X., Luo, J., & Yang, W. (2015). Effects of intravenous patient-controlled sufentanil analgesia and music therapy on pain and hemodynamics after surgery for lung cancer: A randomized parallel study. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 21, 667–672. doi:10.1089/acm.2014.0310

Warth, M., Kessler, J., Hillecke, T. K., & Bardenheuer, H. J. (2016). Trajectories of terminally ill patients’ cardiovascular response to receptive music therapy in palliative care. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 52, 196–204. doi:10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2016.01.008

White, J. M. (1992). Music therapy: An intervention to reduce anxiety in the myocardial infarction patient. Clinical Nurse Specialist, 6, 58–63. Retrieved from https://europepmc.org/abstract/med/1617576

Wiwatwongwana, D., Vichitvejpaisal, P., Thaikruea, L., Klaphajone, J., Tantong, A., & Wiwatwongwana, A. (2016). The effect of music with and without binaural beat audio on operative anxiety in patients undergoing cataract surgery: A randomized controlled trial. Eye, 30, 1407–1414. doi:10.1038/eye.2016.160

Yeo, J. K., Cho, D. Y., Oh, M. M., Park, S. S., & Park, M. G. (2013). Listening to music during cystoscopy decreases anxiety, pain, and dissatisfaction in patients: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Journal of Endourology, 27, 459–462. doi:10.1089/end.2012.0222

Zengin, S., Kabul, S., Al, B., Sarcan, E., Doğan, M., & Yildirim, C. (2013). Effects of music therapy on pain and anxiety in patients undergoing port catheter placement procedure. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 21, 689–696. doi:10.1016/j.ctim.2013.08.017

Zhang, Z. S., Wang, X. L., Xu, C. L., Zhang, C., Cao, Z., Xu, W. D., … Sun, Y. H. (2014). Music reduces panic: An initial study of listening to preferred music improves male patient discomfort and anxiety during flexible cystoscopy. Journal of Endourology, 28, 739–744. doi:10.1089/end.2013.0705

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download PDF (348.9 KB)

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek: [grant number 023.007.068].

References

  • Akin, A., & Iskender, M. (2011). Internet addiction and depression, anxiety and stress. International Online Journal of Educational Sciences, 3, 138–148. Retrieved from http://mts.iojes.net/
  • Aldwin, C. M. (2007). Stress, coping, and development: An integrative perspective (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • American Music Therapy Association. (2019). What is music therapy? Retrieved from https://www.musictherapy.org/about/musictherapy
  • American Psychological Association. (2017). Stress in America: Coping with change. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/news/press/releases/stress/2016/coping-with-change.pdf
  • Amir, S., Brown, Z. W., & Amit, Z. (1980). The role of endorphins in stress: Evidence and speculations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 4, 77–86.
  • Armijo-Olivo, S., Stiles, C. R., Hagen, N. A., Biondo, P. D., & Cummings, G. G. (2012). Assessment of study quality for systematic reviews: A comparison of the Cochrane collaboration risk of bias tool and the effective public health practice project quality assessment tool. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 18, 12–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2753.2010.01516.x
  • Arnon, S., Shapsa, A., Forman, L., Regev, R., Bauer, S., Litmanovitz, I., & Dolfin, T. (2006). Live music is beneficial to preterm infants in the neonatal intensive care unit environment. Birth (berkeley, Calif ), 33, 131–136. doi:10.1111/j.0730–7659.2006.00090.x
  • Assink, M., van der Put, C. E., Hoeve, M., de Vries, S. L., Stams, G. J. J., & Oort, F. J. (2015). Risk factors for persistent delinquent behavior among juveniles: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 47–61. doi:10.1016/j. cpr.2015.08.002
  • Assink, M., & Wibbelink, C. J. M. (2016). Fitting three-level meta-analytic models in R: A step-by-step tutorial. The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 12, 154–174. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.12.3.p154
  • Australian Psychological Society. (2015). Stress & wellbeing: How Australians are coping with life: The findings of the Australian Psychological Society Stress and wellbeing in Australia survey 2015. Retrieved from https://www.headsup.org.au/docs/default-source/default-document-library/stress-and-wellbeing-in-australia-report.pdf
  • Bailey, L. M. (1983). The effects of live music versus tape-recorded music on hospitalized cancer patients. Music Therapy, 3, 17–28. doi: 10.1093/mt/3.1.17
  • Baker, F. (2001). The effects of live, taped, and no music on people experiencing posttraumatic amnesia. Journal of Music Therapy, 38, 170–192. doi: 10.1093/jmt/38.3.170
  • Bally, K., Campbell, D., Chesnick, K., & Tranmer, J. (2003). Effects of patient-controlled music therapy during coronary angiography on procedural pain and anxiety distress syndrome. Critical Care Nurse, 23, 50–58.
  • Bandelow, B., Reitt, M., Röver, C., Michaelis, S., Görlich, Y., & Wedekind, D. (2015). Efficacy of treatments for anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 30, 183–192. doi: 10.1097/YIC.0000000000000078
  • Bernardi, L., Porta, C., & Sleight, P. (2006). Cardiovascular, cerebrovascular, and respiratory changes induced by different types of music in musicians and nonmusicians: The importance of silence. Heart, 92, 445–452. doi: 10.1136/hrt.2005.064600
  • Bernatzky, G., Presch, M., Anderson, M., & Panksepp, J. (2011). Emotional foundations of music as a non-pharmacological pain management tool in modern medicine. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1989–1999. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.06.005
  • Björkman, I., Karlsson, F., Lundberg, A., & Frisman, G. H. (2013). Gender differences when using sedative music during colonoscopy. Gastroenterology Nursing, 36, 14–20. doi: 10.1097/SGA.0b013e31827c4c80
  • Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 11818–11823. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191355898
  • Boer, D., & Abubakar, A. (2014). Music listening in families and peer groups: Benefits for young people’s social cohesion and emotional well-being across four cultures. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 392. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00392
  • Bradt, J., & Dileo, C. (2014). Music interventions for mechanically ventilated patients. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2014(12), 1–73. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD006902.pub3
  • Bradt, J., Dileo, C., Grocke, D., & Magill, L. (2011). Music interventions for improving psychological and physical outcomes in cancer patients. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2011(8), 1–172. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD006911.pub3
  • Bradt, J., Dileo, C., & Potvin, N. (2013a). Music for stress and anxiety reduction in coronary heart disease patients. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2013(12), 1–106.
  • Bradt, J., Dileo, C., & Shim, M. (2013b). Music interventions for preoperative anxiety. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2013(6), 1–84.
  • Bringman, H., Giesecke, K., Thörne, A., & Bringman, S. (2009). Relaxing music as pre-medication before surgery: A randomised controlled trial. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica, 53, 759–764.
  • Burns, J. L., Labbé, E., Arke, B., Capeless, K., Cooksey, B., Steadman, A., & Gonzales, C. (2002). The effects of different types of music on perceived and physiological measures of stress. Journal of Music Therapy, 39, 101–116. doi: 10.1093/jmt/39.2.101
  • Caci, H., Bayle, F. J., Dossios, C., Robert, P., & Boyer, P. (2003). The Spielberger trait anxiety inventory measures more than anxiety. European Psychiatry, 18, 394–400. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2003.05.003
  • Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. (Eds.). (2007). Handbook of psychophysiology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Casey, G. (2017). Stress and disease. Kai Tiaki Nursing New Zealand, 23(6), 20–24. Retrieved from https://www.highbeam.com/doc/1G1-498996813.html
  • Cassileth, B. R., Vickers, A. J., & Magill, L. A. (2003). Music therapy for mood disturbance during hospitalization for autologous stem cell transplantation: A randomized controlled trial. Cancer, 98, 2723–2729. doi: 10.1002/cncr.11842
  • Cepeda, M. S., Carr, D. B., Lau, J., & Alvarez, H. (2006). Music for pain relief. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2006(2), 1–4. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004843.pub2
  • Chanda, M. L., & Levitin, D. J. (2013). The neurochemistry of music. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 179–193. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.02.007
  • Chandola, T., Heraclides, A., & Kumari, M. (2010). Psychophysiological biomarkers of workplace stressors. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.005
  • Cheung, M. W. L. (2014). Modeling dependent effect sizes with three-level meta-analyses: A structural equation modeling approach. Psychological Methods, 19, 211–229. doi: 10.1037/a0032968
  • Chlan, L. L. (2000). Music therapy as a nursing intervention for patients supported by mechanical ventilation. AACN Advanced Critical Care, 11, 128–138. doi: 10.1097/00044067-200002000-00014
  • Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404
  • Dief, A. E., Sivukhina, E. V., & Jirikowski, G. F. (2018). Oxytocin and stress response. Open Journal of Endocrine and Metabolic Diseases, 8, 93–104. doi: 10.4236/ojemd.2018.83010
  • Dileo, C. (2006). Effects of music and music therapy on medical patients: A meta-analysis of the research and implications for the future. Journal of The Society for Integrative Oncology, 4, 67–70. doi: 10.2310/7200.2006.002
  • Dileo, C., & Bradt, J. (2007). Music therapy: Applications to stress management. In P. Lehrer & R. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (3rd ed, pp. 519–544). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Dunbar, R. I., Kaskatis, K., MacDonald, I., & Barra, V. (2012). Performance of music elevates pain threshold and positive affect: Implications for the evolutionary function of music. Evolutionary Psychology, 10, 688–702. doi:10.1177%2F147470491201000403
  • Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000a). A nonparametric ‘trim and fill’ method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 95, 89–99. doi: 10.2307/2669529
  • Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000b). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56, 455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x
  • Effective Public Health Practice Project. (2009). Quality assessment tool for quantitative studies. Retrieved from http://www.ephpp.ca/tools.html
  • Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. E. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7129.469
  • Fancourt, D., Ockelford, A., & Belai, A. (2014). The psychoneuroimmunological effects of music: A systematic review and a new model. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 36, 15–26. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.10.014
  • Fedoroff, I. C., & Taylor, S. (2001). Psychological and pharmacological treatments of social phobia: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 21, 311–324. doi:10.1097/00004714–200106000–00011
  • Finney, J. (2000). Limitations in using existing alcohol treatment trials to develop practice guidelines. Addiction, 95, 1491–1500. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.951014914.x
  • Föhr, T., Tolvanen, A., Myllymäki, T., Järvelä-Reijonen, E., Peuhkuri, K., Rantala, S., … Puttonen, S. (2017). Physical activity, heart rate variability-based stress and recovery, and subjective stress during a 9-month study period. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 27, 612–621. doi: 10.1111/sms.12683
  • Freeman, W. J., III. (2000). A neurobiological role of music in social bonding. In N. Wallin, B. Merkur, & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 411–424). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Galanakis, M., Stalikas, A., Kallia, H., Karagianni, C., & Karela, C. (2009). Gender differences in experiencing occupational stress: The role of age, education and marital status. Stress & Health, 25, 397–404. doi: 10.1002/smi.1248
  • Gan, S. K. E., Lim, K. M. J., & Haw, Y. X. (2016). The relaxation effects of stimulative and sedative music on mathematics anxiety: A perception to physiology model. Psychology of Music, 44, 730–741. doi: 10.1177/0305735615590430
  • Gillen, E., Biley, F., & Allen, D. (2008). Effects of music listening on adult patients’ pre-procedural state anxiety in hospital. International Journal of Evidence-Based Healthcare, 6, 24–49. doi:10.1111/j.1744–1609.2007.00097.x
  • Gold, C., Erkkila, J., Bonde, L. O., Trondalen, G., Maratos, A., & Crawford, M. J. (2011). Music therapy or music medicine? Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 80, 304–304. doi: 10.1159/000323166
  • Gold, C., Solli, H. P., Krüger, V., & Lie, S. A. (2009). Dose-response relationship in music therapy for people with serious mental disorders: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 193–207. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.01.001
  • Good, M., Picot, B. L., Salem, S. G., Chin, C. C., Picot, S. F., & Lane, D. (2000). Cultural differences in music chosen for pain relief. Journal of Holistic Nursing, 18, 245–260. doi: 10.1177/089801010001800306
  • Grös, D. F., Antony, M. M., Simms, L. J., & McCabe, R. E. (2007). Psychometric properties of the state-trait inventory for cognitive and somatic anxiety (STICSA): Comparison to the state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI). Psychological Assessment, 19, 369–381. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.4.369
  • Halpern, S., & Savary, L. (1985). Sound health: Music and sounds that make us whole. San Francisco, CA: Harper & Row.
  • Heiderscheit, A., Chlan, L., & Donley, K. (2011). Instituting a music listening intervention for critically ill patients receiving mechanical ventilation: Exemplars from two patient cases. Music and Medicine, 3, 239–245. doi: 10.1177/1943862111410981
  • Helsing, M., Västfjäll, D., Bjälkebring, P., Juslin, P., & Hartig, T. (2016). An experimental field study of the effects of listening to self-selected music on emotions, stress, and cortisol levels. Music and Medicine, 8, 187–198.
  • Higgins, J., & Green, S. (Eds.). (2011). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions: Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Hilz, M. J., Stadler, P., Gryc, T., Nath, J., Habib-Romstoeck, L., Stemper, B., … Koehn, J. (2014). Music induces different cardiac autonomic arousal effects in young and older persons. Autonomic Neuroscience, 183, 83–93. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2014.02.004
  • Hodges, D. A. (2011). Psychophysiological measures. In P. N. Juslin & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion (pp. 279–311). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Holahan, C. J., Moos, R. H., Holahan, C. K., Brennan, P. L., & Schutte, K. K. (2005). Stress generation, avoidance coping, and depressive symptoms: A 10-year model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 658–666. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.4.658
  • Hook, L., Songwathana, P., & Petpichetchian, W. (2008). Music therapy with female surgical patients: Effect on anxiety and pain. Pacific Rim International Journal of Nursing Research, 12, 259–271. Retrieved from https://tci-thaijo.org/index.php/PRIJNR/article/view/5863/5067
  • Houben, M., Van Den Noortgate, W., & Kuppens, P. (2015). The relation between shortterm emotion dynamics and psychological well-being: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 901–930. doi: 10.1037/a0038822
  • Howe, M., Chang, C. H., & Johnson, R. E. (2013). Understanding affect, stress, and well-being within a self-regulation framework. In P. L. Perrewé, C. C. Rosen, & J. R. B. Halbesleben (Eds.), The role of emotion and emotion regulation in job stress and wellbeing (pp. 1–34). Bingley: Emerald.
  • Hox, J. (2010). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications. New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Iwanaga, M., Kobayashi, A., & Kawasaki, C. (2005). Heart rate variability with repetitive exposure to music. Biological Psychology, 70, 61–66. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.015
  • Jackson, N., & Waters, E. (2005). Criteria for the systematic review of health promotion and public health interventions. Health Promotion International, 20, 367–374. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dai022
  • Jäncke, L. (2008). Music, memory and emotion. Journal of Biology, 7, 21. doi: 10.1186/jbiol82
  • Jiang, J., Rickson, D., & Jiang, C. (2016). The mechanism of music for reducing psychological stress: Music preference as a mediator. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 48, 62–68. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2016.02.002
  • Jiang, J., Zhou, L., Rickson, D., & Jiang, C. (2013). The effects of sedative and stimulative music on stress reduction depend on music preference. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 40, 201–205. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2013.02.002
  • Juslin, P. N., Liljeström, S., Västfjäll, D., Barradas, G., & Silva, A. (2008). An experience sampling study of emotional reactions to music. Emotion, 8, 668–683. doi: 10.1037/a0013505
  • Juslin, P. N., & Västfjäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: The need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 559–575. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08005293
  • Kajantie, E., & Phillips, D. I. (2006). The effects of sex and hormonal status on the physiological response to acute psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 151–178. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.002
  • Kamioka, H., Tsutani, K., Yamada, M., Park, H., Okuizumi, H., Tsuruoka, K., … Abe, T. (2014). Effectiveness of music therapy: A summary of systematic reviews based on randomised controlled trials of music interventions. Patient Preference and Adherence, 8, 727–754. doi: 10.2147/PPA.S61340
  • Karlsson, P., & Bergmark, A. (2015). Compared with what? An analysis of control-group types in Cochrane and Campbell reviews of psychosocial treatment efficacy with substance use disorders. Addiction, 110, 420–428. doi: 10.1111/add.12799
  • Kim, Y., Evangelista, L. S., & Park, Y. G. (2015). Anxiolytic effects of music interventions in patients receiving incenter hemodialysis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Nephrology Nursing Journal, 42, 339–347. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.11.3.187
  • Koelsch, S. (2012). Brain and music. Oxford: John Wiley.
  • Koelsch, S. (2015). Music-evoked emotions: Principles, brain correlates, and implications for therapy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1337, 193–201. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12684
  • Koelsch, S., Boehlig, A., Hohenadel, M., Nitsche, I., Bauer, K., & Sack, U. (2016). The impact of acute stress on hormones and cytokines, and how their recovery is affected by music-evoked mood. Scientific Reports, 6, 23008. doi: 10.1038/srep23008
  • Koelsch, S., Fuermetz, J., Sack, U., Bauer, K., Hohenadel, M., Wiegel, M., … Heinke, W. (2011a). Effects of music listening on cortisol levels and propofol consumption during spinal anesthesia. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 58.
  • Koelsch, S., Offermanns, K., & Franzke, P. (2010). Music in the treatment of affective disorders: An exploratory investigation of a new method for music-therapeutic research. Music Perception, 27, 307–316. doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.27.4.307
  • Koelsch, S., Siebel, W., & Fritz, T. (2011b). Functional neuroimaging. In P. N. Juslin & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion (pp. 313–345). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Koelsch, S., & Stegemann, T. (2012). The brain and positive biological effects in healthy and clinical populations. In R. A. R. MacDonald, D. Kreutz, & L. Mitchell (Eds.), Music, health, and wellbeing (pp. 436–456). doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2011.04.003
  • Kreutz, G., Murcia, C. Q., & Bongard, S. (2012). Psychoneuroendocrine research on music and health: An overview. In R. A. R. MacDonald, D. Kreutz, & L. Mitchell (Eds.), Music, health, and wellbeing (pp. 457–476). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Labbé, E., Schmidt, N., Babin, J., & Pharr, M. (2007). Coping with stress: The effectiveness of different types of music. Applied Psychophysiology & Biofeedback, 32, 163–168. doi: 10.1007/s10484-007-9043-9
  • Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  • Leardi, S., Pietroletti, R., Angeloni, G., Necozione, S., Ranalletta, G., & Del Gusto, B. (2007). Randomised clinical trial examining the effect of music therapy in stress response to day surgery. British Journal of Surgery, 94, 943–947. doi: 10.1002/bjs.5914
  • Lee, O. K. A., Chung, Y. F. L., Chan, M. F., & Chan, W. M. (2005). Music and its effect on the physiological responses and anxiety levels of patients receiving mechanical ventilation: A pilot study. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 14, 609–620. doi:10.1111/j.1365–2702.2004.01103.x
  • Leubner, D., & Hinterberger, T. (2017). Reviewing the effectiveness of music interventions in treating depression. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1109. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01109
  • Levitin, D. J. (2009). The Neural Correlates of Temporal Structure in Music. Music and Medicine, 1(1), 9–13.
  • Li, A. W., & Goldsmith, C.-A. W. (2012). The effects of yoga on anxiety and stress. Alternative Medicine Review, 17(1), 21–35. Retrieved from http://archive.foundationalmedicinereview.com/publications/17/1/21.pdf
  • Limb, C. L., & Braun, A. R. (2008). Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: An fMRI study of jazz improvisation. PLoS One, 3, 1–9. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001679
  • Linnemann, A., Ditzen, B., Strahler, J., Doerr, J. M., & Nater, U. M. (2015). Music listening as a means of stress reduction in daily life. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 60, 82–90. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.06.008
  • Linnemann, A., Strahler, J., & Nater, U. M. (2016). The stress-reducing effect of music listening varies depending on the social context. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 72, 97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.06.003
  • Linnemann, A., Strahler, J., & Nater, U. M. (2017). Assessing the effects of music listening on psychobiological stress in daily life. Journal of Visualized Experiments, 120, e54920. doi: 10.3791/54920
  • Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Lonner, W. J. (2007). Foreword. In P. T. Wong & L. C. Wong (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural perspectives on stress and coping (pp. v–vii). New York, NY: Springer.
  • McCance, K. L., Forshee, B., & Shelby, J. (2006). Stress and disease. In K. L. McCance & S. E. Huether (Eds.), Pathophysiology: The biologic basis for disease in adults and children (5th ed., pp. 311–332). St. Louis, MO: Elsevier.
  • McEwen, B. S., & Gianaros, P. J. (2010). Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: Links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Annals of The New York Academy of Sciences, 1186, 190–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05331.x
  • Moore, K. S. (2013). A systematic review on the neural effects of music on emotion regulation: Implications for music therapy practice. Journal of Music Therapy, 50, 198–242. doi: 10.1093/jmt/50.3.198
  • Dileo, C. (1999). Music therapy and medicine: Theoretical and clinical applications. Silver Spring: American Music Therapy Association.
  • Myint, K., Jayakumar, R., Hoe, S. Z., Kanthimathi, M. S., & Lam, S. K. (2017). Cortisol, β-endorphin and oxidative stress markers in healthy medical students in response to examination stress. Biomedical Research, 28, 3774–3779.
  • Ng, M. Y., Karimzad, Y., Menezes, R. J., Wintersperger, B. J., Li, Q., Forero, J., … Nguyen, E. T. (2016). Randomized controlled trial of relaxation music to reduce heart rate in patients undergoing cardiac CT. European Radiology, 26, 3635–3642. doi: 10.1007/s00330-016-4215-8
  • Nilsson, U. (2008). The anxiety and pain reducing effects of music interventions: A systematic review. AORN Journal, 87, 780–807. doi: 10.1016/j.aorn.2007.09.013
  • Nilsson, U. (2009). The effect of music intervention in stress response to cardiac surgery in a randomized clinical trial. Heart & Lung, 38, 201–207. doi: 10.1016/j.hrtlng.2008.07.008
  • Nomura, S., Yoshimura, K., & Kurosawa, Y. (2013). A pilot study on the effect of music-heart beat feedback system on human heart activity. Journal of Medical Informatics & Technologies, 22, 251–256. Retrieved from http://jmit.us.edu.pl/cms/jmitjrn/22/14_Nomura_3.pdf
  • Olfson, M., King, M., & Schoenbaum, M. (2015). Benzodiazepine use in the United States. JAMA Psychiatry, 72, 136–142. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1763
  • Pearce, E., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. (2015). The ice-breaker effect: Singing mediates fast social bonding. Royal Society Open Science, 2, 150221. doi: 10.1098/rsos.150221
  • Pelletier, C. (2004). The effect of music on decreasing arousal due to stress: A meta-analysis. Journal of Music Therapy, 41, 192–214. doi: 10.1093/jmt/41.3.192
  • Pfaff, D. W., Martin, E. M., & Ribeiro, A. C. (2007). Relations between mechanisms of CNS arousal and mechanisms of stress. Stress, 10, 316–325. doi: 10.1080/10253890701638030
  • Pittman, S., & Kridli, S. (2011). Music intervention and preoperational anxiety: An integrative review. International Nursing Review, 58, 157–163. doi:10.1111/j.1466–7657.2011.00888.x
  • Pritchard, M. J. (2009). Identifying and assessing anxiety in pre-operative patients. Nursing Standard, 23, 35–40. doi: 10.7748/ns2009.08.23.51.35.c7222
  • Puetz, T. W., Youngstedt, S. D., & Herring, M. P. (2015). Effects of pharmacotherapy on combat-related PTSD, anxiety, and depression: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis. PloS One, 10, 1–18. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126529
  • Riley, K. E., & Park, C. L. (2015). How does yoga reduce stress? A systematic review of mechanisms of change and guide to future inquiry. Health Psychology Review, 9, 379–396. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2014.981778
  • Robb, S. L., Carpenter, J. S., & Burns, D. S. (2011). Reporting guidelines for music-based interventions. Journal of Health Psychology, 16, 342–352. doi: 10.1177/1359105310385750
  • Rohner, S. J., & Miller, R. (1980). Degrees of familiar and affective music and their effects on state anxiety. Journal of Music Therapy, 17, 2–15. doi: 10.1093/jmt/17.1.2
  • Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 183–192. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.2.183
  • Rudin, D., Kiss, A., Wetz, R. V., & Sottile, V. M. (2007). Music in the endoscopy suite: A meta-analysis of randomised controlled studies. Endoscopy, 39, 507–510. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-966362
  • Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., & Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 257–262. doi: 10.1038/nn.2726
  • Salimpoor, V. N., van den Bosch, I., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., Dagher, A., & Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Interactions between the nucleus accumbens and auditory cortices predict music reward value. Science, 340, 216–219. doi: 10.1126/science.1232437
  • Sandstrom, G. M., & Russo, F. A. (2010). Music hath charms: The effects of valence and arousal on recovery following an acute stressor. Music and Medicine, 2, 137–143. doi: 10.1177/1943862110371486
  • Särkämö, T., Tervaniemi, M., Laitinen, S., Forsblom, A., Soinila, S., Mikkonen, M., … Hietanen, M. (2008). Music listening enhances cognitive recovery and mood after middle cerebral artery stroke. Brain, 131, 866–876. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn013
  • Särkämö, T., Tervaniemi, M., Laitinen, S., Numminen, A., Kurki, M., Johnson, J. K., & Rantanen, P. (2014). Cognitive, emotional, and social benefits of regular musical activities in early dementia: Randomized controlled study. The Gerontologist, 54, 634–650. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnt100
  • Selye, H. (1956). The stress of life. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  • Selye, H. (1973). The evolution of the stress concept: The originator of the concept traces its development from the discovery in 1936 of the alarm reaction to modern therapeutic applications of syntoxic and catatoxic hormones. American Scientist, 61, 692–699. Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/27844072
  • Selye, H. (1976). The stress concept. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 115, 718. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1878840/
  • Sendelbach, S. E., Halm, M. A., Doran, K. A., Miller, E. H., & Gaillard, P. (2006). Effects of music therapy on physiological and psychological outcomes for patients undergoing cardiac surgery. The Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing, 21, 194–200. doi: 10.1097/00005082-200605000-00007
  • Sokhadze, E. M. (2007). Effects of music on the recovery of autonomic and electrocortical activity after stress induced by aversive visual stimuli. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 32, 31–50. doi: 10.1007/s10484-007-9033-y
  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
  • Spruit, A., Assink, M., van Vugt, E., van der Put, C., & Stams, G. J. (2016). The effects of physical activity interventions on psychosocial outcomes in adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 45, 56–71. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.03.006
  • Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
  • Tarr, B., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. (2014). Music and social bonding: Self-other merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1096. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01096
  • ter Beek, E., Spruit, A., Kuiper, C. H. Z., van der Rijken, R. E. A., Hendriks, J., & Stams, G. J. J. M. (2018). Treatment effect on recidivism for juveniles who have sexually offended: A multilevel meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46, 543–556. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0308-3
  • Thaut, M. H., & Hoemberg, V. (2014). Handbook of neurologic music therapy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Thaut, M. H., Kenyon, G. P., Schauer, M. L., & McIntosh, G. C. (1999). The connection between rhythmicity and brain function. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Magazine, 18, 101–108. doi: 10.1109/51.752991
  • Thaut, M. H., & Wheeler, B. L. (2010). Music therapy. In P. Juslin & J. Slodoba (Eds.), Handbook on music and emotions: Theory, research, applications (pp. 819–848). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Thomas, B. H., Ciliska, D., Dobbins, M., & Micucci, S. (2004). A process for systematically reviewing the literature: Providing the research evidence for public health nursing interventions. Worldviews on Evidence-Based Nursing, 1, 176–184. doi: 10.1111/j.1524-475X.2004.04006.x
  • Tweed, R. G., White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2004). Culture, stress, and coping: Internally- and externally- targeted control strategies of European Canadians, East Asian Canadians, and Japanese. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 652–668. doi: 10.1177/0022022104270109
  • Uhlig, S., Jaschke, A., & Scherder, E. (2013). Effects of music on emotion regulation: A systematic literature review. In G. Luck & O. Brabant (Eds.), The 3rd international conference on music & emotion. Jyväskylä. Retrieved from https://jyx.jyu.fi/handle/123456789/41628
  • UK Health and Safety Executive. (2016). Work related stress, anxiety and depression in Great Britain 2016. Retrieved from http://www.hse.gov.uk/statistics/causdis/stress.pdf
  • Vaajoki, A., Kankkunen, P., Pietilä, A. M., & Vehviläinen-Julkunen, K. (2011). Music as a nursing intervention: Effects of music listening on blood pressure, heart rate, and respiratory rate in abdominal surgery patients. Nursing & Health Sciences, 13, 412–418. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-2018.2011.00633.x
  • Van den Bussche, E., Van den Noortgate, W., & Reynvoet, B. (2009). Mechanisms of masked priming: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 452–477. doi: 10.1037/a0015329
  • Van Den Noortgate, W., & Onghena, P. (2003). Estimating the mean effect size in meta-analysis: Bias, precision, and mean squared error of different weighting methods. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 504–511. doi: 10.3758/BF03195529
  • Verma, R., Balhara, Y. P. S., & Gupta, C. S. (2011). Gender differences in stress response: Role of developmental and biological determinants. Industrial Psychiatry Journal, 20, 4–10. doi: 10.4103/0972-6748.98407
  • Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03
  • Weinstein, D., Launay, J., Pearce, E., Dunbar, R. I., & Stewart, L. (2016). Group music performance causes elevated pain thresholds and social bonding in small and large groups of singers. Evolution and Human Behaviour: Official Journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society, 37, 152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.10.002
  • Wilson, D. B. (2013). Practical meta-analysis effect size calculator. Retrieved from http://www.campbellcollaboration.org/escalc/html/EffectSizeCalculator-Home.php
  • Witvliet, C., & Vrana, S. (2007). Play it again Sam: Repeated exposure to emotionally evocative music polarises liking and smiling responses, and influences other affective reports, facial EMG, and heart rate. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 3–25. doi: 10.1080/02699930601000672
  • World Health Organization. (2010). Global health diplomacy: Negotiating health in the 21st century. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/dg/speeches/2008/20081021/en/
  • Zatorre, R. J. (2015). Musical pleasure and reward: Mechanisms and dysfunction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1337, 202–211. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12677
  • Zeng, X., Zhang, Y., Kwong, J. S., Zhang, C., Li, S., Sun, F., … Du, L. (2015). The methodological quality assessment tools for preclinical and clinical studies, systematic review and meta-analysis, and clinical practice guideline: A systematic review. Journal of Evidence-Based Medicine, 8, 2–10. doi: 10.1111/jebm.12141
  • Zhang, Z. S., Wang, X. L., Xu, C. L., Zhang, C., Cao, Z., Xu, W. D., … Sun, Y. H. (2014). Music reduces panic: An initial study of listening to preferred music improves male patient discomfort and anxiety during flexible cystoscopy. Journal of Endourology, 28, 739–744. doi: 10.1089/end.2013.0705

Appendices

Appendix 1

Table A1. Characteristics of included studies.

Appendix 2

Figure A1. Trim-and-fill plot of the effects of music interventions on physiological stress-related outcomes.

Figure A1. Trim-and-fill plot of the effects of music interventions on physiological stress-related outcomes.

Figure A2. Trim-and-fill plot for the effects of music interventions on psychological stress-related outcomes.

Figure A2. Trim-and-fill plot for the effects of music interventions on psychological stress-related outcomes.