Publication Cover
The Journal of Psychology
Interdisciplinary and Applied
Volume 155, 2021 - Issue 1
498
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Neural and Linguistic Considerations for Assessing Moral Intuitions Using Text-Based Stimuli

Pages 90-114 | Received 27 Jul 2020, Accepted 29 Sep 2020, Published online: 12 Nov 2020

References

  • Allman, J. M., Tetreault, N. A., Hakeem, A. Y., Manaye, K. F., Semendeferi, K., Erwin, J. M., Park, S., Goubert, V., & Hof, P. R. (2010). The von Economo neurons in frontoinsular and anterior cingulate cortex in great apes and humans. Brain Structure & Function, 214(5–6), 495–517. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0254-0
  • Bastin, C., Harrison, B. J., Davey, C. G., Moll, J., & Whittle, S. (2016). Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 71, 455–471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.019
  • Bentin, S., & Deouell, L. Y. (2000). Structural encoding and identification in face processing: Erp evidence for separate mechanisms. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17(1), 35–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/026432900380472
  • Bretl, B. L. (2020). Assessing the structure of moral intuitions in early adolescence [Doctoral dissertation]. University of Kansas.
  • Brunet-Gouet, E., Achim, A. M., Vistoli, D., Passerieux, C., Hardy-Baylé, M.-C., & Jackson, P. L. (2011). The study of social cognition with neuroimaging methods as a means to explore future directions of deficit evaluation in schizophrenia? Psychiatry Research, 190(1), 23–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.11.029
  • Cacciari, C., & Padovani, R. (2007). Further evidence of gender stereotype priming in language: Semantic facilitation and inhibition in Italian role nouns. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(2), 277–293. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070142
  • Cannon, P. R., Schnall, S., & White, M. (2011). Transgressions and expressions: Affective facial muscle activity predicts moral judgments. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(3), 325–331. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550610390525
  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press.
  • Carey, S. (2011). Précis of 'The Origin of Concepts'. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(3), 113–124. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000919
  • Carrington, S. J., & Bailey, A. J. (2009). Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature. Human Brain Mapping, 30(8), 2313–2335. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20671
  • Carter, R. M., & Huettel, S. A. (2013). A nexus model of the temporal-parietal junction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(7), 328–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.007
  • Chen, T., Cai, W., Ryali, S., Supekar, K., & Menon, V. (2016). Distinct global brain dynamics and spatiotemporal organization of the salience network. PLoS Biology, 14(6), e1002469. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002469
  • Clifford, S., Iyengar, V., Cabeza, R., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2015). Moral foundations vignettes: A standardized stimulus database of scenarios based on moral foundations theory. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 1178–1198. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0551-2
  • Curry, O. S. (2016). The evolution of morality. In T. K. Shackelford & R. D. Hansen (Eds.), Morality as cooperation: A problem-centred approach (pp. 27–51). Springer.
  • Davey, C. G., Pujol, J., & Harrison, B. J. (2016). Mapping the self in the brain’s default mode network. NeuroImage, 132, 390–397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.022
  • Decety, J., & Cacioppo, S. (2012). The speed of morality: A high-density electrical neuroimaging study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108(11), 3068–3072. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00473.2012
  • Decety, J., & Cowell, J. M. (2014). The complex relation between morality and empathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 337–339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.008
  • Dickerson, S. S., Gruenewald, T. L., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). When the social self is threatened: Shame, physiology, and health. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1191–1216. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00295.x
  • Eimer, M., & McCarthy, R. (1999). Prosopagnosia and structural encoding of faces: Evidence from event-related potentials. Neuroreport, 10(2), 255–259. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199902050-00010
  • Etkin, A., Egner, T., & Kalisch, R. (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(2), 85–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004
  • Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Power, J. D., Dosenbach, N. U. F., Church, J. A., Miezin, F. M., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2009). Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization. PLoS Computational Biology, 5(5), e1000381. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381
  • Fairhall, S. L., & Ishai, A. (2007). Effective connectivity within the distributed cortical network for face perception. Cerebral Cortex, 17(10), 2400–2406. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhl148
  • Feinberg, M., Antonenko, O., Willer, R., Horberg, E. J., & John, O. P. (2014). Gut check: Reappraisal of disgust helps explain liberal-conservative differences on issues of purity. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 14(3), 513–521. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033727
  • Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of 'theory of mind'. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 77–83.https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6
  • Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Moylan, J., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., & Anderson, A. W. (2000). The fusiform “face area” is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 495–504. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900562165
  • Geangu, E., Gibson, A., Kaduk, K., & Reid, V. M. (2013). The neural correlates of passively viewed sequences of true and false beliefs. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(4), 432–437. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss015
  • Geng, J. J., & Vossel, S. (2013). Re-evaluating the role of TPJ in attentional control: Contextual updating? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10 Part 2), 2608–2620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.010
  • Gollan, T. H., & Frost, R. (2001). Two routes to grammatical gender: Evidence from hebrew. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(6), 627–651. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014235223566 https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014235223566
  • Graham, J. (2010). Left gut, right gut: Ideology and automatic moral reactions. University of Virginia.
  • Graham, J., & Haidt, J., (2012). Sacred values and evil adversaries: A moral foundations approach. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), The social psychology of morality: Exploring the causes of good and evil (pp. 11–31). Magination Press. American Psychological Association.
  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S., & Ditto, P. (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Psychology, 47, 55–130. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00002-4
  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015141
  • Graham, J., Iyer, R., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 366–384. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021847
  • Green, S., Ralph, M. A. L., Moll, J., Stamatakis, E. A., Grafman, J., & Zahn, R. (2010). Selective functional integration between anterior temporal and distinct fronto-mesolimbic regions during guilt and indignation. NeuroImage, 52(4), 1720–1726. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.038
  • Greene, J. D. (2009). The cognitive neuroscience of moral judgment. The Cognitive Neurosciences, 4, 1–48.
  • Gu, Z., Shi, L., & Lu, B. (2011). Evidence of rapid gender processing revealed by ERSP. 2011 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. Boston, MA, USA. https://doi.org/10.1109/IEMBS.2011.6091840
  • Gui, D.-Y., Gan, T., & Liu, C. (2016). Neural evidence for moral intuition and the temporal dynamics of interactions between emotional processes and moral cognition. Social Neuroscience, 11(4), 380–394. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1081401
  • Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Pantheon.
  • Hammer, A., Goebel, R., Schwarzbach, J., Münte, T. F., & Jansma, B. M. (2007). When sex meets syntactic gender on a neural basis during pronoun processing. Brain Research, 1146, 185–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.110
  • Han, H. (2017). Neural correlates of moral sensitivity and moral judgment associated with brain circuitries of selfhood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Moral Education, 46(2), 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2016.1262834
  • Han, H., Chen, J., Jeong, C., & Glover, G. H. (2016). Influence of the cortical midline structures on moral emotion and motivation in moral decision-making. Behavioural Brain Research, 302, 237–251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.01.001
  • Hansen, D., Bretl, B. L., & Amini, B. (2019). Adolescent development in context: Social, psychological, and neurological foundations. University of Kansas Libraries.
  • Hauk, O., Davis, M. H., Ford, M., Pulvermüller, F., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2006). The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data. NeuroImage, 30(4), 1383–1400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.048
  • Hauser, M. D. (2006). The liver and the moral organ. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(3), 214–220. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl026
  • Heim, S., Alter, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). A dual-route account for access to grammatical gender: Evidence from functional MRI. Anatomy and Embryology, 210(5–6), 473–483. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-005-0032-6
  • Heim, S., Eickhoff, S. B., Opitz, B., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). BA 44 in Broca’s area supports syntactic gender decisions in language production. NeuroReport, 17(11), 1097–1101. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000224775.05023.42
  • Helion, C., & Ochsner, K. N. (2018). The role of emotion regulation in moral judgment. Neuroethics, 11(3), 297–308. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-016-9261-z
  • Helion, C., & Pizarro, D. A. (2015). Beyond dual-processes: The interplay of reason and emotion in moral judgment. In N. Levy & J. Clausen (Eds.), Handbook of neuroethics (pp. 109–125). Springer.
  • Henrich, J. (2016). The secrete of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.
  • Hsu, C.-T., Jacobs, A. M., & Conrad, M. (2015). Can Harry Potter still put a spell on us in a second language? An fMRI study on reading emotion-laden literature in late bilinguals. Cortex, 63, 282–295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.09.002
  • Jeurissen, D., Sack, A. T., Roebroeck, A., Russ, B., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2014). TMS affects moral judgment, showing the role of DLPFC and TPJ in cognitive and emotional processing. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2014.00018
  • Johnson, M. H. (2005). Subcortical face processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 766–774. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1766
  • Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 4302–4311.
  • Kaul, C., Rees, G., & Ishai, A. (2011). The gender of face stimuli is represented in multiple regions in the human brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 238. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00238
  • Kennison, S. M., & Trofe, J. L. (2003). Comprehending pronouns: A role for word-specific gender stereotype information. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(3), 355–378. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023599719948
  • Kiefer, M., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012). Conceptual representations in mind and brain: Theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Cortex, 48(7), 805–825. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.006
  • Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Winkler, I., & Junghofer, M. (2009). Emotion and attention in visual word processing: An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 80(1), 75–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.004
  • Krajbich, I., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Denburg, N. L., & Camerer, C. F. (2009). Economic games quantify diminished sense of guilt in patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(7), 2188–2192. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5086-08.2009
  • Landy, J. F. (2016). Representations of moral violations: Category members and associated features. Judgment & Decision Making, 11(5), 496–508.
  • LeDoux, J. E. (2003). Synaptic self: How our brains become who we are. Penguin.
  • Leech, R., Kamourieh, S., Beckmann, C. F., & Sharp, D. J. (2011). Fractionating the default mode network: Distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(9), 3217–3224.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5626-10.2011
  • Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. Psychoanalytic Review, 58(3), 419–438.
  • Li, M., Lu, S., & Zhong, N. (2016). The parahippocampal cortex mediates contextual associative memory: Evidence from an fMRI study. BioMed Research International, 2016, 9860604. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/9860604
  • Liu, Y., Angstadt, M., Taylor, S. F., & Fitzgerald, K. D. (2016). The typical development of posterior medial frontal cortex function and connectivity during task control demands in youth 8-19years old. NeuroImage, 137, 97–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.019
  • Malle, B. F., & Guglielmo, S. (2012). Are intentionality judgments fundamentally moral? In C. Mackenzie & R. Langdon (Eds.), Emotions, imagination, and moral reasoning (pp. 275–293). Taylor & Francis.
  • Marí-Beffa, P., Valdés, B., Cullen, D. J. D., Catena, A., & Houghton, G. (2005). ERP analyses of task effects on semantic processing from words. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 23(2–3), 293–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.10.016
  • Maurer, D., O'Craven, K. M., Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Springer, M. V., Lewis, T. L., & Grady, C. L. (2007). Neural correlates of processing facial identity based on features versus their spacing. Neuropsychologia, 45(7), 1438–1451. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.016
  • McCleery, J. P., Surtees, A. D. R., Graham, K. A., Richards, J. E., & Apperly, I. A. (2011). The neural and cognitive time course of theory of mind. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(36), 12849–12854. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1392-11.2011
  • Meinhardt, J., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., Döhnel, K., & Sommer, M. (2011). True- and false-belief reasoning in children and adults: An event-related potential study of theory of mind. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 67–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2010.08.001
  • Moll, J., De Oliveira-Souza, R., & Zahn, R. (2008). The neural basis of moral cognition: Sentiments, concepts, and values. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 161–180. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.005
  • Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., & Giard, M. H. (2003). Electrophysiological correlates of age and gender perception on human faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(6), 900–910. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903322370816
  • Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M.-H., Bentin, S., Aguera, P.-E., & Pernier, J. (2000). Neurophysiological correlates of face gender processing in humans. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 12(1), 303–310. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00888.x
  • Nachev, P., Wydell, H., O’Neill, K., Husain, M., & Kennard, C. (2007). The role of the pre-supplementary motor area in the control of action. NeuroImage, 36, T155–T163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.034
  • Neumann, R., & Lozo, L. (2012). Priming the activation of fear and disgust: Evidence for semantic processing. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 12(2), 223–228. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026500
  • Northoff, G., Wiebking, C., Feinberg, T., & Panksepp, J. (2011). The 'resting-state hypothesis' of major depressive disorder – A translational subcortical-cortical framework for a system disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(9), 1929–1945. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.007
  • Oaten, M., Williams, K. D., Jones, A., & Zadro, L. (2008). The effects of ostracism on self-regulation in the socially anxious. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27(5), 471–504. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2008.27.5.471
  • Ochsner, K. N., Silvers, J. A., & Buhle, J. T. (2012). Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: A synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251(1), E1–E24.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x
  • Palazova, M., Mantwill, K., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2011). Are effects of emotion in single words non-lexical? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2766–2775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.005
  • Panksepp, J., Lane, R. D., Solms, M., & Smith, R. (2017). Reconciling cognitive and affective neuroscience perspectives on the brain basis of emotional experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 76(Part B), 187–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.010
  • Paré, D., Collins, D. R., & Pelletier, J. G. (2002). Amygdala oscillations and the consolidation of emotional memories. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(7), 306–314. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01924-1 https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01924-1
  • Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976–987. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2277
  • Peelen, M. V., Wiggett, A. J., & Downing, P. E. (2006). Patterns of fMRI activity dissociate overlapping functional brain areas that respond to biological motion. Neuron, 49(6), 815–822. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.004
  • Podrebarac, S. K., Goodale, M. A., van der Zwan, R., & Snow, J. C. (2013). Gender-selective neural populations: Evidence from event-related fMRI repetition suppression. Experimental Brain Research, 226(2), 241–252. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3429-0
  • Pylkkänen, L., & Marantz, A. (2003). Tracking the time course of word recognition with MEG. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(5), 187–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00092-5 https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00092-5
  • Pyykkönen, P., Hyönä, J., & van Gompel, R. P. G. (2010). Activating gender stereotypes during online spoken language processing: Evidence from visual world eye tracking. Experimental Psychology, 57(2), 126–133. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000016
  • Rakić, T., Steffens, M. C., & Wiese, H. (2018). Same-gender distractors are not so easy to reject: ERP evidence of gender categorization. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(5), 825–836. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0607-3
  • Rellecke, J., Palazova, M., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2011). On the automaticity of emotion processing in words and faces: Event-related brain potentials evidence from a superficial task. Brain and Cognition, 77(1), 23–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.001
  • Rest, J. R., Narvaez, D., Thoma, S. J., & Bebeau, M. J. (1999). DIT2: Devising and testing a revised instrument of moral judgment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(4), 644–659. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.4.644
  • Rhodes, M., & Chalik, L. (2013). Social categories as markers of intrinsic interpersonal obligations. Psychological Science, 24(6), 999–1006. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612466267
  • Rosa Salva, O., Mayer, U., & Vallortigara, G. (2015). Roots of a social brain: Developmental models of emerging animacy-detection mechanisms. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 50, 150–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.015
  • Russell, P. S., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2011). Moral anger, but not moral disgust, responds to intentionality. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 11(2), 233–240. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022598
  • Sacco, D. F., Wirth, J. H., Hugenberg, K., Chen, Z., & Williams, K. D. (2011). The world in black and white: Ostracism enhances the categorical perception of social information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(4), 836–842. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.03.001
  • Scarná, A., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). On the assessment of grammatical gender knowledge in aphasia: The danger of relying on explicit metalinguistic tasks. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17(2), 185–201.
  • Scholl, B. J., & Gao, T. (2013). Perceiving animacy and intentionality: Visual processing or higher-level judgment. In V. Kuhlmeier & M. D. Rutherford (Eds.), Social perception: Detection and interpretation of animacy, agency, and intention (Vol. 4629, pp. 197–229). MIT Press.
  • Seghier, M., & Price, C. (2012). Functional heterogeneity within the default network during semantic processing and speech production. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 281. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00281
  • Silva, C., Montant, M., Ponz, A., & Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Emotions in reading: Disgust, empathy and the contextual learning hypothesis. Cognition, 125(2), 333–338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.013
  • Sommerville, J. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2005). Pulling out the intentional structure of action: The relation between action processing and action production in infancy. Cognition, 95(1), 1–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.12.004
  • Tappé, M., Bensman, L., Hayashi, K., & Hatfield, E. (2013). Gender differences in receptivity to sexual offers: A new research prototype. Interpersona: An International Journal on Personal Relationships, 7(2), 323–344. https://doi.org/10.5964/ijpr.v7i2.121
  • Thiruchselvam, R., Blechert, J., Sheppes, G., Rydstrom, A., & Gross, J. J. (2011). The temporal dynamics of emotion regulation: An EEG study of distraction and reappraisal. Biological Psychology, 87(1), 84–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.009
  • Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). TARGET ARTICLE: "Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical model. Psychological Inquiry, 15(2), 103–125. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1502_01
  • Turiel, E. (2010). Domain specificity in social interactions, social thought, and social development. Child Development, 81(3), 720–726. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01429.x
  • Van Berkum, J. J. A., Holleman, B., Nieuwland, M., Otten, M., & Murre, J. (2009). Right or wrong?: The brain’s fast response to morally objectionable statements. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1092–1099. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02411.x
  • van Herten, M., Kolk, H. H. J., & Chwilla, D. J. (2005). An ERP study of P600 effects elicited by semantic anomalies. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 241–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.002
  • Vistoli, D., Brunet-Gouet, E., Baup-Bobin, E., Hardy-Bayle, M. C., & Passerieux, C. (2011). Anatomical and temporal architecture of theory of mind: A MEG insight into the early stages. NeuroImage, 54(2), 1406–1414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.015
  • Vistoli, D., Passerieux, C., El Zein, M., Clumeck, C., Braun, S., & Brunet-Gouet, E. (2015). Characterizing an ERP correlate of intentions understanding using a sequential comic strips paradigm. Social Neuroscience, 10(4), 391–407. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2014.1003272
  • Wang, P., Yang, Y.-P., Tan, C.-H., Chen, Q.-W., & Cantfort, T. (2017). Gender stereotype activation versus lexical semantic activation: An ERP study. The Journal of General Psychology, 144(4), 283–308. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2017.1310714
  • Weitzman, O., Kemeny, M., & Fahey, J. (2004). HIV-related shame and guilt predict CD4 decline [Unpublished Manuscript].
  • Westermann, S., Rief, W., Euteneuer, F., & Kohlmann, S. (2015). Social exclusion and shame in obesity. Eating Behaviors, 17, 74–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.01.001
  • Wheeler, S. C., & Petty, R. E. (2001). The effects of stereotype activation on behavior: A review of possible mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 127(6), 797–826. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.797
  • Wiese, H., Kloth, N., Güllmar, D., Reichenbach, J. R., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2012). Perceiving age and gender in unfamiliar faces: An fMRI study on face categorization. Brain and Cognition, 78(2), 163–168.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.10.012
  • Wise, R., Chollet, F., Hadar, U., Friston, K., Hoffner, E., & Frackowiak, R. (1991). Distribution of cortical neural networks involved in word comprehension and word retrieval. Brain, 114(4), 1803–1817. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/114.4.1803
  • Wong, Y., & Tsai, J. (2007). Cultural models of shame and guilt. In J. L. Tracy, J. P. Tangney, & R. W. Robins (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 209–223). Guilford Press.
  • Yoder, K. J., Harenski, C., Kiehl, K. A., & Decety, J. (2015). Neural networks underlying implicit and explicit moral evaluations in psychopathy. Translational Psychiatry, 5(8), e625. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.117
  • Young, L., & Dungan, J. (2012). Where in the brain is morality? Everywhere and maybe nowhere. Social Neuroscience, 7(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2011.569146
  • Zhang, X., Li, Q., Sun, S., & Zuo, B. (2018). The time course from gender categorization to gender-stereotype activation. Social Neuroscience, 13(1), 52–60. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.125 https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1251965
  • Zhu, R., Wu, H., Xu, Z., Tang, H., Shen, X., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2019). Early distinction between shame and guilt processing in an interpersonal context. Social Neuroscience, 14(1), 53–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2017.1391119

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.