Publication Cover
Inquiry
An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy
Latest Articles
753
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

The representational structure of linguistic understanding

Received 03 May 2022, Accepted 19 Jun 2023, Published online: 24 Jun 2023

References

  • Balcerak Jackson, B. 2019. “Against the Perceptual Model of Utterance Comprehension.” Philosophical Studies 176 (2): 387–405. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-017-1021-3.
  • Begg, I. M., A. Anas, and S. Farinacci. 1992. “Dissociation of Processes in Belief: Source Recollection, Statement Familiarity, and the Illusion of Truth.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 121 (4): 446–458. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.446.
  • Bond, C. F., and B. M. DePaulo. 2006. “Accuracy of Deception Judgments.” Personality and Social Psychology Review 10 (3): 214–234. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2.
  • Borsky, S., B. Tuller, and L. P. Shapiro. 1998. ““How to Milk a Coat:” The Effects of Semantic and Acoustic Information on Phoneme Categorization.” The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 103 (5): 2670–2676. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.422787.
  • Connine, C. M., and C. Clifton. 1987. “Interactive use of Lexical Information in Speech Perception.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 13 (2): 291–299. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.13.2.291.
  • Drożdżowicz, A. 2023. “Do we Hear Meanings? – Between Perception and Cognition.” Inquiry, 1–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/0020174X.2019.1612774.
  • Drożdżowicz, A. ms. Varieties of Linguistic Understanding - On How We Engage with Linguistic Information.
  • Evans, G. 1982. The Varieties of Reference. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Falandays, J. B., B. J. Batzloff, S. C. Spevack, and M. J. Spivey. 2020. “Interactionism in Language: From Neural Networks to Bodies to Dyads.” Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2018.1501501.
  • Fricker, E. 1994. “Against Gullibility.” In Knowing from Words. Synthese Library, Vol. 230, edited by A. Chakrabarti, and B. K. Matilal. Dordrecht: Springer.
  • Fricker, E. 1995. “Critical Notice.” Mind 104 (414): 393–411. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/104.414.393.
  • Fricker, E. 2003. “Understanding and Knowledge of What is Said.” In Epistemology of Language, edited by A. Barber, 325–366. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Gawronski, B., J. Sherman, and Y. Trope. 2014. “Two of What? A Conceptual Analysis of Dual-Process Theories.” In Dual-process Theories of the Social Mind, edited by B. Gawronski, J. Sherman, and Y. Trope, 3–19. New York: Guilford Press.
  • Gilbert, Daniel T. 1991. “How Mental Systems Believe.” American Psychologist 46 (2): 107–119. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.46.2.107.
  • Gilbert, Daniel T., D. S. Krull, and P. S. Malone. 1990. “Unbelieving the Unbelievable: Some Problems in the Rejection of False Information.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59 (4): 601–613. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.4.601.
  • Gilbert, D. T., R. W. Tafarodi, and P. S. Malone. 1993. “You Can’t Not Believe Everything You Read.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65 (2): 221–233. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.221.
  • Grodniewicz, J. P. 2021. “The Process of Linguistic Understanding.” Synthese 198 (12): 11463–11481. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-020-02807-9.
  • Grodniewicz, J. P. 2022a. “Effective Filtering: Language Comprehension and Testimonial Entitlement.” The Philosophical Quarterly, https://doi.org/10.1093/pq/pqac064.
  • Grodniewicz, J. P. 2022b. “The Justification of Comprehension-Based Beliefs.” Review of Philosophy and Psychology 13 (1): 109–126. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00575-0.
  • Hasson, U., J. P. Simmons, and A. Todorov. 2005. “Believe It or Not.” Psychological Science 16 (7): 566–571. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01576.x.
  • Heck, R. 1995. “The Sense of Communication.” Mind 104 (413): 79–106. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/104.413.79.
  • Hermes, J., T. Behne, and H. Rakoczy. 2018. “The Development of Selective Trust: Prospects for a Dual-Process Account.” Child Development Perspectives 12 (2): 134–138. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12274.
  • Hunter, D. 1998. “Understanding and Belief.” Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 58 (3): 559. https://doi.org/10.2307/2653756.
  • Isberner, M.-B., and T. Richter. 2013. “Can Readers Ignore Implausibility? Evidence for Nonstrategic Monitoring of Event-Based Plausibility in Language Comprehension.” Acta Psychologica 142 (1): 15–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.10.003.
  • Isberner, M.-B., and T. Richter. 2014. “Comprehension and Validation: Separable Stages of Information Processing? A Case for Epistemic Monitoring in Language Comprehension.” In Processing Inaccurate Information: Theoretical and Applied Perspectives from Cognitive Science and the Educational Sciences, edited by David N. Rapp and Jason L.G. Braasch, 245–276. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Jary, M. 2010. Assertion. Palgrave Macmillan UK. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230274617.
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1983. Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference, and Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Johnson, M. K., J. D. Bransford, and S. K. Solomon. 1973. “Memory for Tacit Implications of Sentences.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 98 (1): 203–205. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034290.
  • Johnson, M. K., S. Hashtroudi, and D. S. Lindsay. 1993. “Source Monitoring.” Psychological Bulletin 114 (1): 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3.
  • Kendeou, P. 2014. “Validation and Comprehension: An Integrated Overview.” Discourse Processes 51 (1-2): 189–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2013.855874.
  • Kintsch, W. 1988. “The Role of Knowledge in Discourse Comprehension: A Construction-Integration Model.” Psychological Review 95 (2): 163–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.163.
  • Kintsch, W., and T. A. van Dijk. 1978. “Toward a Model of Text Comprehension and Production.” Psychological Review 85 (5): 363–394. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.85.5.363.
  • Kissine, M., and O. Klein. 2013. “Models of Communication, Epistemic Trust and Espistemic Vigilance.” In Social Cognition and Communication, edited by J. P. Forgas, O. Vincze, and J. László, 139–154. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203744628.
  • Long, D. L., and E. M. Freed. 2018. “A Memory Retrieval View of Text Comprehension.” In Deep Comprehension. 1st ed., edited by K. Millis, D. L. Long, J. P. Magliano, and K. Wiemer, 27–40. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315109503-4
  • Longworth, G. 2008. “Comprehending Speech.” Philosophical Perspectives 22 (1): 339–373. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1520-8583.2008.00151.x.
  • Longworth, G. 2018. “Understanding What was Said.” Synthese 195 (2): 815–834. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-016-1243-5.
  • Mandelbaum, E. 2014. “Thinking is Believing.” Inquiry 57 (1): 55–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/0020174X.2014.858417.
  • Mandelbaum, E., J. Quilty-Dunn, and The Harvard Review of Philosophy. 2015. “Believing without Reason: Or: Why Liberals Shouldn’t Watch Fox News.” The Harvard Review of Philosophy 22: 42–52. https://doi.org/10.5840/harvardreview2015226.
  • Marsh, E. J., A. D. Cantor, and N. M. Brashier. 2016. “Believing That Humans Swallow Spiders in Their Sleep: False Beliefs as Side Effects of the Processes that Support Accurate Knowledge.” In Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 64, edited by B. H. Ross, 93–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2015.09.003
  • McNamara, D. S., and J. Magliano. 2009. “Psychology of Learning and Motivation.” In Psychology of Learning and Motivation 51: 297–384. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(09)51009-2.
  • Mercier, H. 2017. “How Gullible are We? A Review of the Evidence from Psychology and Social Science.” Review of General Psychology 21 (2): 103–122. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000111.
  • Michaelian, K. 2010. “In Defence of Gullibility: The Epistemology of Testimony and the Psychology of Deception Detection.” Synthese 176 (3): 399–427. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-009-9573-1.
  • Michaelian, K. 2013. “The Evolution of Testimony: Receiver Vigilance, Speaker Honesty and the Reliability of Communication.” Episteme 10 (1): 37–59. https://doi.org/10.1017/epi.2013.2.
  • Millikan, Ruth G. 2004. Varieties of Meaning: The 2002 Jean Nicod Lectures. Cambridge, Massachusetts : MIT Press.
  • Millikan, Ruth G. 2005. Language: A Biological Model. Oxford: Oxford; New York: Clarendon Press; Oxford University Press.
  • Nadarevic, L., and E. Erdfelder. 2013. “Spinoza’s Error: Memory for Truth and Falsity.” Memory & Cognition 41 (2): 176–186. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0251-z.
  • O’Brien, E. J., and A. E. Cook. 2016. “Separating the Activation, Integration, and Validation Components of Reading.” In Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 65, edited by B. Ross, 249–276. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Elsevier.
  • Peet, A. 2018. “Testimonial Knowledge without Knowledge of What is Said.” Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 99 (1): 65–81. https://doi.org/10.1111/papq.12137.
  • Piest, B. A., M.-B. Isberner, and T. Richter. 2018. “Don’t Believe Everything you Hear: Routine Validation of Audiovisual Information in Children and Adults.” Memory & Cognition 46 (6): 849–863. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0807-7.
  • Polka, L., and T. Nazzi. 2018. “Interacting Processes and Developmental Biases Allow Learners to Crack the “What” Code and the “who” Code in Spoken Language.” Applied Psycholinguistics 39 (4): 757–761. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716418000218.
  • Potts, G. R. 1972. “Information Processing Strategies Used in the Encoding of Linear Orderings.” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 11 (6): 727–740. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80007-0.
  • Recanati, F. 2002. “Does Linguistic Communication Rest on Inference?” Mind & Language 17 (1 & 2): 105–126. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00191.
  • Richter, T. 2015. “Validation and Comprehension of Text Information: Two Sides of the Same Coin.” Discourse Processes 52 (5-6): 337–355. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2015.1025665.
  • Richter, T., S. Schroeder, and B. Wöhrmann. 2009. “You Don’t Have to Believe Everything you Read: Background Knowledge Permits Fast and Efficient Validation of Information.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96 (3): 538–558. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014038.
  • Richter, T., and M. Singer. 2018. “Discourse Updating: Acquiring and Revising Knowledge through Discourse.” In The Routledge Handbook of Discourse Processes, edited by M. F. Schober, D. N. Rapp, and M. A. Britt, 167–190. New York: Routledge.
  • Schroeder, S., T. Richter, and I. Hoever. 2008. “Getting a Picture that is Both Accurate and Stable: Situation Models and Epistemic Validation.” Journal of Memory and Language 59 (3): 237–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.05.001.
  • Shieber, J. 2012. “Against Credibility.” Australasian Journal of Philosophy 90 (1): 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/00048402.2011.560953.
  • Shieber, J. 2015. Testimony: A Philosophical Introduction. New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.
  • Singer, M. 2006. “Verification of Text Ideas During Reading.” Journal of Memory and Language 54 (4): 574–591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.11.003.
  • Singer, M. 2013. “Validation in Reading Comprehension.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 22 (5): 361–366. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413495236.
  • Singer, M. 2017. “Memory for Text and Discourse: Retrieval and Comprehension.” In Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference, 357–381. Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.21065-1
  • Singer, M. 2019. “Challenges in Processes of Validation and Comprehension.” Discourse Processes 56 (5-6): 465–483. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2019.1598167.
  • Sparks, J. R., and D. N. Rapp. 2011. “Readers' Reliance on Source Credibility in the Service of Comprehension.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 37 (1): 230–247. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021331.
  • Sperber, D. 1997. “Intuitive and Reflective Beliefs.” Mind and Language 12 (1): 67–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00036.
  • Sperber, D. 2000. “Introduction.” In Metarepresentations: A Multidisciplinary Perspective, edited by D. Sperber, 3–13. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Sperber, D. 2013. “Speakers Are Honest Because Hearers Are Vigilant Reply to Kourken Michaelian.” Episteme 10 (1): 61–71. https://doi.org/10.1017/epi.2013.7.
  • Sperber, D., F. Clément, C. Heintz, O. Mascaro, H. Mercier, G. Origgi, and D. Wilson. 2010. “Epistemic Vigilance.” Mind & Language 25 (4): 359–393. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.x.
  • Street, C. N. H., and D. C. Richardson. 2015. “Descartes versus Spinoza: Truth, Uncertainty, and Bias.” Social Cognition 33 (3): 227–239. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2015.33.2.2.
  • Thagard, P. 2005. “Testimony, Credibility, and Explanatory Coherence.” Erkenntnis 63 (3): 295–316. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10670-005-4004-2.
  • van Dijk, T. A., and W. Kintsch. 1983. Strategies of Discourse Comprehension. New York: Academic Press.
  • Weil, R., Y. Schul, and R. Mayo. 2020. “Correction of Evident Falsehood Requires Explicit Negation.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000635.
  • Wyer, R. S., and G. A. Radvansky. 1999. “The Comprehension and Validation of Social Information.” Psychological Review 106 (1): 89–118. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.89.
  • Zwaan, R. A. 2016. “Situation Models, Mental Simulations, and Abstract Concepts in Discourse Comprehension.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23 (4): 1028–1034. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0864-x.
  • Zwaan, R. A., M. C. Langston, and A. C. Graesser. 1995. “The Construction of Situation Models in Narrative Comprehension: An Event-Indexing Model.” Psychological Science 6 (5): 292–297. Retrieved from JSTOR. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00513.x
  • Zwaan, R. A., and G. A. Radvansky. 1998. “Situation Models in Language Comprehension and Memory.” Psychological Bulletin 123 (2): 162–185. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.123.2.162.