534
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Editorial

Important Role of Brain Reserve in Lowering the Risk of Alzheimer‘s Disease

Pages 1-4 | Published online: 18 Dec 2008

We have proposed that there are two distinct sets of risk factors for Alzheimer‘s disease (AD), those for the pathology of the disease and those for its clinical expression Citation[1–3]. Although Alzheimer pathology is necessary for the diagnosis, it likely is not sufficient. Autopsy studies have shown that approximately a third of individuals meeting strict criteria for neuropathological AD remain nondemented at the time of their death Citation[2]. Furthermore, people with similar severities of Alzheimer pathology have a very wide distribution of cognitive presentations before death, ranging from intact cognitive function to severe dementia Citation[4].

The ability of individuals to maintain a relatively high level of cognitive function in the presence of substantial Alzheimer pathology provides strong support for the presence of the second set of risk factors for AD, those for its clinical expression. In 1995, I proposed “that clinical expression of AD requires two conditions to be met: first, a propensity, which is largely genetically determined, to accumulate Alzheimer lesions over the life course at a sufficiently rapid rate to meet neuropathological criteria for AD at death; and second, the attainment of a critical threshold of brain reserve beyond which normal cognitive function cannot be maintained” Citation[1]. The existence of a threshold of functioning brain tissue has been appreciated for decades in the case of Parkinson‘s disease, where 80% or more of the dopaminergic cells in the substantia nigra pars compacta must be lost in order for the initial symptoms of this illness to become apparent Citation[5]. As early as 1966, Martin Roth noted that dementia does not occur until a critical number of lesions or volume of brain softening secondary to infarcts is reached Citation[6]. The presence of varying amounts of brain or cognitive reserve buffering the onset of dementia is now widely accepted as a possible explanation for the dissociation between neuropathological severity at autopsy and the cognitive status before death.

Brain and cognitive reserve could reflect several variables, including the number and size of neurons and their density of interconnection at the time of maturity in early adulthood, the collection of cognitive strategies for solving problems or taking neuropsychological tests, the loss of functioning brain tissue over the life course, and the growth and maintenance of brain tissue as a result of mental and/or physical exercise.

Besides genes, three risk factors related to the clinical expression of AD are determined early in life: educational attainment, IQ and brain volume. Numerous studies have shown that low educational attainment is a strong risk factor for dementia of the Alzheimer type Citation[3]. For example, in an epidemiologic study of the East Boston, Massachusetts, USA community, the incidence of AD over 3 years was eight-times higher in participants with an elementary school education or less compared with those with a high school education or more Citation[7]. Although years of education can be reliably measured, access to education may be limited in many populations by socioeconomic status Citation[8]. Furthermore, it is unlikely that merely attending school for a number of years protects one from getting AD. More likely, greater education is a surrogate for other risk factors, including higher IQ and greater cognitive activity during life. Higher IQ has been shown to reduce the risk of AD and dementia in late life and to have a stronger association with dementia than education Citation[9,10]. Since IQ has a heritability of approximately 0.50, some of the association between attained education and dementia attributable to IQ may be genetically mediated Citation[11]. However, the association of lower education with dementia remains strong even when monozygotic co-twins discordant for dementia are compared with eachother Citation[12].

Brain volume at any age reflects genetic, nutritional, vascular, inflammatory, oxidative, hormonal and other processes that have occurred since conception. However, an upper limit is placed on the size of the brain by intracranial volume, which is determined largely prior to age 10 years Citation[13]. In 1988, Katzman reported that among people with numerous Alzheimer plaques at autopsy, those who scored in the upper quintile on cognitive and functional performance had significantly higher brain weights and greater numbers of neurons in the cerebral cortex, suggesting that a larger endowment of neurons in childhood might offer protection from cognitive loss in the presence of Alzheimer neuropathology Citation[14]. This finding was followed by studies showing that larger brain sizes inferred from larger intracranial cross-sectional areas, head circumferences or intracranial volumes were associated with later ages of onset of AD, less severe clinical symptoms and lower incidence of cognitive decline and AD Citation[3,15,16]. In a recent study of over 15,000 elderly residents from seven low and middle income countries, small head circumference was found to be significantly related to prevalence of dementia Citation[17]. When education and head circumference are considered together, small head circumference is only a risk factor among those with low education Citation[16]. Finally, intracranial volume is correlated with both education Citation[18] and IQ Citation[19], and education and IQ are correlated with one another Citation[9]. The consequence of these correlations is that sufficient brain reserve to reduce the risk of dementia may be afforded by either high IQ, higher education or large brain volume, so that in studies in which most participants have one of these characteristics, it may be impossible to see associations with the others.

If there are two distinct sets of risk factors, risk factors for clinical expression should be independent of the severity of pathology. Recent clinicopathologic studies have supported this claim, showing a lack of association of head circumference and educational attainment with the severity of Alzheimer lesions at autopsy Citation[16,20].

Given the protective effect of larger brains and higher education, what are the implications of these findings for prevention? Brain size at maturity, while in part genetically controlled, is also determined by under- or malnutrition during the first few years of life Citation[21]. In the future, improved nutrition in early childhood could lower the incidence of AD by maximizing the size of the mature brain. Improvements in education could also lead to reduction in dementia occurrence by providing individuals with more efficient and flexible cognitive strategies for addressing problems – what has been termed active cognitive reserve Citation[22] – as well as stimulating greater intellectual activity over the adult life course. Although more efficient use of the brain is unlikely to be reflected in anatomically detectable differences, increased mental exercise among those who are better educated could well be reflected in synaptic or neuronal growth. To date, few human studies have assessed the role of education in brain growth or maintenance of functioning tissue. One exception was a study of MR images obtained from London taxi drivers, which showed that the length of time spent as a taxi driver was associated with the degree of redistribution of gray matter from the anterior to the posterior part of the right hippocampus, a region known to be involved in spatial localization Citation[23]. It is likely that higher education, or associated experiences, can affect the growth of brain tissue in a similar manner to how physical exercise of specific muscles affects their growth.

Although it is impossible to increase intracranial volume after childhood, loss of functional brain tissue within the cranium can be ameliorated, particularly through control of risk factors for cerebrovascular disease and stroke, including hypertension, diabetes and hypercholesterolemia. Most studies suggest that infarcts appear to contribute to dementia primarily by modifying the risk caused by Alzheimer lesions. In the Nun Study, for example, the risk of dementia among individuals satisfying neuropathological criteria for AD was increased 20-times by the presence of one to two lacunar infarcts Citation[24]. Therefore, prevention and control of vascular risk factors and cerebrovascular disease has the potential to greatly reduce the incidence of clinical AD.

Another intervention with potential to increase brain reserve is aerobic physical exercise. Most longitudinal epidemiologic studies have shown a decreased risk of incident dementia or AD in individuals who exercise regularly Citation[25–27]. Other studies have examined the effects of physical exercise on cognitive decline, again with most showing positive effects Citation[28]. Colcombe and collaborators performed a randomized 6-month clinical trial comparing aerobic exercise with stretching and toning in 50 healthy but sedentary volunteers, aged 60–79 years, who received two MRIs Citation[29]. Significant increases in brain volume of both gray and white matter regions occurred in the aerobic exercise group but not in the group who did stretching exercises. Increases in brain volume may reflect increases in the number or size of neurons, increases in the number of dendritic connections, increased capillary bed volume and/or increases in glial cell size and number.

In conclusion, although most of the risk of Alzheimer dementia is attributable to genetic inheritance Citation[30], a variety of factors attributable in part to the environment, including attained education, intracranial volume, intelligence, absence of vascular risk factors and cerebrovascular disease, and mental and physical exercise, significantly modify the risk of this condition in late life by increasing brain reserve. Since these factors are potentially modifiable, they offer a way to decrease the incidence of this condition in the presence of significant Alzheimer brain pathology. Randomized clinical trials, utilizing mental and physical exercise interventions, are needed to demonstrate the effectiveness of these interventions in preventing AD and related dementias in the general population.

Financial & competing interests disclosure

The author has no relevant affiliations or financial involvement with any organization or entity with a financial interest in or financial conflict with the subject matter or materials discussed in the manuscript. This includes employment, consultancies, honoraria, stock ownership or options, expert testimony, grants or patents received or pending, or royalties.

No writing assistance was utilized in the production of this manuscript.

Bibliography

  • Mortimer JA : The continuum hypothesis of Alzheimer‘s disease and normal aging: the role of brain reserve.Alzheimers Res.1, 67–70 (1995).
  • Mortimer JA , BorensteinAR, GoscheKM, SnowdonDA: Very early detection of Alzheimer neuropathology and the role of brain reserve in modifying its clinical expression.J. Geriat. Psychiat. Neurol.18, 218–223 (2005).
  • Borenstein AR , CopenhaverCI, MortimerJA: Early-life risk factors for Alzheimer disease.Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord.20, 63–72 (2006).
  • Riley KP , SnowdonDA, MarkesberyWR: Alzheimer‘s neurofibrillary pathology and the spectrum of cognitive function: findings from the Nun Study.Ann. Neurol.51, 567–577 (2002).
  • McGeer PL , McGeerEG: Aging and neurotransmitter systems. In: Parkinson‘s Disease-II. Aging and Neuroendocrine Relationships. FinchCE, PotterDE, KennyAD (Eds). Plenum Press, NY, USA41–57 (1978).
  • Roth MR , TomlinsonBE, BlessedG: Correlation between scores for dementia and counts of ‘senile plaques‘ in cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects.Nature209, 109–110 (1966).
  • Evans DA , HebertLE, BeckettLAet al.: Education and other measures of socioeconomic status and risk of incident Alzheimer‘s disease in a defined population of older persons.Arch. Neurol.54, 1399–1405 (1997).
  • Kaplan GA , TurrellG, LynchJW, EversonSA, HelkalaEL, SalonenJT: Childhood socioeconomic position and cognitive function in adulthood.Int. J. Epidemiol.30, 256–263 (2001).
  • Schmand B , SmitJH, GeerlingsMIet al.: The effects of intelligence and education on the development of dementia: a test of the brain reserve hypothesis.Psychol. Med.27, 1337–1344 (1997).
  • Fritsch T , SmythKA, McClendonMJet al.: Associations between dementia/mild cognitive impairment and cognitive performance and activity levels in youth.J. Am Geriatr. Soc.53, 1191–1196 (2005)
  • Plassman BL , WelshKA, HelmsMet al.: Intelligence and education as predictors of cognitive state in late life: a 50-year follow-up.Neurology45, 1446–1450 (1995).
  • Gatz M , MortimerJA, FratiglioniLet al.: Potentially modifiable risk factors for dementia in identical twins.Alzheimers Dement.2, 110–117 (2006).
  • Gale CR , WaltonS, MartynCN: Foetal and postnatal head growth and risk of cognitive decline in old age.Brain126, 2273–2278 (2003).
  • Katzman R , TerryR, DeTeresaRet al.: Clinical, pathological and neurochemical changes in dementia: a subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques.Ann. Neurol.23, 138–144 (1988).
  • Graves AB , MortimerJA, BowenJDet al.: Head circumference and incident Alzheimer‘s disease: modification by apolipoprotein E.Neurology57, 1453–1460 (2001).
  • Mortimer JA , SnowdonDA, MarkesberyWR: Head circumference, education and risk of dementia: findings from the Nun Study.J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol.25, 671–679 (2003).
  • Acosta D , FerriCP, GuerraMet al.: Skull circumference, leg length and handedness as developmental indicators, and their association with dementia in low and middle income countries: evidence from the 10.66 population-based studies.Alzheimers Dement.4(Suppl. 2), T666 (2008).
  • Coffey CE , SaxtonJA, RatcliffGet al.: Relation of education to brain size in normal aging: implications for the reserve hypothesis.Neurology53, 189–196 (1999).
  • Wolf H , JulinP, GertzHJet al.: Intracranial volume in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer‘s disease and vascular dementia: evidence for brain reserve?Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiat.19, 995–1007 (2004).
  • Bennett DA , WilsonRS, SchneiderJAet al.: Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons.Neurology60, 1909–1915 (2003).
  • Dobbing J : Infant nutrition and later achievement.Nutr. Rev.42, 1–7 (1984).
  • Stern Y : What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept.J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc.8, 448–460 (2002).
  • Maguire EA , GadianDG, JohnsrudeISet al.: Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers.Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA97, 4398–4403 (2000).
  • Snowdon DA , GreinerLH, MortimerJA, RileyKP, GreinerPA, MarkesberyWR: Brain infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer disease. The Nun Study.JAMA277, 813–817 (1997).
  • Rovio S , KareholtI, HeikalaELet al.: Leisure-time physical activity at midlife and risk of dementia and Alzheimer‘s disease.Lancet Neurol.4, 705–711 (2005).
  • Podewils LJ , GuallarE, KullerLHet al.: Physical activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study.Am. J. Epidemiol.161, 639–651 (2005).
  • Larson ED , WangL, BowenJDet al.: Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older.Ann. Intern. Med.144, 73–81 (2006).
  • Weuve J , KangJH, MansonJE, BretelerMM, WareJH, GrodsteinF: Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women.JAMA292, 1454–1461 (2004).
  • Colcumbe SJ , WricksonKI, ScalfPEet al.: Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans.J. Gerontol. Med. Sci.61A, 1166–1170 (2006).
  • Gatz M , ReynoldsCA, FratiglioniLet al.: Role of genes and environment for explaining Alzheimer disease.Arch. Gen. Psychiat.63, 168–174 (2006).

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.