235
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Sensitivity to the Regularity of Letter Patterns Within Print Among Preschoolers: Implications for Emerging Literacy

&
Pages 379-391 | Received 16 May 2017, Accepted 16 Mar 2018, Published online: 21 Aug 2018

References

  • Apel, K., Brimo, D., Wilson-Fowler, E. B., Vorstius, C., & Radach, R. (2013). Children develop initial orthographic knowledge during storybook reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(4), 286–302. doi:10.1080/10888438.2012.692742
  • Apel, K., Wolter, J. A., & Materson, J. (2006). Effects of phonotactic and orthographic probabilities during fast mapping on 5-year-olds’ learning to spell. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 21–42. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2901_3
  • Arciuli, J., & Simpson, I. C. (2012). Statistical learning is related to reading ability in children and adults. Cognitive Science, 36(2), 286–304. doi:10.1111/j.155106709.2011.01200.x
  • Cassar, M., & Treiman, R. (1997). The beginnings of orthographic knowledge: Children’s knowledge of double letters in words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(4), 631–644. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.89.4.631
  • Chetail, F. (2017). What do we do with what we learn? Statistical learning of orthographic regularities impacts written word processing. Cognition, 163, 103–120. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.02.015
  • Conrad, N. J., Harris, N., & Williams, J. (2013). Individual differences in children’s literacy development: The contribution of orthographic knowledge. Reading and Writing: An International Journal, 26(8), 1223–1239. doi:10.1007/s11145-012-9415-2
  • Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M., & Vinckier, F. (2005). The neural code for written words: A proposal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 335–341. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.004
  • Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Pedrolli, K., & Facoetti, A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Current Biology, 22(9), 814–819. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.013
  • Heroman, C., Burts, D. C., Berke, K., & Bickart, T. S. (2010). The creative curriculum for preschool—Volume 5, Objectives for development & learning: Birth through kindergarten. Washington, DC: Teaching Strategies, LLC.
  • Ise, E., Arnoldi, C. J., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2014). Development of orthographic knowledge in German-speaking children: A 2-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Reading, 37(3), 233–249. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01535.x
  • Kessler, B. (2009). Statistical learning mechanisms of conditional orthogrpahic correspondences. Writing Systems Research, 1(1), 19–34. doi:10.1093/wsr/wsp004
  • Kessler, B., Pollo, T. C., Treiman, R., & Cardoso-Martins, C. (2013). Frequency analyses of prephonological spellings as predictors of success in conventional spelling. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(3), 252–259. doi:10.1177/0022219412449440
  • Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., & Johnson, S. P. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain-general learning mechanism. Cognition, 83(2), B35–B42. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00004-5
  • Lambert, R. G., Kim, D. H., Taylor, H., & McGee, J. (2010). Technical manual for the Teaching Strategies GOLD® assessment system. Charlotte, NC: Center for Educational Measurement and Evaluation.
  • Mano, Q. R. (2016). Developing sensitivity to subword combinatorial orthographic regularity (SCORe): A two-process framework. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(3), 231–247. doi:10.1080/10888438.2016.1141210
  • Mano, Q. R., & Guerin, J. M. (2018). Direct and indirect effects of print exposure on silent reading fluency. Reading and Writing, 31(2), 483–502. doi:10.1007/s11145-017-9794-5
  • Mano, Q. R., & Medler, D. A. (2017). Descriptive analysis of subword orthographic structures within the English orthography. Retrieved from http://homepages.uc.edu/~manoqo/LCAN/top30subwords.html
  • Medler, D. A., & Binder, J. R. (2005). MCWord: An on-line orthographic database of the English language. Retrieved from http://www.neuro.mcw.edu/mcword/
  • O’Brien, B. A. (2014). The development of sensitivity to sublexical orthographic constraints: An investigation of positional frequency and consistency using a wordlikeness choice task. Reading Psychology, 35(4), 285–311. doi:10.1080/02702711.2012.724042
  • Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning out of the lab: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 401–426. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.401
  • Pollo, T. C., Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2008). Three perspectives on spelling development. In E. J. Griogorenko & A. Naples (Eds.), Single-word reading: Cognitive, behavioral, and biological perspectives (pp. 175–189). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Pollo, T. C., Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2009). Statistical patterns in children’s early writing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104(4), 410–426. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.07.003
  • Rothe, J., Schulte-Körne, G., & Ise, E. (2014). Does sensitivity to orthographic regularities influence reading and spelling acquisition? A 1-year prospective study. Reading and Writing: An International Journal, 27(7), 1141–1161. doi:10.1007/s11145-013-9479-7
  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.
  • Saffran, J. R., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70(1), 27–52. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00075-4
  • Sperlich, A., Meixner, J., & Laubrock, J. (2016). Development of the perceptual span in reading: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 146, 181–201. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.007
  • Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 24–53. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.24
  • Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2014). How children learn to write words. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Treiman, R., Kessler, B., Boland, K., Clocksin, H., & Chen, Z. (2017). Statistical learning and spelling: Older prephonological spellers produce more wordlike spellings than younger prephonological spellers. Child Development. doi:10.1111/cdev.12893
  • Turk-Browne, N. B., Jungé, J. A., & Scholl, B. J. (2005). The automaticity of visual statistical learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(4), 552–564. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.552
  • Vinckier, F., Qiao, E., Pallier, C., Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2011). The impact of letter spacing on reading: A test of the bigram coding hypothesis. Journal of Vision, 11(8). doi:10.1167/11.6.8
  • Whitney, C. (2001). How the brrain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(2), 221–243. doi:10.3758/BF03196158
  • Wright, D.-M., & Ehri, L. C. (2007). Beginners remember orthography when they learn to read words: The case of doubled letters. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(1), 115–133. doi:10.1017/S0142716406070068

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.