1,783
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Editorial

Promoting Transparent Reporting of Conflicts of Interests and Statistical Analyses at The Journal of Sex Research

&

References

  • American Psychological Association. (2009). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
  • Baker, M. (2016, May 25). 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility. Nature. Retrieved from http://www.nature.com/news/1-500-scientists-lift-the-lid-on-reproducibility-1.19970
  • Bakker, M., & Wicherts, J. M. (2011). The (mis)reporting of statistical results in psychology journals. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 666–678. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0089-5
  • Balzarini, R. N., Dobson, K., Chin, K., & Campbell, L. (2017). Does exposure to erotica reduce attraction and love for romantic partners in men? Independent replications of Kenrick, Gutierres, and Goldberg (1989) Study 2. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 191–197. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2016.11.003
  • Begley, C. G., & Ellis, L. M. (2012). Drug development: Raise standards for preclinical cancer research. Nature, 483(7391), 531–533. doi:10.1038/483531a
  • Belia, S., Fidler, F., Williams, J., & Cumming, G. (2005). Researchers misunderstand confidence intervals and standard error bars. Psychological Methods, 10, 389–396. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.10.4.389
  • Card, N. A. (2012). Applied meta-analysis for social science research. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Carter, E. C., Schönbrodt, F. D., Gervais, W. M., & Hilgard, J. (2017, September 26). Correcting for bias in psychology: A comparison of meta-analytic methods. Retrieved from psyarxiv.com/9h3nu
  • Chan, M. E., & Arvey, R. D. (2012). Meta-analysis and the development of knowledge. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 79–92. doi:10.1177/1745691611429355
  • Crandall, C. S., & Sherman, J. W. (2016). On the scientific superiority of conceptual replications for scientific progress. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 66, 93–99. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.002
  • Cumming, G. (2014). The new statistics: Why and how. Psychological Science, 25, 7–29. doi:10.1177/0956797613504966
  • DeAngelis, C. D. (2000). Conflict of interest and the public trust. JAMA, 284, 2237–2238. doi:10.1001/jama.284.17.2237
  • Dienes, Z. (2008). Understanding psychology as a science: An introduction to scientific and statistical inference. Hampshire, United Kingdom: Palgrave Macmillan.
  • Epskamp, S., & Nuijten, M. B. (2016). Statcheck: Extract statistics from articles and recompute p values. R package version 1.2.2. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=statcheck
  • Epskamp, S., Nuijten, M. B., & Rife, S. (2016). Statcheck on the web. Retrieved from http://statcheck.io
  • Etz, A., & Vandekerckhove, J. (2016). A Bayesian perspective on the reproducibility project: Psychology. PLOS ONE, 11(2), e0149794. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0149794
  • Fanelli, D. (2009). How many scientists fabricate and falsify research? A systematic review and meta-analysis of survey data. PLOS ONE, 4(5), e5738. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005738
  • Fanelli, D. (2010). “Positive” results increase down the hierarchy of the sciences. PLOS ONE, 5(4,), e10068. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010068
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146
  • Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W., & Reis, H. T. (2017). Replicability and other features of a high-quality science: Toward a balanced and empirical approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(2), 244–253. doi:10.1037/pspi0000075
  • Gelman, A., & Loken, E. (2013). The garden of forking paths: Why multiple comparisons can be a problem, even when there is no “fishing expedition” or “p-hacking” and the research hypothesis was posited ahead of time. Retrieved from http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~gelman/research/unpublished/p_hacking.pdf
  • Gilbert, D. T., King, G., Pettigrew, S., & Wilson, T. D. (2016). Comment on “Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science.” Science, 351(6277), 1037. doi:10.1126/science.aad7243
  • Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581–592. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581
  • International Committee of Medical Journal Editors. (2010). Uniform requirements for manuscripts submitted to biomedical journals: Writing and editing for biomedical publication. Journal of Pharmacology and Pharmacotherapeutics, 1, 42–58.
  • Ioannidis, J. P. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLOS Medicine, 2(8), e124. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124
  • John, L. K., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2012). Measuring the prevalence of questionable research practices with incentives for truth telling. Psychological Science, 23, 524–532. doi:10.1177/0956797611430953
  • Johns, M. E., Barnes, M., & Florencio, P. S. (2003). Restoring balance to industry–academia relationships in an era of institutional financial conflicts of interest: Promoting research while maintaining trust. JAMA, 289, 741–746. doi:10.1001/jama.289.6.741
  • Judd, C. M., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. A. (2017). Experiments with more than one random factor: Designs, analytic models, and statistical power. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 601–625. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033702
  • Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863
  • Lakens, D. (2014). Performing high‐powered studies efficiently with sequential analyses. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 701–710. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2023
  • Lakens, D., Hilgard, J., & Staaks, J. (2016). On the reproducibility of meta-analyses: Six practical recommendations. BMC Psychology, 4, 24. doi:10.1186/s40359-016-0126-3
  • Lalumière, M. (2016, September). Why I no longer care about p values, or the notion of “statistical significance”: A user’s perspective. Paper presented at the Canadian Sex Research Forum, Quebec City, Canada.
  • LeBel, E. P., Berger, D., Campbell, L., & Loving, T. J. (2017). Falsifiability is not optional. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(2), 254–261. doi:10.1037/pspi0000106
  • Maxwell, S. E. (2004). The persistence of underpowered studies in psychological research: Causes, consequences, and remedies. Psychological Methods, 9, 147–163. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.9.2.147
  • Maxwell, S. E., Delaney, H. D., & Kelley, K. (in press). Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. Retrieved from https://designingexperiments.com/shiny-r-web-apps/
  • Nelson, L. D., Simmons, J., & Simonsohn, U. (2017). Psychology’s renaissance. Annual Review of Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011836
  • Nosek, B. A., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2012). Scientific utopia: I. Opening scientific communication. Psychological Inquiry, 23, 217–243. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2012.692215
  • Nosek, B. A., Ebersole, C. R., DeHaven, A. C., & Mellor, D. T. (2017, August 24). The preregistration revolution. Retrieved from osf.io/2dxu5
  • Nosek, B. A., & Errington, T. M. (2017). Reproducibility in cancer biology: Making sense of replications. eLife, 6, e23383. doi:10.7554/eLife.23383
  • Nosek, B. A., Spies, J. R., & Motyl, M. (2012). Scientific utopia: II. Restructuring incentives and practices to promote truth over publishability. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 615–631. doi:10.1177/1745691612459058
  • Nuijten, M. B., Hartgerink, C. H., Van Assen, M. A., Epskamp, S., & Wicherts, J. M. (2016). The prevalence of statistical reporting errors in psychology (1985–2013). Behavior Research Methods, 48, 1205–1226. doi:10.3758/s13428-015-0664-2
  • Office of Research Integrity, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2017, September 25). A brief overview on conflict of interests. Retrieved from https://ori.hhs.gov/plagiarism-35
  • Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), aac4716. doi:10.1126/science.aac4716
  • Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1992). When small effects are impressive. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 160–164. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.160
  • Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F., Jr., & Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred years of social psychology quantitatively described. Review of General Psychology, 7, 331–363. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.7.4.331
  • Sakaluk, J. K. (2016a). Exploring small, confirming big: An alternative system to the new statistics for advancing cumulative and replicable psychological research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 66, 47–54. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2015.09.013
  • Sakaluk, J. K. (2016b). Promoting replicable sexual science: A methodological review and call for metascience. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 25, 1–8. doi:10.3138/cjhs.251-CO1
  • Sakaluk, J. K. (2017, January). What the replication crisis means for sexual science—And why sexual scientists should care. Paper presented at the Sexuality Preconference of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, San Antonio, TX.
  • Sakaluk, J. K., Williams, A., & Biernat, M. (2014). Analytic review as a solution to the misreporting of statistical results in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 652–660. doi:10.1177/1745691614549257
  • Schimmack, U. (2012). The ironic effect of significant results on the credibility of multiple-study articles. Psychological Methods, 17, 551–566. doi:10.1037/a0029487
  • Schönbrodt, F. D. (2015). p-checker: One-for-all p-value analyzer. Retrieved from http://shinyapps.org/apps/p-checker/.
  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22, 1359–1366. doi:10.1177/0956797611417632
  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2012). A 21 word solution. Dialogue, the Official Newsletter of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, 26(2), 4–7.
  • Simons, D. J. (2014). The value of direct replication. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 76–80. doi:10.1177/1745691613514755
  • Spellman, B. (2015). A short (personal) future history of revolution 2.0. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 886–899. doi:10.1177/1745691615609918
  • Stapel, D. (2014). Faking science: A true story of academic fraud ( Trans. N. J. L. Brown.). Retrieved from https://errorstatistics.files.wordpress.com/2014/12/fakingscience-20141214.pdf
  • Westfall, J. (2016). PANGEA: Power ANalysis for GEneral Anova designs. Unpublished manuscript. Retrieved from http://jakewestfall.org/publications/pangea.pdf.
  • Wicherts, J. M., Bakker, M., & Molenaar, D. (2011). Willingness to share research data is related to the strength of the evidence and the quality of reporting of statistical results. PLOS ONE, 6(11), e26828. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026828
  • Wicherts, J. M., Borsboom, D., Kats, J., & Molenaar, D. (2006). The poor availability of psychological research data for reanalysis. American Psychologist, 61, 726–728. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.726

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.