Publication Cover
Educational Psychology
An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology
Volume 41, 2021 - Issue 6
289
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Evaluating the rapid automatized naming and arithmetical fluency relationship in Chilean first grade students

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 730-747 | Received 20 Nov 2019, Accepted 05 Mar 2021, Published online: 30 Mar 2021

References

  • Altani, A., Protopapas, A., & Georgiou, G. K. (2017). The contribution of executive functions to naming digits, objects, and words. Reading and Writing, 30(1), 121–141. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9666-4
  • Anthony, J., & Francis, D. (2005). Development of phonological awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(5), 255–259. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00376.x
  • Araújo, S., Pacheco, A., Faísca, L., Petersson, K. M., & Reis, A. (2010). Visual rapid naming and phonological abilities: Different subtypes in dyslexic children. International Journal of Psychology: Journal International de Psychologie, 45(6), 443–452. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2010.499949
  • Araújo, S., Reis, A., Petersson, K. M., & Faísca, L. (2015). Rapid automatized naming and reading performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 868–883. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000006
  • Baddeley, A., Lewis, V., & Vallar, G. (1984). Exploring the articulatory loop. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 36(2), 233–252. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748408402157
  • Baker, S., Gersten, R., Flojo, J., Katz, R., Chard, D., & Clarke, B. (2002). Preventing mathematics difficulties in young children: Focus on effective screening of early number sense delays (Tech. Rep. No. 0305). Pacific Institutes for Research.
  • Balhinez, R., & Shaul, S. (2019). The relationship between reading fluency and arithmetic fact fluency and their shared cognitive skills: A developmental perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1281. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.0128131214086
  • Barrouillet, P., & Fayol, M. (1998). From algorithmic computing to direct retrieval: Evidence from number and alphabetic arithmetic in children and adults. Memory & Cognition, 26(2), 355–368. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03201146
  • Bexkens, A., Wildenberg, W., & Tijms, J. (2015). Rapid automatized naming in children with dyslexia: Is inhibitory control involved? Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 21(3), 212–234. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1487
  • Bowers, P. G., & Newby-Clark, E. (2002). The role of naming speed within a model of reading acquisition. Reading and Writing, 15(1/2), 109–126. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013820421199
  • Bull, R., Espy, K., & Wiebe, S. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in pre-schoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 205–228. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640801982312
  • Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273–293. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN1903_3
  • Cirino, P. (2011). The interrelationships of mathematical precursors in kindergarten. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(4), 713–733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.004
  • Clark, C., Pritchard, V., & Woodward, L. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1176–1191. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019672
  • Clarke, B., & Shinn, M. R. (2004). A preliminary investigation into the identification and development of early mathematics curriculum-based measurement. School Psychology Review, 33(2), 234–248. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2004.12086245
  • Cragg, L., & Gilmore, C. (2014). Skills underlying mathematics: The role of executive function in the development of mathematics proficiency. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 3(2), 63–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2013.12.001
  • Cui, J., Georgiou, G., Zhang, Y., Li, Y., Shu, H., & Zhou, X. (2017). Examining the relationship between rapid automatized naming and arithmetic fluency in Chinese kindergarten children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 154, 146–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.10.008
  • D'Amico, A., & Guarnera, M. (2005). Exploring working memory in children with low arithmetical achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 15(3), 189–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2005.01.002
  • D'Amico, A., & Passolunghi, M. C. (2009). Naming speed and effortful and automatic inhibition in children with arithmetic learning disabilities. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(2), 170–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.01.001
  • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037–2078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006
  • De Smedt, B., & Boets, B. (2010). Phonological processing and arithmetic fact retrieval: Evidence from developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48(14), 3973–3981. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.018
  • De Smedt, B., Taylor, J., Archibald, L., & Ansari, D. (2010). How is phonological processing related to individual differences in children’s arithmetic skills? Developmental Science, 13(3), 508–520. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00897.x
  • DeFlorio, L., & Beliakoff, A. (2015). Socioeconomic status and pre-schoolers' mathematical knowledge: The contribution of home activities and parent beliefs. Early Education and Development, 26(3), 319–341. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.968239
  • Denckla, M., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10(2), 186–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(74)80009-2
  • Diamond, A., Barnett, W., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science (New York, N.Y.), 318(5855), 1387–1388. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1151148
  • Diamond, A., & Wright, A. (2014). An effect of inhibitory load in children while keeping working memory load constant. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 213. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00213
  • Donker, M., Kroesbergen, E., Slot, E., Van Viersen, S., & De Bree, E. (2016). Alphanumeric and non-alphanumeric Rapid automatized naming in children with reading and/or spelling difficulties and mathematical difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 80–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.12.011
  • Durand, M., Hulme, C., Larkin, R., & Snowling, M. (2005). The cognitive foundations of reading and arithmetic skills in 7- to 10-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91(2), 113–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.01.003
  • Escobar, J., Rosas-Díaz, R., Ceric, F., Aparicio, A., Arango, P., Arroyo, R., Espinoza, V., Garolera, M., Pizarro, M., Porflitt, F., Ramírez, M., & Urzua, D. (2018). The role of executive functions in the relation between socioeconomic level and the development of reading and maths skills. Cultura y Educación, 30(2), 368–392. https://doi.org/10.1080/11356405.2018.1462903
  • Foster, M., Anthony, J., Clements, D., & Sarama, J. (2015). Processes in the development of mathematics in kindergarten children from Title 1 schools. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 56–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.004
  • Fuhs, M., & McNeil, N. (2013). ANS acuity and mathematics ability in pre-schoolers from low-income homes: Contributions of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 16(1), 136–148. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12013
  • Furnes, B., Elwér, Å., Samuelsson, S., Olson, R., & Byrne, B. (2019). Investigating the double-deficit hypothesis in more and less transparent orthographies: A longitudinal study from preschool to Grade 2. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23(6), 478–493. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2019.1610410
  • Geary, D. (1993). Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 345–362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.2.345
  • Geary, D. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1539–1552. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025510
  • Geary, D., Hoard, M., & Nugent, L. (2012). Independent contributions of the central executive, intelligence, and in-class attentive behavior to developmental change in the strategies used to solve addition problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(1), 49–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.03.003
  • Georgiou, G., Das, J., & Hayward, D. (2008). Comparing the contribution of two tests of working memory to reading in relation to phonological awareness and rapid naming speed. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(3), 302–318. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2008.00373.x
  • Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., & Liao, C. (2008). Rapid naming speed and reading across languages that vary in orthographic consistency. Reading and Writing, 21(9), 885–903. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9096-4
  • Georgiou, G., Tziraki, N., Manolitsis, G., & Fella, A. (2013). Is rapid automatized naming related to reading and mathematics for the same reason(s)? A follow-up study from kindergarten to Grade 1. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(3), 481–496. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.01.004
  • Georgiou, G., Wei, W., Inoue, T., & Deng, C. (2020). Are the relations of rapid automatized naming with reading and mathematics accuracy and fluency bidirectional? Evidence from a 5-year longitudinal study with Chinese children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(8), 1506–1520. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000452
  • Gersten, R., Jordan, N., & Flojo, J. (2005). Early identification and interventions for students with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(4), 293–304. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222194050380040301
  • Guzmán, B., Rodriguez, C., Sepúlveda, F., & Ferreira, R. (2019). Number sense abilities, working memory and RAN: A longitudinal approximation of typical and atypical development in Chilean children. Revista de Psicodidáctica (English ed.), 24(1), 62–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psicoe.2018.11.003
  • Hecht, S., Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., & Rashotte, C. (2001). The relations between phonological processing abilities and emerging individual differences in mathematical computation skills: A longitudinal study from second to fifth grades. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(2), 192–227. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2000.2586
  • Hornung, C., Martin, R., & Fayol, M. (2017). General and specific contributions of RAN to reading and arithmetic fluency in first graders: A longitudinal latent variable approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1746. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01746
  • Kail, R., Hall, L., & Caskey, B. (1999). Processing speed, exposure to print, and naming speed. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20(2), 303–314. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716499002076
  • Koponen, T., Aunola, K., Ahonen, T., & Nurmi, J. (2007). Cognitive predictors of single-digit and procedural calculation skills and their covariation with reading skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(3), 220–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2007.03.001
  • Koponen, T., Eklund, K., Heikkilä, R., Salminen, J., Fuchs, L., Fuchs, D., & Aro, M. (2020). Cognitive correlates of the covariance in reading and arithmetic fluency: Importance of serial retrieval fluency. Child Development, 91(4), 1063–1080. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.1328731292957
  • Koponen, T., Georgiou, G., Salmi, P., Leskinen, M., & Aro, M. (2017). A meta-analysis of the relation between RAN and mathematics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(7), 977–992. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000182
  • Koponen, T., Salmi, P., Eklund, K., & Aro, T. (2013). Counting and RAN: Predictors of arithmetic calculation and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(1), 162–175. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029285
  • Koponen, T., Salmi, P., Torppa, M., Eklund, K., Aro, T., Aro, M., Pikkeus, M., Lerkkanen, M., & Nurmi, J. (2016). Counting and rapid naming predict the fluency of arithmetic and reading skills. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 44, 83–94.
  • Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). Exploring the impact of phonological awareness, visual-spatial working memory, and preschool quantity-number competencies on mathematics achievement in elementary school: Findings from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 516–531. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.009
  • Lopes-Silva, J., Moura, R., Júlio-Costa, A., Wood, G., Salles, J., & Haase, V. (2016). What is specific and what is shared between numbers and words? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 22. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00022
  • McCallum, R., Bell, S., Wood, M., Below, J., Choate, S., & McCane, S. (2006). What is the role of working memory in reading relative to the big three processing variables (orthography, phonology, and rapid naming)? Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 24(3), 243–259. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282906287938
  • MINEDUC. (2020). National curriculum of mathematic of first grade. https://curriculumnacional.mineduc.cl/614/w3-propertyvalue-53688.html
  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458
  • Montoya, M., Susperreguy, M., Dinarte, L., Morrison, F., San Martin, E., Rojas-Barahona, C., & Förster, C. (2019). Executive function in Chilean preschool children: Do short-term memory, working memory, and response inhibition contribute differentially to early academic skills? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 187–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.02.009
  • Mullis, I., Martin, M., Foy, P., Hooper, M. (2016). TIMSS 2015 international results in mathematics. http://timssandpirls.bc.edu/timss2015/international-results
  • Muñoz-Sandoval, A., Woodcock, R., McGrew, K., & Mather, N. (2005). Woodcock-Muñoz battery III: Cognitive abilities test. Riverside Publishing Company.
  • Noël, M. (2009). Counting on working memory when learning to count and to add: A preschool study. Developmental Psychology, 45(6), 1630–1643. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016224
  • Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 427–452. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-10043121838545
  • Obradović, J., Sulik, M., Finch, J., & Tirado-Strayer, N. (2018). Assessing executive functions in the classroom: Validating a scalable group-based procedure. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 55, 4–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2017.03.003
  • OECD. (2019). PISA 2018 results (Volume I): What students know and can do, PISA. OECD https://www.oecd.org/education/pisa-2018-results-volume-i-5f07c754-en.htm
  • Parrila, R., Kirby, J. R., & Mcquarrie, L. (2004). Articulation rate, naming speed, verbal short-term memory, and phonological awareness: Longitudinal predictors of early reading development?. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(1), 3–26. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0801_2
  • Passolunghi, M. C., & Siegel, L. S. (2001). Short-term memory, working memory, and inhibitory control in children with difficulties in arithmetic problem solving. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80(1), 44–57. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2000.262611511134
  • Passolunghi, M., & Cornoldi, C. (2000). Working memory and cognitive abilities in children with specific difficulties in arithmetic word problem solving. Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities, 14, 155–178.
  • Passolunghi, M., Vercelloni, B., & Schadee, H. (2007). The precursors of mathematics learning: Working memory, phonological ability and numerical competence. Cognitive Development, 22(2), 165–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.09.001
  • Poulsen, M., & Elbro, C. (2013). What's in a name depends on the type of name: The relationships between semantic and phonological access, reading fluency, and reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(4), 303–314. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012.692743
  • Ramírez, V., & Rosas, R. (2007). WISC-III, Chilean version. Ediciones UC.
  • Rosas, R., Espinoza, V., Garolera, M. (2019, August). Intercultural evidence of a tablet-based executive functions test for children between 7 and 10 years [Paper Presentation]. EARLI Conference, Aachen, Germany. https://earli.org/sites/default/files/2019-09/BOA-2019.pdf
  • Rosas, R., Espinoza, V., Porflitt, F., & Ceric, F. (2019). Executive functions can be improved in preschoolers through systematic playing in educational settings: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2024. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02024
  • Rosas, R., & Santa Cruz, C. (2013). Tell me in which school you studied and I will tell you what IQ you have: Radiography of the unequal access to cognitive capital in Chile. Ediciones Universidad Católica de Chile.
  • Shaheen, S. (2014). How child's play impacts executive function-related behaviors. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 3(3), 182–187. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2013.839612
  • Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. (2012). Sources of individual differences in the speed of naming objects and actions: The contribution of executive control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 65(10), 1927–1944. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.670252
  • Simmons, F., & Singleton, C. (2008). Do weak phonological representations impact on arithmetic development? A review of research into arithmetic and dyslexia. Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 14(2), 77–94. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.341
  • Singer, V., Strasser, K., & Cuadro, A. (2019). Direct and indirect paths from linguistic skills to arithmetic school performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(3), 434–445. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000290
  • Susperreguy, M., Douglas, H., Xu, C., Molina-Rojas, N., & LeFevre, J. (2020). Expanding the Home Numeracy Model to Chilean children: Relations among parental expectations, attitudes, activities, and children’s mathematical outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 16–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.06.010
  • Swanson, L., & Kim, K. (2007). Working memory, short-term memory, and naming speed as predictors of children’s mathematical performance. Intelligence, 35(2), 151–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.001
  • Szücs, D. (2016). Subtypes and comorbidity in mathematical learning disabilities: Multidimensional study of verbal and visual memory processes is key to understanding. Progress in Brain Research, 227, 277–304.
  • Szücs, D., Devine, A., Soltesz, F., Nobes, A., & Gabriel, F. (2013). Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuospatial memory and inhibition impairment. Cortex, 49(10), 2674–2688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.06.007
  • Vaessen, A., Gerretsen, P., & Blomert, L. (2009). Naming problems do not reflect a second independent core deficit in dyslexia: Double deficits explored. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(2), 202–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.12.004
  • Wei, W., Li, Y., & Su, H. (2020). Predicting the growth patterns in early mathematics achievement from cognitive and environmental factors among Chinese kindergarten children. Learning and Individual Differences, 79(2020), 101841. Article https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2020.101841
  • Willburger, E., Fussenegger, B., Moll, K., Wood, G., & Landerl, K. (2008). Naming speed in dyslexia and dyscalculia. Learning and Individual Differences, 18(2), 224–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2008.01.003
  • Wise, J., Pae, H., Wolfe, C., Sevcik, R., Morris, R., Lovett, M., & Wolf, M. (2008). Phonological awareness and rapid naming skills of children with reading disabilities and children with reading disabilities who are at risk for mathematics difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice : a Publication of the Division for Learning Disabilities, Council for Exceptional Children, 23(3), 125–136. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2008.00270.x
  • Zhang, X., Koponen, T., Räsänen, P., Aunola, K., Lerkkanen, M., & Nurmi, J. (2014). Linguistic and spatial skills predict early arithmetic development via counting sequence knowledge. Child Development, 85(3), 1091–1107. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12173

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.