110
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Temporal grouping modulates ipsilateral capture in right visual neglect

&
Pages 584-605 | Received 28 Feb 2014, Accepted 23 Sep 2014, Published online: 22 Oct 2014

REFERENCES

  • Audet, T., Bub, D., & Lecours, A. R. (1991). Visual neglect and left-sided context effects. Brain and Cognition, 16, 11–28. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(91)90082-J
  • Bartolomeo, P., Thiebaut De Schotten, M., & Doricchi, F. (2007). Left unilateral neglect as a disconnection syndrome. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2479–2490. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl181
  • Baylis, C., & Driver, J. (1992). Visual parsing and response competition: The effect of grouping factors. Perception and Psychophysics, 51, 145–162. doi: 10.3758/BF03212239
  • Berti, A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Visual processing without awareness: Evidence from unilateral neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 345–351. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1992.4.4.345
  • Chechlacz, M., Rotshtein, P., Demeyere, N., Bickerton, W-L., & Humphreys, G. W. (2014). The frequency and severity of extinction after stroke affecting different vascular territories. Neuropsychologia, 54, 11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.016
  • Cohen, A., Ivry, R. B., Rafal, R., & Kohn, C. (1995). Activating response codes by stimuli in the neglected visual field. Neuropsychology, 9, 165–173. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.9.2.165
  • Coles, M. G., Gratton, G., Bashore, T. R., Eriksen, C. W. & Donchin, E. (1985). A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 529–553.
  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 215–229. doi: 10.1038/nrn755
  • Danckert, J., Maruff, P., Kinsella, G., Graaff, S., & Currie, J. (1999). Attentional modulation of implicit processing of information in spatial neglect. Neuroreport, 10, 1077–1083. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199904060-00032
  • Diedrichsen, J., Ivry, R. B., Cohen, A., & Danziger, S. (2000). Asymmetries in a unilateral flanker task depend on the direction of the response: The role of attentional shift and perceptual grouping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 113–126.
  • Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1989). Movement and visual attention: The spotlight metaphor breaks down. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 448–456.
  • Duncan, J. (1996). Cooperating brain systems in selective perception and action. In T. Inui & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Attention and performance XVI (pp. 1261–1270). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–458. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.433
  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267
  • Eriksen, C. W., Coles, M. G. H., Morris, L. R., & O'Hara, W. P. (1985). An electromyographic examination of response competition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 165–168. doi: 10.3758/BF03329816
  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., Sirevaag, E. J., Eriksen, C. W., & Donchin, E. (1988). Pre and poststimulus activation of response channels: A psychophysiological analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 331–344.
  • Harris, L., Olson, A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2013). Overcoming the effect of letter confusability in LBL reading: A rehabilitation study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 23, 429–462. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2013.776500
  • Humphreys, G. W. (1998). Neural representation of objects in space: A dual coding account. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 353, 1341–1351. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0288
  • Humphreys, G. W., Riddoch, M. J., Nys, G., & Heinke, D. (2002). Transient binding by time: Neuropsychological evidence from anti-extinction. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 361–380. doi: 10.1080/02643290143000222
  • Làdavas, E., Paladini, R., & Cubelli, R. (1993). Implicit associative priming in a patient with left visual neglect. Neuropsychologia, 31, 1307–1320. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(93)90100-E
  • Marshall, J. C., & Halligan, P. W. (1988). Blindsight and insight in visuo-spatial neglect. Nature, 336, 766–767. doi: 10.1038/336766a0
  • Paquet, L., & Lortie, C. (1990). Evidence for early selection: Precuing target location reduces interference from same-category distractors. Perception & Psychophysics, 48, 382–388. doi: 10.3758/BF03206692
  • Peelen, M. V., Heslenfeld, D. J., & Theeuwes, J. (2004). Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the same large-scale neural network. NeuroImage, 22, 822–830. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.044
  • Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., & Rafal, R. D. (1984). Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 1863–1874.
  • Psychological Software Tools. (2002). E-Prime (Version 1.2). [Computer software]. Pittsburgh, PA: Author.
  • Ptak, R., & Schnider, A. (2010). The dorsal attention network mediates orienting toward behaviorally relevant stimuli in spatial neglect. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 12557–12565. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2722-10.2010
  • Rafal, R. D., Gershberg, F., Egly, R., Ivry, R., Kingstone, A., & Ro, T. (1996). Response channel activation and the lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 34, 1197–1202. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(96)00045-0
  • Riddoch, M. J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1993). The birmingham object recognition battery (BORB). London: Erlbaum.
  • Snow, J. C., & Mattingley, J. B. (2006). Stimulus and goal-driven biases of selective attention following brain damage: Implications for rehabilitation of spatial neglect and extinction. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 24, 233–245.
  • Snow, J. C., & Mattingley, J. B. (2008). Central perceptual load does not reduce ipsilesional flanker interference in parietal extinction. Neuropsychology, 22, 371–382. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.371
  • Vivas, A. B., Humphreys, G. W., & Fuentes, L. J. (2006). Abnormal inhibition of return: A review and new data on patients with parietal lobe damage. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 1049–1064. doi: 10.1080/02643290600588400
  • Ward, R., Goodrich, S. J., & Driver, J. (1994). Grouping reduces visual extinction: Neuropsychological evidence for weight-linkage in visual selection. Visual Cognition, 1, 101–129. doi: 10.1080/13506289408402295
  • Warrington, E. K., & James, M. (1991). The visual object and space perception battery. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley.
  • Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.
  • Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Voluntary versus automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 121–134.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.