722
Views
27
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
SURVEY PAPER

Cartographic Design and Usability of Visual Variables for Linear Features

, , , , &

References

  • Allen, G. L., Cowan, C. R. M. and Power, H. (2006). ‘Acquiring information from simple weather maps: influences of domain-specific knowledge and general visual–spatial abilities’, Learning and Individual Differences, 16(4), pp. 337–349. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.01.003
  • Andrienko, G., Andrienko, N., Jankowski, P., Keim, D., Kraak, M. J., MacEachren, A., Wrobel, S. (2007). ‘Geovisual analytics for spatial decision support: setting the research agenda’, International Journal of Geographical Information Science, 21(8), pp. 839–857. doi: 10.1080/13658810701349011
  • Bertin, J. (1983). Semiology of Graphics: Diagrams, Networks, Maps, University of Wisconsin Press, Madison.
  • Blajenkova, O., Kozhevnikov, M. and Motes, M. A. (2006). ‘Object-spatial imagery: a new selfreport imagery questionnaire’, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), pp. 239–263. doi: 10.1002/acp.1182
  • Blazhenkova, O. and Kozhevnikov, M. (2009). ‘The new object-spatial-verbal cognitive style model: theory and measurement’, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(5), pp. 638–663. doi: 10.1002/acp.1473
  • Brand, J. and Johnson, A. P. (2014). ‘Attention to local and global levels of hierarchical Navon figures affects rapid scene categorization’, Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1274. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4251296/.
  • Brunswik, E. (1955). ‘Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology’, Psychological Review, 62, pp. 193–217. doi: 10.1037/h0047470
  • Ciołkosz-Styk, A. (2012). ‘The visual search method in map perception research’, Geoinformation Issues, 4(1/4), pp. 33–42.
  • Daneman, M. and Carpenter, P. A. (1980). ‘Individual differences in working memory and reading’, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), pp. 450–466. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6
  • Dong, W., Ran, J. and Wang, J. (2012). ‘Effectiveness and efficiency of map symbols for dynamic geographic information visualization’, Cartography and Geographic Information Science, 39(2), pp. 98–106. doi: 10.1559/1523040639298
  • Duchaine, B., Yovel, G. and Nakayama, K. (2007). ‘No global processing deficit in the Navon task in 14 developmental prosopagnosics’, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(2), pp. 104–113. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm003
  • Edler, D., Bestgen, A. K., Kuchinke, L. and Dickmann, F. (2014). ‘Grids in topographic maps reduce distortions in the recall of learned object locations’, PLoS ONE, 9(5), 1–10: e98148. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098148
  • Fabrikant, S. I. and Goldsberry, K. (2005). ‘Thematic Relevance and Perceptual Salience of Dynamic Geovisualization Display’ in ICA International Cartographic Conference, A Coruna, Spain, July 9–16. http://www.geo.uzh.ch/~sara/pubs/fabrikant_goldsberry_ica05.pdf.
  • Gardner, R. W., Holzman, P. S., Klein, G. S., Linton, H. B. and Spence, D. P. (1959) Cognitive Control. A Study of Individual Consistencies in Cognitive Behavior: Part 4. Psychological Issues, International Universities Press, New York.
  • Garlandini, S. and Fabrikant, S. I. (2009). ‘Evaluating the effectiveness and efficiency of visual variables for geographic information visualization’, in Spatial Information Theory: 9th International Conference, COSIT 2009 Aber Wrac’h, France, September 21–25, 2009 Proceedings, eds. by Hornsby, K. S., Claramunt, Ch., Denis, M. and Ligozat, G., pp. 195–211, Springer-Verlag, Berlin/Heidelberg.
  • Goldsberry, K. (2008). ‘GeoVisualization of Automobile Congestion’, in GeoVisualization of Dynamics, Movement and Change, Proceedings of the AGILE 2008 Conference. Girona, Spain, May 5, 2008.
  • Graff, M. (2003). ‘Learning from web-based instructional systems and cognitive style’, British Journal of Educational Technology, 34(4), pp. 407–418. doi: 10.1111/1467-8535.00338
  • Griffin, A. L. (2008). ‘Visual Variables’, in Encyclopedia of Geographic Information Science, ed. by Kemp, K., pp. 506–509, SAGE Publication, Thousand Oaks.
  • Hahn, U., Chater, N. and Richardson, L. B. (2003). ‘Similarity as transformation’, Cognition, 87, pp. 1–32. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00184-1
  • Hegarty, M., Canham, M. S. and Fabrikant, S. I. (2010). ‘Thinking about the weather: how display salience and knowledge affect performance in a graphic inference task’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(1), pp. 37–53.
  • Henderson, J. M. and Hollingworth, A. (1999). ‘High-level scene perception’, Annual Review of Psychology, 50, pp. 243–271. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.243
  • Horz, H. and Schnotz, W. (2010). ‘Cognitive load in learning with multiple representations’, in Cognitive Load Theory, ed. by Plass, J. L., Moreno, R. and Brünken, R., pp. 229–252, Cambridge University Press, New York.
  • Isaksen, S. G. and Puccio, G. J. (2008). ‘Adaption-innovation and the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: The level-style issue revisited’, in The Creative Problem Solving Group, Inc., http://cpsb.com/research/articles/problem-solving-style/KAI-TTCT.html
  • Koláčný, A. (1969). ‘Utilitární kartografie cesta k optimální účinnosti kartografické informace’, Geodetický a kartografický obzor, 15(10), pp. 239–244.
  • Kozhevnikov, M. (2007). ‘Cognitive styles in the context of modern psychology: toward an integrated framework of cognitive style’, Psychological Bulletin, 133, pp. 464–481. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.3.464
  • Lautenschütz, A. K. (2012). ‘Map readers’ assessment of path elements and context to identify movement behaviour in visualisations’, The Cartographic Journal, 49(4), pp. 337–349. doi: 10.1179/1743277412Y.0000000029
  • MacEachren, A. M. (1995). How Maps Work: Representation, Visualization, and Design, the Guilford Press, New York.
  • MacEachren, A. M. and Kraak, M. J. (2001). ‘Research challenges in geovisualization’, Cartography and Geographic Information Science, 28(1), pp. 3–12. doi: 10.1559/152304001782173970
  • MacEachren, A. M., Roth, R. E., O’Brien, J., Li, B., Swingley, D. and Gahegan, M. (2012). ‘Visual semiotics and uncertainty visualization: an empirical study’, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 18, pp. 2496–2505. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2012.279
  • Medyckyj-Scott, D. and Board, C. (1991). ‘Cognitive cartography: a new heart for a lost soul’, in Advances in cartography, ed. by Muller, J. C., pp. 201–230, Elsevier, London.
  • Montello, D. (2009). ‘Cognitive research in GIScience: recent achievements and future prospects’, Geography Compass, 3(5), pp. 1824–1840. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-8198.2009.00273.x
  • Navon, D. (1977). ‘Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception’, Cognitive Psychology, 9, pp. 353–383. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
  • Neisser, U. (1967) Cognitive psychology, Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.
  • Nelson, E. S. (1999). ‘Using selective attention theory to design bivariate point symbols’, Cartographic Perspectives, 32, pp. 6–28. doi: 10.14714/CP32.625
  • Nelson, E. S. (2000). ‘Designing effective bivariate symbols: the influence of perceptual grouping processes’, Cartography and Geographic Information Science, 27(4), pp. 261–278. doi: 10.1559/152304000783547786
  • Ogao, P. J. (2002) Exploratory Visualization of Temporal Geospatial Data Using Animation, Ph.D. Thesis, Faculteit Ruimtelijke Wetenschappen, Universiteit Utrecht.
  • Ooms, K., De Maeyer, P., Fack, V., Van Assche, E. and Witlox, F. (2012). ‘Investigating the effectiveness of an efficient label placement method using eye movement data’, The Cartographic Journal, 49(3), pp. 234–246. doi: 10.1179/1743277412Y.0000000010
  • Pinker, S. (1984). ‘Visual cognition: an introduction’, Cognition, 18(13), pp. 1–63. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(84)90021-0
  • Popelka, S., Dvorský, J., Brychtová, A. and Hanzelka, J. (2013). ‘User typology based on eye-movement paths’, in International Multidisciplinary Scientific GeoConference: SGEM: Surveying Geology & mining Ecology Management, Vol I., pp. 1041–1048, Sofia.
  • Rezaei, A. R. and Katz, L. (2004). ‘Evaluation of the reliability and validity of the cognitive styles analysis inventory and recommendations for improvement’, Personality and Individual Differences, 36(6), pp. 1317–1327. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00219-8
  • Riding, R. and Cheema, I. (1991). ‘Cognitive Styles—an overview and integration’, Educational Psychology, 11(3/4), pp. 193–215. doi: 10.1080/0144341910110301
  • Sadler-Smith, E. (2001). ‘The relationship between learning style and cognitive style’, Personality and Individual Differences, 30(4), pp. 609–616. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00059-3
  • Šašinka, Č. (2013) Interindividuální rozdíly v percepci prostoru a map (Interindividual differences in perception of space and maps), Ph.D. thesis, Masaryk University, Brno.
  • Slocum, T., McMaster, R. B., Kesseler, F., and Howard, H., (2009). Thematic Cartography and Geographic Visualization – 3rd edition, Prentice Hall, New Jersey.
  • Stachoň, Z. Šašinka, Č., Štěrba, Z., Zbořil, J., Březinová, Š. and Švancara, J. (2013). ‘Influence of graphic design of cartographic symbols on perception structure’, Kartographische Nachtrichten, 4, pp. 216–220.
  • Štěrba, Z., Šašinka, Č., Stachoň, Z., Kubíček, P., and Tamm, S. (2014). Mixed Research Design in Cartography: A Combination of Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches. Kartographische Nachrichten, 64(5), 262–269.
  • Thorndyke, P. W. and Stasz, C. (1979). ‘Individual differences in procedures for knowledge acquisition from maps’, Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), pp. 137–75. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90006-7
  • Vidláková, I. (2010). ‘Object – Spatial Imagery Questionnaire (OSIQ)’, Annales Psychologici, 14, pp. 67–76.
  • Voženílek, V., Kaňok, J. et al. (2011). Metody tematické kartografie – Vizualizace prostorových jevů, Univerzita Palackého v Olomouci.
  • White, D. A., Myerson, J. and Hale, S. (1997). ‘How cognitive is psychomotor slowing in depression? Evidence from a meta-analysis’, Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 4(3), pp. 166–174. doi: 10.1080/13825589708256645
  • Wolfe, J. M. (1994). ‘Guided search 2.0 A revised model of visual search’, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(2), pp. 202–238. doi: 10.3758/BF03200774
  • Wolfe, J. M. (1998). ‘What can 1 million trials tell us about visual search?’, Psychological Science, 9(1), pp. 33–39. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00006
  • Wolfe, J. M. and Horowitz, T. S. (2004). ‘Opinion: what attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it?’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(6), pp. 495–501. doi: 10.1038/nrn1411
  • Yovel, I., Revelle, W. and Mineka, S. (2005). ‘Who sees trees before forest? The obsessive-compulsive style of visual attention’, Psychological Science, 16(2), pp. 123–129. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00792.x

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.