82
Views
78
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

The Effects of Time and Distance on Accuracy of Target-Directed Locomotion

Does an Accurate Short-Term Memory for Spatial Location Exist?

&
Pages 399-415 | Received 14 Mar 1988, Published online: 13 Aug 2013

References

  • Adams, J. A., & Dijsktra, S. (1966). Short-term memory for motor responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 314–318.
  • Corballis, M. C. (1982). Mental rotation: Anatomy of a paradigm. In M. Potegal (Ed.), Spatial abilities: Development and physiological foundations (pp. 173–198). New York: Academic Press.
  • Dale, H. C. A. (1973). Short-term memory for visual information. British Journal of Psychology, 64, 1–8.
  • Elliott, D. (1986). Continuous visual information may be important after all: A failure to replicate Thomson. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 388–391.
  • Fisk, J. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1985). The organization of eye and limb movements during unrestricted reaching to targets in contralateral and ipsilateral visual space. Experimental Brain Research, 60, 159–178.
  • Gibson, E. J., & Bergman, R. (1954). The effect of training on absolute estimation of distance over the ground. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 473–482.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Goodale, M. A. (1983a). Neural mechanisms of visual orientation in rodents: Targets versus places. In M. Jeannerod & A. Hein (Eds.), Spatially oriented behavior (pp. 3557). New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Goodale, M. A. (1983b). Vision as a sensorimotor system. In T. E. Robinson (Ed.), Behavioral Approaches to Brain Research (pp. 41–61). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Goodale, M. A. (1988). Modularity in visuomotor control: From input to output. In Z. Pylyshyn (Ed.), Computational processes in human vision: An interdisciplinary perspective, (pp. 262–285). Norwood, N. J.: Ablex Publishing.
  • Goodale, M. A., Pelisson, D., & Prablanc, C. (1986). Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature, 320, 748–750.
  • Harway, N. I. (1963). Judgment of distance in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 385–390.
  • Ingle, D. J. (1982). Organization of visuomotor behaviors in vertebrates. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 67–109). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Kirk, R. E. (1968). Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sciences. Belmont, CA: Brook/Cole.
  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1985). Mental imagery ability. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Human abilities: An information-processing approach (pp. 151–172). New York: W. H. Freeman.
  • Lee, D. N. (1978). The functions of vision. In H. L. Pick and E. Saltzman (Eds.), Modes of perceiving and processing information. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Lee, D. N., & Thomson, J. A. (1982). Vision in action: The control of locomotion. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 411433). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Posner, M. I. (1967). Characteristics of visual and kinesthetic memory codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 103–107.
  • Poulton, E. C. (1975). Range effects in experiments on people. American Journal of Psychology, 88, 3–32.
  • Searle, L. V., & Taylor, F. V (1948). Studies of tracking behavior. I. Rate and time characteristics of simple corrective movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 615–631.
  • Skavenski, A. A., & Steinman, R. M. (1970). Control of eye position in the dark. Vision Research, 10, 193–203.
  • Slack, C. W. (1953). Some characteristics of the “range effect.” Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46(2), 76–80.
  • Steenhuis, R. E., & Goodale, M. A. (1986). Short-term memory for spatial location: A target-directed locomotion task for normal human subjects. Society for Neurosciences Abstracts, 12, 1447.
  • Thomson, J. A. (1980). How do we use visual information to control locomotion? Trends in Neurosciences, 3, 247–250.
  • Thomson, J. A. (1983). Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 427–443.
  • Thomson, J. A. (1986). Intermittent versus continuous visual control: A reply to Elliott. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 392393.
  • Woodworth, R. S. (1899). The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychological Review, 3, (Monograph Supplement, 2), 1–119.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.