858
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Contextual cues prompt greater improvements in alcohol consumption recall for people with higher working memory capacities

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Article: 2089052 | Received 26 Aug 2021, Accepted 04 Jun 2022, Published online: 29 Jun 2022

References

  • Australian Institute of Health and Welfare 2018. Impact of alcohol and illicit drug use on the burden of disease and injury in Australia: Australian Burden of Disease Study 2011. Australian Burden of Disease Study series no. (17). Cat. no. Australian Burden of Disease Study series Vol. 19. AIHW.
  • Baddeley, A. D. (1983). Working memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: B, 302(1110), 311–10. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1983.0057
  • Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1736359
  • Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2
  • Best, J. B. (2010). The contribution of working memory components to reading comprehension in children [ Doctoral dissertation]. https://dspace.library.uvic.ca/bitstream/handle/1828/3189/EDITS%20TO%20GINA%20DEC%2017%20Reformatted%202.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y
  • Borella, E., Carretti, B., & De Beni, R. (2008). Working memory and inhibition across the adult life-span. Acta Psychologica, 128(1), 33–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.09.008
  • Brener, N. D., Billy, J. O., & Grady, W. R. (2003). Assessment of factors affecting the validity of self-reported health-risk behavior among adolescents: Evidence from the scientific literature. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33(6), 436–457. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(03)00052-1
  • Buchanan, T. (2016). Self-report measures of executive function problems correlate with personality, not performance-based executive function measures, in nonclinical samples. Psychological Assessment, 28(4), 372. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000192
  • Callinan, S., Livingston, M., Room, R., & Dietze, P. (2016). Drinking contexts and alcohol consumption: How much alcohol is consumed in different Australian locations? Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 77(4), 612–619. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2016.77.612
  • Callinan, S. (2019). Setting a cap on the maximum average number of drinks per day in australian survey research. The International Journal of Alcohol and Drug Research, 8(1), 7–11. https://doi.org/10.7895/ijadr.257
  • Casler, K., Bickel, L., & Hackett, E. (2013). Separate but equal? a comparison of participants and data gathered via Amazon’s MTurk, social media, and face-to-face behavioral testing. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(6), 2156–2160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.05.009
  • Casswell, S., Huckle, T., & Pledger, M. (2002). Survey data need not underestimate alcohol consumption. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 26(10), 1561–1567. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02456.x
  • Dawson, D. A. (2003). Methodological issues in measuring alcohol use. Alcohol Research and Health, 27 (1), 18–30. https://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/arh27-1/18-29.pdf
  • Dulin, P. L., Alvarado, C. E., Fitterling, J. M., & Gonzalez, V. M. (2017). Comparisons of Alcohol Consumption by Time-Line Follow Back vs. Smartphone-Based Daily Interviews. Addiction Research & Theory, 25(3), 195–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2016.1239081
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
  • Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2000). Assessment of working memory in six- and seven-year-old children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 377–390. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.2.377
  • Geiser, C., Lehmann, W., & Eid, M. (2006). Separating“rotators” from“nonrotators” in the mental rotations test: A multigroup latent class analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41(3), 261–293. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr4103_2
  • Gmel, G., Studer, J., Deline, S., Baggio, S., N’Goran, A., Mohler-Kuo, M., & Daeppen, J. B. (2014). More is not always better—comparison of three instruments measuring volume of drinking in a sample of young men and their association with consequences. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 75(5), 880–888. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2014.75.880
  • Greenfield, T. K. (2000). Ways of measuring drinking patterns and the difference they make: Experience with graduated frequencies. Journal of Substance Abuse, 12(1–2), 33–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0899-3289(00)00039-0
  • Greenfield, T. K., & Kerr, W. C. (2008). Alcohol measurement methodology in Epidemiology: Recent advances and opportunities. Addiction, 103(7), 1082–1099. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02197.x
  • Greenfield, T. K., Kerr, W. C., Bond, J., Ye, Y., & Stockwell, T. (2009). Improving graduated frequencies alcohol measures for monitoring consumption patterns: Results from an Australian national survey and a US diary validity study. Contemporary Drug Problems, 36(3–4), 705–733. https://doi.org/10.1177/009145090903600320
  • Hilton, M. E. (1989). A comparison of a prospective diary and two summary recall techniques for recording alcohol consumption. British Journal of Addiction, 84(9), 1085–1092. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.1989.tb00792.x
  • Jacobs, J. (1887). Experiments on “prehension”. Mind, 12(45), 75–79. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/os-12.45.75
  • Kerr, W. C., & Stockwell, T. (2012). Understanding standard drinks and drinking guidelines. Drug and Alcohol Review, 31(2), 200–205. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-3362.2011.00374.x
  • Lau-Barraco, C., Braitman, A. L., Linden Carmichael, A. N., & Stamates, A. L. (2016). Differences in weekday versus weekend drinking among nonstudent emerging adults. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 24(2), 100. https://doi.org/10.1037/pha0000068
  • Livingston, M., & Callinan, S. (2015). Underreporting in alcohol surveys: Whose drinking is underestimated? Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 76(1), 158–164. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2015.76.158
  • Ma, L., Chang, L., Chen, X., Zhou, R., & Laks, J. (2017). Working memory test battery for young adults: Computerized working memory assessment. PLoS ONE, 12(3), e0175047. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175047
  • Midanik, L. T., & Hines, A. M. (1991). ‘Unstandard’ ways of answering standard questions: Protocol analysis in alcohol survey research. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 27(3), 245–252. https://doi.org/10.1016/0376-8716(91)90007-L
  • Monk, R. L., & Heim, D. (2014). A systematic review of the alcohol norms literature: A focus on context. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 21(4), 263–282. https://doi.org/10.3109/09687637.2014.899990
  • Monk, R. L., & Heim, D. (2014). A systematic review of the Alcohol Norms literature: A focus on context. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 21(4), 263–282. https://doi.org/10.3109/09687637.2014.899990
  • Monk, R. L., Heim, D., Qureshi, A., Price, A., & Ryabinin, A. E. (2015). “I have no clue what I drunk last night” Using Smartphone technology to compare in-vivo and retrospective self-reports of alcohol consumption. PLoS ONE, 10(5), e0126209. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126209
  • Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.17.2.299
  • Redick, T. S., Broadway, J. M., Meier, M. E., Kuriakose, P. S., Unsworth, N., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Measuring working memory capacity with automated complex span tasks. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 28(3), 164–171. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000123
  • Rehm, J. (1998). Measuring quantity, frequency, and volume of drinking. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 22, 4s–14s. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb04368.x
  • Sensis. (2018). The must-know stats from the 2018 yellow social media report. https://www.sensis.com.au/about/our-reports/sensis-social-media-report
  • Stockwell, T., Donath, S., Cooper-Stanbury, M., Chikritzhs, T., Catalano, P., & Mateo, C. (2004). Under-Reporting of alcohol consumption in household surveys: A comparison of quantity–frequency, graduated–frequency and recent recall. Addiction, 99(8), 1024–1033. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00815.x
  • Stone, A. A., Turkkan, J. S., Bachrach, C. A., Jobe, J. B., Kurtzman, H. S., & Cain, V. S. (1999). The science of self-report: Implications for research and practice. Psychology Press.
  • Thoresen, J. C., Francelet, R., Coltekin, A., Richter, K. F., Fabrikant, S. I., & Sandi, C. (2016). Not all anxious individuals get lost: Trait anxiety and mental rotation ability interact to explain performance in map-based route learning in men. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 132, 1–8. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1074742716300314?via%3Dihub
  • Tucker, J. S., Orlando, M., & Ellickson, P. L. (2003). Patterns and correlates of binge drinking trajectories from early adolescence to young adulthood. Health Psychology, 22(1), 79–87. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.1.79
  • Turgut, M. (2015). Development of the spatial ability self-report scale (SASRS): Reliability and validity studies. Quality & Quantity, 49(5), 1997–2014. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-014-0086-8
  • Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114(1), 104–132. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.104
  • Unsworth, N., & Spillers, G. J. (2010). Variation in working memory capacity and episodic recall: The contributions of strategic encoding and contextual retrieval. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(2), 200–205. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.2.200
  • Unsworth, N., Spillers, G. J., & Brewer, G. A. (2012). The role of working memory capacity in autobiographical retrieval: Individual differences in strategic search. Memory, 20(2), 167–176. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.651087
  • Unsworth, N., Brewer, G. A., & Spillers, G. J. (2013). Working memory capacity and retrieval from long-term memory: The role of controlled search. Memory & Cognition, 41(2), 242‐254. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0261-x
  • Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47(2), 599–604. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599
  • Woodyard, C. D., & Hallam, J. S. (2010). Differences in college student typical drinking and celebration drinking. Journal of American College Health, 58(6), 533–538. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481003621734
  • World Health Organization. (2000). International guide for monitoring alcohol consumption and related harm. https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/66529/WHO_MSD_MSB_00.4.pdf?sequence=1
  • World Health Organization. (2009). Global health risks: Mortality and burden of disease attributable to selected major risks. https://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/GlobalHealthRisks_report_full.pdf