153
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Papers / Artículos

Maternal teaching of the symbolic function of a scale model / La enseñanza materna de la función simbólica de una maqueta

, , &
Pages 617-646 | Received 26 Sep 2013, Accepted 23 Oct 2014, Published online: 06 Jul 2015

References / Referencias

  • Bloom, P., & Markson, L. (1998). Intention and analogy in children’s naming of pictorial representations. Psychological Science, 9, 200–204. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00038
  • Callaghan, T. C. (2000). Factors affecting children’s graphic symbol use in the third year. Cognitive Development, 15, 185–214. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00026-5
  • Callaghan, T. C., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., Liszkowski, U., Behne, T., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Early social cognition in three cultural contexts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76, 1–142. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00603.x
  • Callaghan, T. C., Rochat, P., MacGillivray, T., & MacLellan, C. (2004). Modeling referential actions in 6- to 18-month-olds infants: A precursor to symbolic understanding. Child Development, 75, 1733–1744. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00813.x
  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148–153. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005
  • DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science, 238, 1556–1557. doi:10.1126/science.2446392
  • DeLoache, J. S. (2002). Early development of the understanding and use of symbolic objects. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 206–226). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
  • DeLoache, J. S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 66–70. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.004
  • DeLoache, J. S., Peralta de Mendoza, O. A. P., & Anderson, K. (1999). Multiple factors in early symbol use: Instructions, similarity, and age in understanding a symbol-referent relation. Cognitive Development, 14, 299–312. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(99)00006-4
  • DeLoache, J. S., Kolstad, D. V., & Anderson, K. (1991). Physical similarity and young children understanding of scale models. Child Development, 62, 111–126. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01518.x
  • Fiese, B. H. (1990). Playful relationships: A contextual analysis of mother-toddler interaction and symbolic play. Child Development, 61, 1648–1656. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02891.x
  • Gauvain, M., De la Ossa, J. L., & Hurtado-Ortiz, M. T. (2001). Parental guidance as children learn to use cultural tools. The case of pictorial plans. Cognitive Development, 16, 551–575. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00049-1
  • Gelman, S. A., & Ebeling, K. S. (1998). Shape and representational status in children’s early naming. Cognition, 66, B35-47. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00022-5
  • Gentner, D., & Markman, A. B. (1997). Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. American Psychologist, 52, 45–56. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.1.45
  • Gentner, D., & Namy, L. L. (1999). Comparison in the development of categories. Cognitive Development, 14, 487–513. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(99)00016-7
  • Homer, B. D., & Nelson, K. (2009). Naming facilitates young children’s understanding of scale models: Language and the development of symbolic understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 115–134. doi:10.1080/15248370903041298
  • Ittelson, W. H. (1996). Visual perception of markings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 171–187. doi:10.3758/BF03212416
  • Lillard, A. S. (2007). Pretend play in toddlers. In C. A. Brownell, & C. B. Kopp (Eds.), Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations (pp. 149–176). New York, NY: Guilford.
  • Maita, M. R., & Peralta, O. A. (2012). Estrategias maternas de enseñanza de la función simbólica de un objeto. Cultura y Educación, 24, 259–271. doi:10.1174/113564012802845686
  • Namy, L. L., & Gentner, D. (2002). Making a silk purse out of two sow’s ears: Young children’s use of comparison in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 5–15. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.5
  • Newcombe, N., & Huttenlocher, J. (2003). Extending space: Exploring the territory of spatial development: Book review. Human Development, 46, 61–68. doi:10.1159/000067777
  • Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C., & Bornstein, M. (1999). Mothers’ attuned responses to infant affect expressivity promote earlier achievement of language milestones. Infant Behavior & Development, 22, 557–568. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(00)00023-0
  • Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. Journal of Child Language, 5, 1–15. doi:10.1017/s0305000900001896
  • O’Doherty, K., Troseth, G., Shimpi, P., Goldenberg, E., Akhtar, N., & Saylor, M. (2011). Third-party social interaction and word learning from video. Child Development, 82, 902–915. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01579.x
  • Papousek, H., & Papousek, M. (2002). Intuitive parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2: Biology and ecology of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 183–203). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Peralta de Mendoza, O. A. P. (1995). Developmental changes and socioeconomic differences in mother-infant picturebook reading. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 10, 261–272. doi:10.1007/BF03172920
  • Peralta de Mendoza, O. A., & Salsa, A. M. (2003). Instruction in early comprehension and use of a symbol-referent relation. Cognitive Development, 18, 269–284. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(03)00024-8
  • Peralta de Mendoza, O. A., & Salsa, A. M. (2004). Desarrollo temprano de la comprensión de objetos simbólicos: El papel de la instrucción. Cognitiva, 16, 57–71. doi:10.1174/021435504322839162
  • Pierce, C. S. (1974). Collected Papers. C. Hartshorne, & P. Weiss (Eds.) Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Salsa, A. M., & Peralta de Mendoza, O. P. (2007). Routes to symbolization: Intentionality and correspondence in early understanding of pictures. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 79–92. doi:10.1080/15248370709336994
  • Szechter, L. E., & Liben, S. (2004). Parental guidance in preschoolers’ understanding of spatial-graphic representations. Child Development, 75, 869–885. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00711.x
  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural ecology of young children’s interactions with objects and artifacts. In E. Winograd, R. Fivush, & W. Hirst (Eds.), Ecological approaches to cognition: Essays in honor of Ulric Neisser (pp. 153–170). Mahawah, N.J.: Erlbaum.
  • Tomassello, M. (2000). The cultural origins of human cognition. London: Harvard Universtiy Press.
  • Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cole, M., John-Steiner, S., & Souberman, E. (Compiladores y traductores). Cambrige, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Wertsch, J. V. (1979). From social interaction to higher psychological processes a clarification and application of Vygotsky’s theory. Human Development, 22, 1–22. doi:10.1159/000272425.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.