324
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Readers’ processing of perceptual perspective and stance

&

References

  • Akaike, H. (1973). Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. Proceedings from 2nd International Symposium on Information Theory, Budapest, Hungary.
  • Bal, M. (1983). The narrating and the focalizing: A theory of the agents in narrative. Style, 17(2), 234–269.
  • Bal, M. (1985). Narratology: Introduction to the theory of narrative. C. V. Boheemen, Trans. Toronto, Canada: University of Toronto Press.
  • Banfield, A. (1982). Unspeakable sentences: Narration and representation in the language of fiction. New Literary History, 6(2), 247–272.
  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Black, J. B., Turner, T. J., & Bower, G. H. (1979). Point of view in narrative comprehension, memory, and production. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18(2), 187–198. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90118-X
  • Bohan, J., & Filik, R. (2018). Perspective effects during reading: Evidence from text change-detection. Discourse Processes, 55(2), 113–122. doi:10.1080/0163853X.2017.1330020
  • Bortolussi, M., & Dixon, P. (2003). Psychonarratology: Foundations for the empirical study of literary response. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Boyne, J. (2006). The boy in the striped pyjamas. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Brunyé, T. T., Ditman, T., Giles, G. E., Holmes, A., & Taylor, H. A. (2016). Mentally simulating narrative perspective is not universal or necessary for language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(10), 1592.
  • Brunyé, T. T., Ditman, T., Mahoney, C. R., Augustyn, J. S., & Taylor, H. A. (2009). When you and I share perspectives pronouns modulate perspective taking during narrative comprehension. Psychological Science, 20(1), 27–32. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02249.x
  • Chatman, S. (1986). Characters and narrators: Filter, center, slant, and interest-focus. Poetics Today, 7, 189–204.
  • Chatman, S. (1990). Coming to terms: The rhetoric of narrative in fiction and film. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
  • Child, S., Oakhill, J., & Garnham, A. (2018). You’re the emotional one: The role of perspective for emotion processing in reading comprehension. In Language, cognition and neuroscience (pp. 1–12).
  • Cohen, J. (1994). The earth is round (p<.05). American Psychologist, 49, 997–1003.
  • Currie, M. (2011). Postmodern narrative theory. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. (Original work published in 1988).
  • De Vega, M. (1994). Characters and their perspectives in narratives describing spatial environments. Psychological Research, 56(2), 116–126. doi:10.1007/BF00419719
  • Dixon, P. (2003). The p value fallacy and how to avoid it. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale, 57, 189–202.
  • Dixon, P., & Bortolussi, M. (2012). The puzzle of memory for literary text. Fictions, 11, 25–40.
  • Duchan, J. F., Bruder, G. A., & Hewitt, L. E. (Eds.). (1995). Deixis in narrative. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Ehrlich, S. (1990). Point of view: A linguistic analysis of literary style. London, UK: Routledge.
  • Faulkner, W. (1990). As I lay dying. New York, NY: Vintage. (Original work published in 1930). Protective activity of glycolipid antigen against infection by Leptospira interrogans serovar canicola. Protective activity of glycolipid antigen against infection by Leptospira interrogans serovar canicola.
  • Fludernik, M. (2001). The establishment of internal focalization in odd pronominal contexts. In W. Van Peer & S. Chatman (Eds.), New perspective on narrative perspective (pp. 101–113). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
  • Foy, J. E., LoCasto, P. C., Briner, S. W., & Dyar, S. (2017). Would a madman have been so wise as this?” the effects of source credibility and message credibility on validation. Memory & Cognition, 45(2), 281–295. doi:10.3758/s13421-016-0656-1
  • Franklin, N., & Tversky, B. (1990). Searching imagined environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119(1), 63–76. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.119.1.63
  • Franklin, N., Tversky, B., & Coon, V. (1992). Switching points-of-view in spatial mental models. Memory & Cognition, 20(5), 507–518. doi:10.3758/BF03199583
  • Genette, G. (1980). Narrative discourse. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
  • Genette, G. (1997). Paratexts: Thresholds of interpretation (J. E. Lewin & R. Macksey Trans). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University.
  • Gerrig, R. J. (1993). Experiencing narrative worlds. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
  • Gilman, C. P. (1997). The yellow wallpaper and other stories. New York, NY: Dover Publications. (Original work published in 1892).
  • Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2004). Likelihood ratios: A simple and flexible statistic for empirical psychologists. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 791–806. doi:10.3758/BF03196706
  • Graesser, A. C., Bowers, C., Olde, B., White, K., & Person, N. K. (1999). Who knows what? Propagation of knowledge among agents in a literary story world. Poetics, 26(3), 143–175. doi:10.1016/S0304-422X(98)00016-3
  • Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101(3), 371–395.
  • Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics (Vol. 3, pp. 41–58). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Hakala, C. M. (1999). Accessibility of spatial information in a situation model. Discourse Processes, 27(3), 261–279. doi:10.1080/01638539909545063
  • Horton, W. S., & Rapp, D. N. (2003). Out of sight, out of mind: Occlusion and the accessibility of information in narrative comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(1), 104–110. doi:10.3758/BF03196473
  • Jackson, S. (2005). The lottery and other stories. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
  • Jaswal, V. K., Croft, A. C., Setia, A. R., & Cole, C. A. (2010). Young children have a specific, highly robust bias to trust testimony. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1541–1547. doi:10.1177/0956797610383438
  • Kaakinen, J. K., Hyönä, J., & Keenan, J. M. (2002). Perspective effects on online text processing. Discourse Processes, 33(2), 159–173. doi:10.1207/S15326950DP3302_03
  • Kintsch, W., Welsch, D., Schmalhofer, F., & Zimny, S. (1990). Sentence memory: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 29(2), 133–159. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(90)90069-C
  • Lanser, S. (1981). The narrative act. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
  • Lardner, R. (1922). Haircut and other stories. New York, NY: Amereon Ltd.
  • Leech, G. N., & Short, M. (2007). Style in fiction: A linguistic introduction to English fictional prose (13). Harlow, UK: Pearson Education. (Original work published in 1981).
  • Lefcheck, J. S. (2016). PiecewiseSEM: Piecewise structural equation modelling in R for ecology, evolution, and systematics. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 7(5), 573–579. doi:10.1111/2041-210X.12512
  • Márquez, G. G. (2014). One hundred years of solitude. London, UK: Penguin. (Original work published in 1967.)
  • Mohr, N. (1975). El Bronx remembered: A novella and stories. San Francisco, CA and London, UK: Harper and Row.
  • Morrow, D. G., Greenspan, S. L., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Accessibility and situation models in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26(2), 165–187. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(87)90122-7
  • Nakagawa, S., & Schielzeth, H. (2013). A general and simple method for obtaining r2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 4(2), 133–142. doi:10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x
  • Nünning, A. F. (2005). Reconceptualizing unreliable narration: Synthesizing cognitive and rhetorical approaches. In J. R. Phelan, P (Ed.), Companion to narrative theory (pp. 89–107). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Oatley, K. (1999). Meeting of minds: Dialogue, sympathy, and identification in reading fiction. Poetics, 26, 439–454. doi:10.1016/S0304-422X(99)00011-X
  • Pouillon, J. (1946). Temps et roman [time and the novel]. Paris, France: Gallimard.
  • Prince, G. (1987). Dictionary of narratology. Lincoln, NE and London, UK: University of Nebraska Press.
  • R Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved May 1, 2015 from http://www.R-project.org/
  • Rall, J., & Harris, P. L. (2000). In Cinderella’s slippers? Story comprehension from the protagonist’s point of view. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 202. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.202
  • Raney, A. A. (2004). Expanding disposition theory: Reconsidering character likeing, moral evaluations, and enjoyment. Communication Theory, 14(4), 348–369. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2004.tb00319.x
  • Rimmon-Kenan, S. (1983). Narrative fiction: Contemporary poetics. London, UK: Methuen.
  • Schmalhofer, F., & Glavanov, D. (1986). Three components of understanding a programmer’s manual: Verbatim, propositional, and situational representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 25(3), 279–294. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(86)90002-1
  • Segal, E. M., Miller, G., Hosenfeld, C., Mendelsohn, A., Russell, W., Julian, J., … Delphonse, J. (1997). Person and Tense in Narrative Interpretation. Discourse Processes, 24(2–3), 271–307.
  • Singer, M., & Kintsch, W. (2001). Text retrieval: A theoretical exploration. Discourse Processes, 31(1), 27–59. doi:10.1207/S15326950dp3101_2
  • Tolkien, J. R. R. (1974). The fellowship of the ring. ( Original work published in 1954).
  • van den Broek, P., Young, M., Tzeng, Y., & Linderholm, T. (1999). The landscape model of reading: Inferences and the online construction of a memory representation. In S. R. Goldman & H. van Oostendorp (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 71–98). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • van Krieken, K., Hoeken, H., & Sanders, J. (2017). Evoking and measuring identification with narrative characters – A linguistic cues framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1190. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01190
  • van Lissa, C. J., Caracciolo, M., van Duuren, T., & van Leuveren, B. (2016). Difficult empathy: The effect of narrative perspective on readers’ engagement with a first-person narrator. Diegesis, 5, 1.
  • van Peer, W., & Pander Maat, H. (1996). Perspectivation and sympathy: Effects of narrative point of view. In R. J. Kreuz & M. S. MacNealy (Eds.), Empirical approaches to literature and aesthetics (pp. 143–154). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
  • Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2007). A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 779–804. doi:10.3758/BF03194105
  • Wharton, E. (2012). The custom of the country. New York, NJ: Vintage Books. (Original work published in 1913).
  • Ziegler, F., Mitchell, P., & Currie, G. (2005). How does narrative cue children’s perspective taking. Developmental Psychology, 41(1), 115. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.115
  • Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 35–62.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.