354
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
ARTICLES

Individual Differences in Reading Speed are Linked to Variability in the Processing of Lexical and Contextual Information: Evidence from Single-trial Event-related Brain Potentials

&

References

  • Andrews, Sally. 2012. Individual differences in skilled visual word recognition and reading. Visual Word Recognition 2. 151–72.
  • Ashby, Jane, Keith Rayner, and Charles Clifton. 2005. Eye movements of highly skilled and average readers: Differential effects of frequency and predictability. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 58(6). 1065–86.
  • Barber, Horacio A., Leun J. Otten, Stavroula-Thaleia Kousta, and Gabriella Vigliocco. 2013. Concreteness in word processing: ERP and behavioral effects in a lexical decision task. Brain and Language 125(1). 47–53.
  • Barr, Dale J. 2013. Random effects structure for testing interactions in linear mixed-effects models. Frontiers in Psychology 4. 328.
  • Barr, Dale J., Roger Levy, Christoph Scheepers, and Harry J. Tily. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68(3). 255–78.
  • Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Ben Bolker, and Steve Walker. 2014. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint arXiv:1406.5823.
  • Bates, Douglas, Reinhold Kliegl, Shravan Vasishth, and Harald Baayen. 2015. Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv preprint arXiv:1506.04967.
  • Bauer, Daniel J., and Patrick J. Curran. 2005. Probing interactions in fixed and multilevel regression: Inferential and graphical techniques. Multivariate Behavioral Research 40(3). 373–400.
  • Berkum, Jos J. V., Peter Hagoort, and C. Mackenzie Brown. 1999. Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: evidence from the N400. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11(6). 657–71.
  • Boudewyn, Megan Ann, Debra L. Long, and Tamara Y. Swaab. 2013. Effects of working memory span on processing of lexical associations and congruence in spoken discourse. Frontiers in Psychology 4. 1–16.
  • Brysbaert, Marc, and Boris New. 2009. Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods 41(4). 977–90.
  • Camblin, C. Christine, Peter C. Gordon, and Tamara Y. Swaab. 2007(a). The interplay of discourse congruence and lexical association during sentence processing: evidence from ERPs and eye tracking. Journal of Memory and Language 56(1). 103–28.
  • Camblin, C. Christine, Kerry Ledoux, Megan Boudewyn, Peter C. Gordon, and Tamara Y. Swaab. 2007(b). Processing new and repeated names: effects of coreference on repetition priming with speech and fast RSVP. Brain Research 1146. 172–84.
  • Coltheart, Max, Eileen Davelaar, Jón Torfi Jónasson, and Derek Besner. 1977. Access to the internal lexicon. In Stan Dornic (ed.), Attention & Performance VI, 535–55. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Cumming, Geoff. 2014. The new statistics: Why and how. Psychological Science 25(1). 7–29.
  • Curran, Patrick. J., Daniel J. Bauer, and Michael T. Willoughby. 2004. Testing main effects and interactions in latent curve analysis. Psychological Methods 9(2). 220.
  • Dambacher, Michael, Reinhold Kliegl, Markus Hofmann, and Arthur M. Jacobs. 2006. Frequency and predictability effects on event-related potentials during reading. Brain Research 1084(1). 89–103.
  • Delorme, Arnaud, and Scott Makeig. 2004. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 134(1). 9–21.
  • Dimigen, Olaf, Werner Sommer, Annette Hohlfeld, Arthur M. Jacobs, and Reinhold Kliegl. 2011. Coregistration of eye movements and EEG in natural reading: analyses and review. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 140(4). 552.
  • Ditman, Tall, Phillip J. Holcomb, and Gina R. Kuperberg. 2007. An investigation of concurrent ERP and self-paced reading methodologies. Psychophysiology 44(6). 927–35.
  • Everatt, John, and Geoffrey Underwood. 1994. Individual differences in reading subprocesses: Relationships between reading ability, lexical access, and eye movement control. Language and Speech 37(3). 283–97.
  • Fabiani, Monika, Gabriele Gratton, and Kara D Federmeier. 2007. Event-related brain potentials: methods, theory, and applications. In John T. Cacioppo, Louis G. Tassinary, and Gary G. Berntson (eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology, 85–119, New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Federmeier, Kara D. 2007. Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology 44(4). 491–505.
  • Federmeier, Kara D., and Sarah Laszlo. 2009. Time for meaning: electrophysiology provides insights into the dynamics of representation and processing in semantic memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 51. 1–44.
  • Federmeier, Kara D, Edward W Wlotko, Esmerelda De Ochoa-Dewald, and Marta Kutas. 2007. Multiple effects of sentential constraint on word processing. Brain Research 1146. 75–84.
  • Fischer-Baum, Simon, Daniel S. Dickson, and Kara D. Federmeier. 2014. Frequency and regularity effects in reading are task dependent: evidence from ERPs. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29(10). 1342–55.
  • Häikiö, Tuomo, Raymond Bertram, Jukka Hyönä, and Pekka Niemi. 2009. Development of the letter identity span in reading: evidence from the eye movement moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102(2). 167–81.
  • Hawelka, Stefan, Sarah Schuster, Benjamin Gagl, and Florian Hutzler. 2015. On forward inferences of fast and slow readers. An eye movement study. Scientific Reports 5. 8432.
  • Henderson, John M., and Fernanda Ferreira. 1990. Effects of foveal processing difficulty on the perceptual span in reading: implications for attention and eye movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16(3). 417.
  • Hersch, Jolyn, and Sally Andrews. 2012. Lexical quality and reading skill: bottom-up and top-down contributions to sentence processing. Scientific Studies of Reading 16(3). 240–62.
  • Holcomb, Phillip. J., Jonathan Grainger, and Tim O'rourke. 2002. An electrophysiological study of the effects of orthographic neighborhood size on printed word perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14(6). 938–50.
  • Jegerski, Jill. 2014. Self-paced reading. Research Methods in Second Language Psycholinguistics 20–49.
  • Jordan, Timothy R., Jasmine Dixon, Victoria A. McGowan, Stoyan Kurtev, and Kevin B. Paterson. 2016. Fast and slow readers and the effectiveness of the spatial frequency content of text: evidence from reading times and eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 42(8). 1066.
  • Kappenman, Emily. S., and Steven J Luck. 2012. ERP components: The ups and downs of brainwave recordings. The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components 1. 3–30.
  • Kintsch, Walter, and Janice Keenan. 1973. Reading rate and retention as a function of the number of propositions in the base structure of sentences. Cognitive Psychology 5(3). 257–74.
  • Kretzschmar, Franziska, Matthias Schlesewsky, and Adrian Staub. 2015. Dissociating word frequency and predictability effects in reading: evidence from coregistration of eye movements and EEG. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 41(6). 1648.
  • Krieber, Magdalena, Katrin D. Bartl-Pokorny, Florian B. Pokorny, Christa Einspieler, Andrea Langmann, Christof Körner, Terje Falck-Ytter, and Peter B. Marschik. 2016. The relation between reading skills and eye movement patterns in adolescent readers: evidence from a regular orthography. PloS One 11. e0145934.
  • Kutas, Marta, and Kara D Federmeier. 2000. Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(12). 463–70.
  • Kutas, Marta, and Kara D. Federmeier. 2011. Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology 62. 621–47.
  • Kutas, Marta, and Steven A. Hillyard. 1984. Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature 307(5947). 161.
  • Laszlo, Sarah, and Kara D. Federmeier. 2009. A beautiful day in the neighborhood: An event-related potential study of lexical relationships and prediction in context. Journal of Memory and Language 61(3). 326–38.
  • Laszlo, Sarah, and Kara D. Federmeier. 2014. Never seem to find the time: evaluating the physiological time course of visual word recognition with regression analysis of single-item event-related potentials. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29(5). 642–61.
  • Lau, Ellen F., Colin Phillips, and David Poeppel. 2008. A cortical network for semantics:(de) constructing the N400. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9(12). 920.
  • Ledoux, Kerry, Peter C. Gordon, C. Christine Camblin, and Tamara Y. Swaab. 2007. Coreference and lexical repetition: mechanisms of discourse integration. Memory & Cognition 35(4). 801–15.
  • Leinenger, Mallorie, and Keith Rayner. 2017. What we know about skilled, beginning, and older readers from monitoring their eye movements. Reading Comprehension in Educational Settings 16. 1.
  • Levenshtein, Vladimir I. 1966. Binary codes capable of correcting deletions, insertions, and reversals. Soviet Physics Doklady, 10. 707–10.
  • Lopez-Calderon, Javier, and Steven J. Luck. 2014. ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8. 213.
  • Lorch, Robert F., and Jerome L. Myers. 1990. Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16(1). 149.
  • Mitchell, Don C. 2004. On-line methods in language processing: Introduction and historical review. In Manuel Carreiras and Charles Clifton, Jr. (eds) The On-line study of sentence comprehension: Eyetracking, ERPs and Beyond, 15–32. New York: Psychological Press.
  • Murray, John D., and Kelly Burke. 2003. Activation and encoding of predictive inferences: The role of reading skill. Discourse Processes 35(2). 81–102.
  • Ng, Shukhan, Brennan R. Payne, Allison A. Steen, Elizabeth A. Stine-Morrow, and Kara D. Federmeier. 2017. Use of contextual information and prediction by struggling adult readers: evidence from reading times and event-related potentials. Scientific Studies of Reading 21(5). 359–75.
  • Nieuwland, Mante S., and Joe J. van Berkum. 2006. When peanuts fall in love: N400 evidence for the power of discourse. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18(7). 1098–111.
  • Payne, Brennan R., and Kara D. Federmeier. 2017. Pace yourself: Intraindividual variability in context use revealed by self-paced event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 29(5). 837–54.
  • Payne, Brennan R., and Kara D. Federmeier. 2018. Contextual constraints on lexico-semantic processing in aging: evidence from single-word event-related brain potentials. Brain Research 1687. 117–28.
  • Payne, Brennan R., Xuefei Gao, Soo Rim Noh, Carolyn Jane Anderson, and Elizabeth Stine-Morrow. 2012. The effects of print exposure on sentence processing and memory in older adults: evidence for efficiency and reserve. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 19(1-2). 122–49.
  • Payne, Brennan R., Sarah Grison, Xuefei Gao, Kiel Christianson, Daniel G. Morrow, and Elizabeth AL Stine-Morrow. 2014. Aging and individual differences in binding during sentence understanding: evidence from temporary and global syntactic attachment ambiguities. Cognition 130. 157–73.
  • Payne, Brennan R., Chia-lin Lee, and Kara D. Federmeier. 2015. Revisiting the incremental effects of context on word processing: evidence from single-word event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology 52(11). 1456–69.
  • Payne, Brennan R., and Jack W Silcox. 2019. Aging, context processing, and comprehension. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 71. 215–64.
  • Payne, Brennan R., and Elizabeth AL Stine-Morrow. 2014. Risk for mild cognitive impairment is associated with semantic integration deficits in sentence processing and memory. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 71. 243–53.
  • Perfetti, Charles. 2007. Reading ability: lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading 11(4). 357–83.
  • Preacher, Kristopher J., Patrick J. Curran, and Daniel J. Bauer. 2006. Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31(4), 437–48.
  • Rayner, Keith, Timothy J. Slattery, and Nethalie N. Bélanger. 2010. Eye movements, the perceptual span, and reading speed. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 17(6). 834–9.
  • Rommers, Joost, and Kara D. Federmeier. 2017. Tracking language through time using electrophysiological methods. In Annette M. B. de Groot and Peter Hagoort (eds.), Research methods in psycholinguistics: A practical guide, 247–65. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Rugg, Michael D. 1990. Event-related potentials dissociate repetition effects of high and low frequency words. Memory and Cognition 18. 367–79.
  • Slattery, Timothy J., and Mark Yates. 2018. Word skipping: effects of word length, predictability, spelling and reading skill. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 71(1). 250–9.
  • St. George, Marie, Suzanne Mannes, and James E. Hoffman. 1994. Global semantic expectancy and language comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 6(1). 70–83.
  • Steen-Baker, Allison A., Shukhan Ng, Brennan R. Payne, Carolyn J. Anderson, Kara D. Federmeier, and Elizabeth AL Stine-Morrow. 2017 The effects of context on processing words during sentence reading among adults varying in age and literacy skill. Psychology and Aging 32. 460–72.
  • Swaab, Tamara Y., C. Christine Camblin, and Peter C. Gordon. 2004. Electrophysiological evidence for reversed lexical repetition effects in language processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16(5). 715–26.
  • Taylor, Wilson. L. 1953. “Cloze procedure”: A new tool for measuring readability. Journalism Bulletin 30(4). 415–33.
  • Underwood, Geoffrey, Alison Hubbard, and Howard Wilkinson. 1990. Eye fixations predict reading comprehension: The relationships between reading skill, reading speed, and visual inspection. Language and Speech 33(1). 69–81.
  • Van Petten, Cyma. 1993. A comparison of lexical and sentence-level context effects in event-related potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes 8. 485–531.
  • Van Petten, Cyma, and Marta Kutas. 1990. Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. Memory & Cognition 18(4). 380–93.
  • Van Petten, Cyma, and Marta Kutas. 1991. Influences of semantic and syntactic context on open-and closed-class words. Memory & Cognition 19(1). 95–112.
  • Van Petten, Cyma, and Barbara J. Luka. 2006. Neural localization of semantic context effects in electromagnetic and hemodynamic studies. Brain and Language 97(3). 279–93.
  • Veldre, Aaron, and Sally Andrews. 2014. Lexical quality and eye movements: individual differences in the perceptual span of skilled adult readers. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 67. 703–27.
  • Vergara-Martínez, Marta, and Tamara Y Swaab. 2012. Orthographic neighborhood effects as a function of word frequency: An event-related potential study. Psychophysiology 49(9). 1277–89.
  • Wlotko, Edward W., and Kara D. Federmeier. 2012. So that's what you meant! event-related potentials reveal multiple aspects of context use during construction of message-level meaning. NeuroImage 62(1). 356–66.
  • Wlotko, Edward W., and Kara D. Federmeier. 2015. Time for prediction? The effect of presentation rate on predictive sentence comprehension during word-by-word reading. Cortex 68. 20–32.
  • Yarkoni, Tal, David Balota, and Melvin Yap. 2008. Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: A new measure of orthographic similarity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 15(5). 971–9.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.