531
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Convergent and Divergent Thinking in the Context of Narrative Mysteries

&

References

  • Albrecht, J. E., & O'Brien, E. J. (1993). Updating a mental model: Maintaining both local and global coherence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1061–1070.
  • Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 997–1013.
  • Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
  • Amis, M. (2012). Lionel Asbo: State of England. New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.
  • Bezdek, M. A., Foy, J. E., & Gerrig, R. J. (2013). “Run for it!”: Viewers' participatory responses to film narratives. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 409–416.
  • Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 634–639.
  • Brophy, D. R. (2001). Comparing the attributes, activities, and performance of divergent, convergent, and combination thinkers. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 439–455.
  • Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591–621.
  • Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments for psychology. Psychological Assessment, 6, 284–290.
  • Cook, A. E., & O'Brien, E. J. (2014). Knowledge activation, integration, and validation during narrative text comprehension. Discourse Processes, 51, 26–49.
  • Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 391–404.
  • Cropley, A., & Cropley, D. (2008a). Resolving the paradoxes of creativity: An extended phase model. Cambridge Journal of Education, 38, 355–373.
  • Cropley, D., & Cropley, A. (2008b). Elements of a universal aesthetic of creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 155–161.
  • De Dreu, C. K. W., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood-creativity link: Toward a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 739–756.
  • Egidi, G., & Gerrig, R. J. (2006). Readers' experiences of characters' goals and actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1322–1329.
  • Eidelman, S., Crandall, C. S., & Pattershall, J. (2009). The existence bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 765–775.
  • Eysenck, H. J. (2003). Creativity, personality, and the convergent divergent continuum. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Critical creative processes (pp. 95–114). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
  • Gerrig, R. J. (1993). Experiencing narrative worlds. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
  • Gerrig, R. J., & Jacovina, M. E. (2009). Reader participation in the experience of narrative. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 51, pp. 223–254). Burlington, MA: Academic Press.
  • Gerrig, R. J., Love, J., & McKoon, G. (2009). Waiting for Brandon: How readers respond to small mysteries. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 144–153.
  • Gerrig, R. J., & Wenzel, W. G. (in press). The role of inferences in narrative experiences. In E. J. O'Brien, A. E. Cook, & R. F. Lorch (Eds.), Inferences during reading. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Getzels, J. W., & Jackson, P. W. (1962). Creativity and intelligence: Explorations with gifted students. New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Gilbert, D. (2013). & sons. New York, NY: Random House.
  • Guilford, J. P. (1959). Traits of creativity. In H. H. Anderson (Ed.), Creativity and its cultivation (pp. 142–161). New York, NY: Harper.
  • Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  • Huitema, J. S., Dopkins, S., Klin, C. M., & Myers, J. L. (1993). Connecting goals and actions during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1053–1060.
  • Ivcevic, Z. (2009). Creativity map: Toward the next generation of theories of creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 17–21.
  • Kaufman, J. C. (2011). Individual differences in creativity. In T. Chamorro-Premuzic, S. von Stumm, & A. Furnham (Eds.), Wiley-Blackwell handbook of individual differences (pp. 679–698). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Kaufman, J. C., Pumaccahua, T. T., & Holt, R. E. (2013). Personality and creativity in realistic, investigative, artistic, social, and enterprising college majors. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 913–917.
  • Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–182.
  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Kühn, S., Ritter, S. M., Müller, B. C. N., van Baaren, R. B., Brass, M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). The importance of the default mode network in creativity—A structural MRI study. Journal of Creative Behavior, 48, 152–163.
  • Landers, R. N. (2011). Computing intraclass correlations (ICC) as estimates of interrater reliability in SPSS. Retrieved from http://neoacademic.com/2011/11/16/computing-intraclasscorrelations-icc-as-estimates-of-interrater-reliability-in-spss/.
  • Lin, W. L., Tsai, P. H., Lin, H. Y., & Chen, H. C. (2014). How does emotion influence different creative performances? The mediating role of cognitive flexibility. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 834–844.
  • Love, J., McKoon, G., & Gerrig, R. J. (2010). Searching for Judy: How small mysteries affect narrative processes and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 790–796.
  • McNamara, D. S., & Magliano, J. (2009). Toward a comprehensive model of comprehension. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 51, pp. 298–384). Burlington, MA: Academic Press.
  • Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220–232.
  • Mueller, J. S., Melwani, S., & Goncalo, J. A. (2012). The bias against creativity: Why people desire but reject creative ideas. Psychological Science, 23, 13–17.
  • Myers, J. L., & O'Brien, E. J. (1998). Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse processes, 26, 131–157.
  • Nichols, D. P. (1998). Choosing an intraclass correlation coefficient. UCLA: Statistical Consulting Group. Retrieved from http://www.ats.ucla.edu/stat/spss/library/whichicc.htm.
  • O'Brien, E. J., & Albrecht, J. E. (1992). Comprehension strategies in the development of a mental model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 777–784.
  • O'Brien, E. J., & Myers, J. L. (1999). Text comprehension: A view from the bottom up. In S. R. Goldman, A. C. Graesser, & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Narrative comprehension, causality, and coherence: Essays in honor of Tom Trabasso (pp. 35–53). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • O'Brien, E. J., Rizzella, M. L., Albrecht, J. E., & Halleran, J. G. (1998). Updating a situation model: A memory-based text processing view. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1200–1210.
  • Perrotta, T. (2007). The abstinence teacher. New York, NY: St. Martin's Press.
  • Poynor, D. V., & Morris, R. K. (2003). Inferred goals in narratives: Evidence from self paced reading, recall, and eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 3–9.
  • Rieger, C. J. (1975). Conceptual memory and inference. In R. C. Schank (Ed.), Conceptual information processing (pp. 157–288). New York, NY: Elsevier.
  • Shrout, P., & Fleiss, J. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420–428.
  • Simonton, D. K. (2014). Creative performance, expertise acquisition, individual differences, and developmental antecedents: An integrative research agenda. Intelligence, 45, 66–73.
  • Spielberg, S. (Director) (1975). Jaws [motion picture]. United States: Universal Pictures.
  • Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self-efficacy: Its potential antecedents and relationship to creative performance. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 1137–1148.
  • van den Broek, P., Risden, K., Fletcher, C. R., & Thurlow, R. (1996). A “landscape” view of reading: Fluctuating patterns of activation and the construction of a memory representation. In B. K. Britton & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Models of understanding text (pp. 165–187). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children: A study of the creativity-intelligence distinction. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
  • Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 224–228.
  • Zwaan, R. A., Magliano, J. P., & Graesser, A. C. (1995). Dimensions of situation model construction in narrative comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 386–397.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.