5,307
Views
111
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Tool-use: An open window into body representation and its plasticity

, , &
Pages 82-101 | Received 01 Nov 2015, Accepted 15 Mar 2016, Published online: 17 Jun 2016

References

  • Ackroyd, K., Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Nightingale, S., & Townsend, S. (2002). Widening the sphere of influence: Using a tool to extend extrapersonal visual space in a patient with severe neglect. Neurocase, 8(1-2), 1–12. doi:10.1093/neucas/8.1.1
  • Anelli, F., Candini, M., Cappelletti, M., Oliveri, M., & Frassinetti, F. (2015). The remapping of time by active tool-use. PloS One, 10(12), e0146175. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146175
  • Anema, H. A., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., de Haan, E. H. F., Kappelle, L. J., de Kort, P. L. M., Jansen, B. P. W., & Dijkerman, H. C. (2009). A double dissociation between somatosensory processing for perception and action. Neuropsychologia, 47(6), 1615–1620. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.001
  • Arbib, M. A., Bonaiuto, J. B., Jacobs, S., & Frey, S. H. (2009). Tool use and the distalization of the end-effector. Psychological Research, 73(4), 441–462. doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0242-2
  • Baccarini, M., Martel, M., Cardinali, L., Sillan, O., Farnè, A., & Roy, A. C. (2014). Tool use imagery triggers tool incorporation in the body schema. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 105. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00492
  • Baier, B., & Karnath, H.-O. (2008). Tight link between our sense of limb ownership and self-awareness of actions. Stroke: A Journal of Cerebral Circulation, 39(2), 486–488. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.495606
  • Bassolino, M., Finisguerra, A., Canzoneri, E., Serino, A., & Pozzo, T. (2014). Dissociating effect of upper limb non-use and overuse on space and body representations. Neuropsychologia. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.028
  • Bassolino, M., Serino, A., Ubaldi, S., & Làdavas, E. (2010). Everyday use of the computer mouse extends peripersonal space representation. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 803–811. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.009
  • Berlucchi, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (1997). The body in the brain: Neural bases of corporeal awareness. Trends in Neurosciences, 20(12), 560–564.
  • Berlucchi, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2010). The body in the brain revisited. Experimental Brain Research, 200(1), 25–35. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1970-7
  • Berti, A., & Frassinetti, F. (2000). When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 415–420.
  • Bjoertomt, O., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2002). Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 125(Pt 9), 2012–2022.
  • Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., & Seeck, M. (2002). Neuropsychology: Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature, 419(6904), 269–270. doi:10.1038/419269a
  • Blanke, O., Slater, M., & Serino, A. (2015). Behavioral, neural, and computational principles of bodily self-consciousness. Neuron, 88(1), 145–166. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.029
  • Bonifazi, S., Farnè, A., Rinaldesi, L., & Làdavas, E. (2007). Dynamic size-change of peri-hand space through tool-use: Spatial extension or shift of the multi-sensory area. Journal of Neuropsychology, 1(Pt 1), 101–114.
  • Bonnier, P. (1905). L’Aschématie. Revue Neurologique, 13, 606–609.
  • Bourgeois, J., Farnè, A., & Coello, Y. (2014). Costs and benefits of tool-use on the perception of reachable space. Acta Psychologica, 148, 91–95. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.008
  • Brozzoli, C., Cardinali, L., Pavani, F., & Farnè, A. (2010). Action-specific remapping of peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 796–802. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.009
  • Brozzoli, C., Demattè, M. L., Pavani, F., Frassinetti, F., & Farnè, A. (2006). Neglect and extinction: Within and between sensory modalities. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 24(4–6), 217–232.
  • Brozzoli, C., Makin, T. R., Cardinali, L., Holmes, N. P., & Farnè, A. (2012). Peripersonal space: A multisensory interface for body–object interactions. In M. M. Murray & M. T. Wallace (Ed.), The neural bases of multisensory processes. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Consulté à l’adresse http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK92879/
  • Brozzoli, C., Pavani, F., Urquizar, C., Cardinali, L., & Farnè, A. (2009). Grasping actions remap peripersonal space. Neuroreport, 20(10), 913–917. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832c0b9b
  • Buxbaum, L. J., & Coslett, H. B. (2001). Specialised structural descriptions for human body parts: Evidence from autotopagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18(4), 289–306. doi:10.1080/02643290126172
  • Buxbaum, L. J., Shapiro, A. D., & Coslett, H. B. (2014). Critical brain regions for tool-related and imitative actions: A componential analysis. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 137(Pt 7), 1971–1985.
  • Caçola, P., & Gabbard, C. (2012). Modulating peripersonal and extrapersonal reach space via tool use: A comparison between 6- to 12-year-olds and young adults. Experimental Brain Research, 218(2), 321–330. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3017-8
  • Canzoneri, E., Ubaldi, S., Rastelli, V., Finisguerra, A., Bassolino, M., & Serino, A. (2013). Tool-use reshapes the boundaries of body and peripersonal space representations. Experimental Brain Research, 228(1), 25–42. doi:10.1007/s00221-013-3532-2
  • Cardinali, L., Brozzoli, C., & Farnè, A. (2009). Peripersonal space and body schema: Two labels for the same concept? Brain Topography, 21(3–4), 252–260. doi:10.1007/s10548-009-0092-7
  • Cardinali, L., Brozzoli, C., Finos, L., Roy, A. C., & Farnè, A. (2016). The rules of tool incorporation: Tool morpho-functional & sensori-motor constraints. Cognition, 149, 1–5. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.001
  • Cardinali, L., Brozzoli, C., Urquizar, C., Salemme, R., Roy, A. C., & Farnè, A. (2011). When action is not enough: Tool-use reveals tactile-dependent access to body schema. Neuropsychologia, 49(13), 3750–3757. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.033
  • Cardinali, L., Frassinetti, F., Brozzoli, C., Urquizar, C., Roy, A. C., & Farnè, A. (2009). Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Current Biology, 19(12), R478–R479.
  • Cardinali, L., Jacobs, S., Brozzoli, C., Frassinetti, F., Roy, A. C., & Farnè, A. (2012). Grab an object with a tool and change your body: Tool-use-dependent changes of body representation for action. Experimental Brain Research, 218(2), 259–271. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3028-5
  • Cicmil, N., & Eli, K. (2014). Body image among eating disorder patients with disabilities: A review of published case studies. Body Image, 11(3), 266–274. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.04.001
  • Cimmino, R. L., Spitoni, G., Serino, A., Antonucci, G., Catagni, M., Camagni, M., … Pizzamiglio, L. (2013). Plasticity of body representations after surgical arm elongation in an achondroplasic patient. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 31(3), 287–298. doi:10.3233/RNN-120286
  • Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C., Hesse, M. D., Rumiati, R. I., & Fink, G. R. (2008). Where is a nose with respect to a foot? The left posterior parietal cortex processes spatial relationships among body parts. Cerebral Cortex, 18(12), 2879–2890. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn046
  • Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C., Tomasino, B., & Fink, G. R. (2009). What is the position of an arm relative to the body? Neural correlates of body schema and body structural description. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(13), 4162–4171. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4861-08.2009
  • Coslett, H. B., Saffran, E. M., & Schwoebel, J. (2002). Knowledge of the human body: A distinct semantic domain. Neurology, 59(3), 357–363.
  • Costantini, M., Ambrosini, E., Sinigaglia, C., & Gallese, V. (2011). Tool-use observation makes far objects ready-to-hand. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2658–2663. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.013
  • D’Alonzo, M., Clemente, F., & Cipriani, C. (2015). Vibrotactile stimulation promotes embodiment of an alien hand in amputees with phantom sensations. IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering: A Publication of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, 23(3), 450–457. doi:10.1109/TNSRE.2014.2337952
  • Davoli, C. C., Brockmole, J. R., & Witt, J. K. (2012). Compressing perceived distance with remote tool-use: Real, imagined, and remembered. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(1), 80–89. doi:10.1037/a0024981
  • de Vignemont, F. (2010). Body schema and body image--pros and cons. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 669–680. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.022
  • de Vignemont, F. (2011). Embodiment, ownership and disownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(1), 82–93. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.004
  • de Vignemont, F. (in press). The extended body hypothesis. In A. Newen, L. de Bruin, S. Gallagher (Eds.), Oxford handbook of 4e cognition. Mind the body. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • de Vignemont, F., Ehrsson, H. H., & Haggard, P. (2005). Bodily illusions modulate tactile perception. Current Biology, 15(14), 1286–1290. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.06.067
  • de Vignemont, F., & Farnè, A. (2010). Widening the body to rubber hands and tools: What’s the difference? Revue de neuropsychologie, 2(3), 203–211.
  • de Vignemont, F., & Iannetti, G. D. (2014). How many peripersonal spaces? Neuropsychologia. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.018
  • di Pellegrino, G., & Làdavas, E. (2015). Peripersonal space in the brain. Neuropsychologia, 66, 126–133. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.011
  • di Pellegrino, G., Làdavas, E., & Farnè, A. (1997). Seeing where your hands are. Nature, 388(6644), 730. doi:10.1038/41921
  • Di Russo, F., Committeri, G., Pitzalis, S., Spitoni, G., Piccardi, L., Galati, G., … Pizzamiglio, L. (2006). Cortical plasticity following surgical extension of lower limbs. NeuroImage, 30(1), 172–183. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.051
  • Ehrsson, H. H., Holmes, N. P., & Passingham, R. E. (2005). Touching a rubber hand: Feeling of body ownership is associated with activity in multisensory brain areas. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 25(45), 10564–10573. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0800-05.2005
  • Ehrsson, H. H., Kito, T., Sadato, N., Passingham, R. E., & Naito, E. (2005). Neural substrate of body size: Illusory feeling of shrinking of the waist. PLoS Biology, 3(12), e412. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030412
  • Ehrsson, H. H., Rosén, B., Stockselius, A., Ragnö, C., Köhler, P., & Lundborg, G. (2008). Upper limb amputees can be induced to experience a rubber hand as their own. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 131(Pt 12), 3443–3452. doi:10.1093/brain/awn297
  • Farnè, A., Bonifazi, S., & Làdavas, E. (2005). The role played by tool-use and tool-length on the plastic elongation of peri-hand space: A single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(3), 408–418. doi:10.1080/02643290442000112
  • Farnè, A., Iriki, A., & Làdavas, E. (2005). Shaping multisensory action-space with tools: Evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 43(2), 238–248. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.010
  • Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2000). Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. Neuroreport, 11(8), 1645–1649.
  • Farnè, A., Serino, A., & Làdavas, E. (2007). Dynamic size-change of peri-hand space following tool-use: Determinants and spatial characteristics revealed through cross-modal extinction. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 43(3), 436–443.
  • Feinberg, T. E., Venneri, A., Simone, A. M., Fan, Y., & Northoff, G. (2010). The neuroanatomy of asomatognosia and somatoparaphrenia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 81(3), 276–281. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2009.188946
  • Felician, O., Ceccaldi, M., Didic, M., Thinus-Blanc, C., & Poncet, M. (2003). Pointing to body parts: A double dissociation study. Neuropsychologia, 41(10), 1307–1316.
  • Felician, O., Romaiguère, P., Anton, J.-L., Nazarian, B., Roth, M., Poncet, M., & Roll, J.-P. (2004). The role of human left superior parietal lobule in body part localization. Annals of Neurology, 55(5), 749–751. doi:10.1002/ana.20109
  • Fierro, B., Brighina, F., Oliveri, M., Piazza, A., La Bua, V., Buffa, D., & Bisiach, E. (2000). Contralateral neglect induced by right posterior parietal rTMS in healthy subjects. Neuroreport, 11(7), 1519–1521.
  • Fierro, B., Brighina, F., Piazza, A., Oliveri, M., & Bisiach, E. (2001). Timing of right parietal and frontal cortex activity in visuo-spatial perception: A TMS study in normal individuals. Neuroreport, 12(11), 2605–2607.
  • Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Shah, N. J., Weiss, P. H., Halligan, P. W., Grosse-Ruyken, M., … Freund, H. J. (2000). Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fMRI. Neurology, 54(6), 1324–1331.
  • Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Weiss, P. H., & Zilles, K. (2001). The neural basis of vertical and horizontal line bisection judgments: An fMRI study of normal volunteers. NeuroImage, 14(1 Pt 2), S59–S67. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0819
  • Frak, V., Paulignan, Y., & Jeannerod, M. (2001). Orientation of the opposition axis in mentally simulated grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 136(1), 120–127.
  • Fraser, C. (1984). Does an artificial limb become part of the user? British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 47(2), 43–45.
  • Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Galli, G., Noel, J. P., Canzoneri, E., Blanke, O., & Serino, A. (2015). The wheelchair as a full-body tool extending the peripersonal space. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 639. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00639
  • Gallivan, J. P., McLean, D. A., Valyear, K. F., & Culham, J. C. (2013). Decoding the neural mechanisms of human tool use. eLife, 2, 189. doi:10.7554/eLife.00425
  • Gandola, M., Invernizzi, P., Sedda, A., Ferrè, E. R., Sterzi, R., Sberna, M., … Bottini, G. (2012). An anatomical account of somatoparaphrenia. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 48(9), 1165–1178. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.012
  • Ganesh, G., Yoshioka, T., Osu, R., & Ikegami, T. (2014). Immediate tool incorporation processes determine human motor planning with tools. Nature Communications, 5, 4524. doi:10.1038/ncomms5524
  • Garbarini, F., Fossataro, C., Berti, A., Gindri, P., Romano, D., Pia, L., … Neppi-Modona, M. (2015). When your arm becomes mine: Pathological embodiment of alien limbs using tools modulates own body representation. Neuropsychologia, 70, 402–413. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.008
  • Gardner, R. M., & Brown, D. L. (2014). Body size estimation in anorexia nervosa: A brief review of findings from 2003 through 2013. Psychiatry Research, 219(3), 407–410. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.06.029
  • Giglia, G., Mattaliano, P., Puma, A., Rizzo, S., Fierro, B., & Brighina, F. (2011). Neglect-like effects induced by tDCS modulation of posterior parietal cortices in healthy subjects. Brain Stimulation, 4(4), 294–299. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2011.01.003
  • Giglia, G., Pia, L., Folegatti, A., Puma, A., Fierro, B., Cosentino, G., … Brighina, F. (2015). Far Space Remapping by Tool Use: A rTMS Study Over the Right Posterior Parietal Cortex. Brain Stimulation. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2015.01.412
  • Giummarra, M. J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Nicholls, M. E. R., Gibson, S. J., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2010). The phantom in the mirror: A modified rubber-hand illusion in amputees and normals. Perception, 39(1), 103–118.
  • Goldenberg, G., & Spatt, J. (2009). The neural basis of tool use. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 132(Pt 6), 1645–1655. doi:10.1093/brain/awp080
  • Graziano, M. S. A., & Cooke, D. F. (2006). Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior. Neuropsychologia, 44(6), 845–859. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.009
  • Graziano, M. S. A., Cooke, D. F., & Taylor, C. S. (2000). Coding the location of the arm by sight. Science, 290(5497), 1782–1786.
  • Head, H., & Holmes, G. (1911). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain, 34, 102–254.
  • Hermsdörfer, J., Li, Y., Randerath, J., Roby-Brami, A., & Goldenberg, G. (2013). Tool use kinematics across different modes of execution. Implications for action representation and apraxia. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 49(1), 184–199. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.10.010
  • Holmes, N. P. (2012). Does tool use extend peripersonal space? A review and re-analysis. Experimental Brain Research, 218(2), 273–282. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3042-7
  • Holmes, N. P., Calvert, G. A., & Spence, C. (2004). Extending or projecting peripersonal space with tools? Multisensory interactions highlight only the distal and proximal ends of tools. Neuroscience Letters, 372(1-2), 62–67. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.024
  • Holmes, N. P., Calvert, G. A., & Spence, C. (2007). Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: Evidence from the crossmodal congruency task. Experimental Brain Research, 183(4), 465–476. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-1060-7
  • Holmes, N. P., Sanabria, D., Calvert, G. A., & Spence, C. (2007). Tool-use: Capturing multisensory spatial attention or extending multisensory peripersonal space? Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 43(3), 469–489.
  • Holmes, N. P., Spence, C., Hansen, P. C., Mackay, C. E., & Calvert, G. A. (2008). The multisensory attentional consequences of tool use: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. PloS One, 3(10), e3502. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003502
  • Inoue, K., Kawashima, R., Sugiura, M., Ogawa, A., Schormann, T., Zilles, K., & Fukuda, H. (2001). Activation in the ipsilateral posterior parietal cortex during tool use: A PET study. NeuroImage, 14(6), 1469–1475. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0942
  • Invernizzi, P., Gandola, M., Romano, D., Zapparoli, L., Bottini, G., & Paulesu, E. (2013). What is mine? Behavioral and anatomical dissociations between somatoparaphrenia and anosognosia for hemiplegia. Behavioural Neurology, 26(1-2), 139–150. doi:10.3233/BEN-2012-110226
  • Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. Neuroreport, 7(14), 2325–2330.
  • Jacobs, S., Bussel, B., Combeaud, M., & Roby-Brami, A. (2009). The use of a tool requires its incorporation into the movement: Evidence from stick-pointing in apraxia. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 45(4), 444–455. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.12.009
  • Jacobs, S., Danielmeier, C., & Frey, S. H. (2010). Human anterior intraparietal and ventral premotor cortices support representations of grasping with the hand or a novel tool. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(11), 2594–2608. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21372
  • Jovanov, K., Clifton, P., Mazalek, A., Nitsche, M., & Welsh, T. N. (2015). The limb-specific embodiment of a tool following experience. Experimental Brain Research. doi:10.1007/s00221-015-4342-5
  • Kammers, M. P. M., Kootker, J. A., Hogendoorn, H., & Dijkerman, H. C. (2010). How many motoric body representations can we grasp? Experimental Brain Research, 202(1), 203–212. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2124-7
  • Kilteni, K., Maselli, A., Kording, K. P., & Slater, M. (2015). Over my fake body: Body ownership illusions for studying the multisensory basis of own-body perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 613. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00141
  • Lang, K., Lopez, C., Stahl, D., Tchanturia, K., & Treasure, J. (2014). Central coherence in eating disorders: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry: The Official Journal of the World Federation of Societies of Biological Psychiatry, 15(8), 586–598. doi:10.3109/15622975.2014.909606
  • Làdavas, E. (2002). Functional and dynamic properties of visual peripersonal space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 17–22.
  • Làdavas, E., & Serino, A. (2008). Action-dependent plasticity in peripersonal space representations. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25(7), 1099–1113. doi:10.1080/02643290802359113
  • Lewis, J. W. (2006). Cortical networks related to human use of tools. The Neuroscientist: A Review Journal Bringing Neurobiology, Neurology and Psychiatry, 12(3), 211–231. doi:10.1177/1073858406288327
  • Longo, M. R., & Haggard, P. (2010). An implicit body representation underlying human position sense. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(26), 11727–11732. doi:10.1073/pnas.1003483107
  • Longo, M. R., & Lourenco, S. F. (2007). Space perception and body morphology: Extent of near space scales with arm length. Experimental Brain Research, 177(2), 285–290. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-0855-x
  • Macuga, K. L., & Papailiou, A. P. (2012). Motor imagery of tool use: Relationship to actual use and adherence to Fitts’ law across tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 218(2), 169–179. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3004-0
  • Makin, T. R., Holmes, N. P., Brozzoli, C., & Farnè, A. (2012). Keeping the world at hand: Rapid visuomotor processing for hand-object interactions. Experimental Brain Research, 219(4), 421–428. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3089-5
  • Makin, T. R., Holmes, N. P., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2008). On the other hand: Dummy hands and peripersonal space. Behavioural Brain Research, 191(1), 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.041
  • Maravita, A., Clarke, K., Husain, M., & Driver, J. (2002). Active tool use with the contralesional hand can reduce cross-modal extinction of touch on that hand. Neurocase, 8(6), 411–416. doi:10.1076/neur.8.5.411.16177
  • Maravita, A., Husain, M., Clarke, K., & Driver, J. (2001). Reaching with a tool extends visual-tactile interactions into far space: Evidence from cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 39(6), 580–585.
  • Maravita, A., & Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 79–86. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008
  • Maravita, A., Spence, C., Clarke, K., Husain, M., & Driver, J. (2000). Vision and touch through the looking glass in a case of crossmodal extinction. Neuroreport, 11(16), 3521–3526.
  • Maravita, A., Spence, C., Kennett, S., & Driver, J. (2002). Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans. Cognition, 83(2), B25–34.
  • Martel, M., Cardinali, L., Jouffrais, C., Finos, L., Farnè, A., & Roy, A. C. (2014). Proprioception alone drives Body Schema plasticity (CNS abstract).
  • McDonnell, P. M., Scott, R. N., Dickison, J., Theriault, R. A., & Wood, B. (1989). Do artificial limbs become part of the user? New evidence. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 26(2), 17–24.
  • Medina, J., & Coslett, H. B. (2010). From maps to form to space: Touch and the body schema. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 645–654. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.017
  • Medina, J., Khurana, P., & Coslett, H. B. (2015). The influence of embodiment on multisensory integration using the mirror box illusion. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 71–82. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.08.011
  • Miller, L. E., Longo, M. R., & Saygin, A. (2015). Vision during tool use is both necessary and sufficient for recalibration of tactile perception of body size. Journal of Vision, 15(12), 362. doi:10.1167/15.12.362
  • Miller, L. E., Longo, M. R., & Saygin, A. P. (2014). Tool morphology constrains the effects of tool use on body representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(6), 2143–2153. doi:10.1037/a0037777
  • Mort, D. J., Malhotra, P., Mannan, S. K., Rorden, C., Pambakian, A., Kennard, C., & Husain, M. (2003). The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 126(Pt 9), 1986–1997. doi:10.1093/brain/awg200
  • Murray, C. D. (2004). An interpretative phenomenological analysis of the embodiment of artificial limbs. Disability and Rehabilitation, 26(16), 963–973. doi:10.1080/09638280410001696764
  • Murray, C. D. (2008). Embodiment and prosthetics. In P. Gallagher, D. Desmond, M. MacLachlan (Eds.), Psychoprosthetics (pp. 119–129). London: Springer.
  • Neppi-Mòdona, M., Rabuffetti, M., Folegatti, A., Ricci, R., Spinazzola, L., Schiavone, F., … Berti, A. (2007). Bisecting lines with different tools in right brain damaged patients: The role of action programming and sensory feedback in modulating spatial remapping. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 43(3), 397–410.
  • Osiurak, F., & Massen, C. (2014). The cognitive and neural bases of human tool use. Cognition, 5, 1107. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01107
  • Paillard, J., Michel, F., & Stelmach, G. (1983). Localization without content. A tactile analogue of “blind sight”. Archives of Neurology, 40(9), 548–551.
  • Pazzaglia, M., Galli, G., Scivoletto, G., & Molinari, M. (2013). A functionally relevant tool for the body following spinal cord injury. PloS One, 8(3), e58312. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058312
  • Pegna, A. J., Petit, L., Caldara-Schnetzer, A. S., Khateb, A., Annoni, J. M., Sztajzel, R., & Landis, T. (2001). So near yet so far: Neglect in far or near space depends on tool use. Annals of Neurology, 50(6), 820–822.
  • Quallo, M. M., Price, C. J., Ueno, K., Asamizuya, T., Cheng, K., Lemon, R. N., & Iriki, A. (2009). Gray and white matter changes associated with tool-use learning in macaque monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(43), 18379–18384. doi:10.1073/pnas.0909751106
  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (1997). The space around us. Science, 277(5323), 190–191. doi:10.1126/science.277.5323.190
  • Rizzolatti, G., Scandolara, C., Matelli, M., & Gentilucci, M. (1981a). Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses. Behavioural Brain Research, 2(2), 147–163.
  • Rizzolatti, G., Scandolara, C., Matelli, M., & Gentilucci, M. (1981b). Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. I. Somatosensory responses. Behavioural Brain Research, 2(2), 125–146.
  • Romano, D., Caffa, E., Hernandez-Arieta, A., Brugger, P., & Maravita, A. (2015). The robot hand illusion: Inducing proprioceptive drift through visuo-motor congruency. Neuropsychologia, 70, 414–420. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.033
  • Romano, D., Gandola, M., Bottini, G., & Maravita, A. (2014). Arousal responses to noxious stimuli in somatoparaphrenia and anosognosia: Clues to body awareness. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 137(Pt 4), 1213–1223. doi:10.1093/brain/awu009
  • Rosén, B., Ehrsson, H. H., Antfolk, C., Cipriani, C., Sebelius, F., & Lundborg, G. (2009). Referral of sensation to an advanced humanoid robotic hand prosthesis. Scandinavian Journal of Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery and Hand Surgery, 43(5), 260–266. doi:10.3109/02844310903113107
  • Rossetti, A., Romano, D., Bolognini, N., & Maravita, A. (2015). Dynamic expansion of alert responses to incoming painful stimuli following tool use. Neuropsychologia, 70, 486–494. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.019
  • Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., & Boisson, D. (1995). Implicit processing of somaesthetic information: A dissociation between where and how? Neuroreport, 6(3), 506–510.
  • Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., & Boisson, D. (2001). Numbsense: A case study and implications. In B. D. Gelder, E. H. F. D. Haan, & C. A. Heywood (Eds.), Out of mind: Varieties of unconscious processes (pp. 265–292). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Schilder, P. (1935). The image and appearance of the human body; studies in the constructive energies of the psyche. London: Kegan Paul.
  • Schmalzl, L., Kalckert, A., Ragnö, C., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2014). Neural correlates of the rubber hand illusion in amputees: A report of two cases. Neurocase, 20(4), 407–420. doi:10.1080/13554794.2013.791861
  • Schwoebel, J., Buxbaum, L. J., & Coslett, H. B. (2004). Representations of the human body in the production and imitation of complex movements. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21(2), 285–298. doi:10.1080/02643290342000348
  • Schwoebel, J., & Coslett, H. B. (2005). Evidence for multiple, distinct representations of the human body. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(4), 543–553. doi:10.1162/0898929053467587
  • Schwoebel, J., Coslett, H. B., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2001). Compensatory coding of body part location in autotopagnosia: Evidence for extrinsic egocentric coding. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18(4), 363–381. doi:10.1080/02643290126218
  • Serino, A., Bassolino, M., Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2007). Extended multisensory space in blind cane users. Psychological Science, 18(7), 642–648. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01952.x
  • Serino, A., Canzoneri, E., & Avenanti, A. (2011). Fronto-parietal areas necessary for a multisensory representation of peripersonal space in humans: An rTMS study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2956–2967. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00006
  • Serino, A., Canzoneri, E., Marzolla, M., di Pellegrino, G., & Magosso, E. (2015). Extending peripersonal space representation without tool-use: Evidence from a combined behavioral-computational approach. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 257. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00004
  • Shenton, J. T., Schwoebel, J., & Coslett, H. B. (2004). Mental motor imagery and the body schema: Evidence for proprioceptive dominance. Neuroscience Letters, 370(1), 19–24. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2004.07.053
  • Sirigu, A., Grafman, J., Bressler, K., & Sunderland, T. (1991). Multiple representations contribute to body knowledge processing. Evidence from a case of autotopagnosia. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 114(Pt 1B), 629–642.
  • Spence, C., Pavani, F., & Driver, J. (2004). Spatial constraints on visual-tactile cross-modal distractor congruency effects. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4(2), 148–169.
  • Sposito, A., Bolognini, N., Vallar, G., & Maravita, A. (2012). Extension of perceived arm length following tool-use: Clues to plasticity of body metrics. Neuropsychologia, 50(9), 2187–2194. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.022
  • Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Tsakiris, M., Marquardt, T., & Bianchi-Berthouze, N. (2015). Action sounds update the mental representation of arm dimension: Contributions of kinaesthesia and agency. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 689. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00689
  • Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Väljamäe, A., Toshima, I., Kimura, T., Tsakiris, M., & Kitagawa, N. (2012). Action sounds recalibrate perceived tactile distance. Current Biology, 22(13), R516–517. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.028
  • Tamè, L., Farnè, A., & Pavani, F. (2011). Spatial coding of touch at the fingers: Insights from double simultaneous stimulation within and between hands. Neuroscience Letters, 487(1), 78–82. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.078
  • Tamè, L., Farnè, A., & Pavani, F. (2013). Vision of the body and the differentiation of perceived body side in touch. Cortex:A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 49(5), 1340–1351. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.03.016
  • Tarhan, L. Y., Watson, C. E., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2015). Shared and Distinct Neuroanatomic Regions Critical for Tool-related Action Production and Recognition: Evidence from 131 Left-hemisphere Stroke Patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1–21. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00876
  • Tessari, A., Ottoboni, G., Baroni, G., Symes, E., & Nicoletti, R. (2012). Is access to the body structural description sensitive to a body part’s significance for action and cognition? A study of the sidedness effect using feet. Experimental Brain Research, 218(4), 515–525. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3045-4
  • Tomasino, B., Weiss, P. H., & Fink, G. R. (2012). Imagined tool-use in near and far space modulates the extra-striate body area. Neuropsychologia, 50(10), 2467–2476. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.018
  • Treshi-Marie Perera, A., Newport, R., & McKenzie, K. J. (2015). Multisensory distortions of the hand have differential effects on tactile perception. Experimental Brain Research. doi:10.1007/s00221-015-4384-8
  • Tsakiris, M. (2010). My body in the brain: A neurocognitive model of body-ownership. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 703–712. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.034
  • Tsakiris, M., Carpenter, L., James, D., & Fotopoulou, A. (2010). Hands only illusion: Multisensory integration elicits sense of ownership for body parts but not for non-corporeal objects. Experimental Brain Research, 204(3), 343–352. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2039-3
  • Tsakiris, M., Costantini, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). The role of the right temporo-parietal junction in maintaining a coherent sense of one’s body. Neuropsychologia, 46(12), 3014–3018. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.06.004
  • Urgesi, C. (2015). Multiple Perspectives on Body Image Research. European Psychologist, 20(1), 1–5. doi:10.1027/1016-9040/a000223
  • Vallar, G., & Ronchi, R. (2009). Somatoparaphrenia: A body delusion. A review of the neuropsychological literature. Experimental Brain Research, 192(3), 533–551. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1562-y
  • Valyear, K. F., Cavina-Pratesi, C., Stiglick, A. J., & Culham, J. C. (2007). Does tool-related fMRI activity within the intraparietal sulcus reflect the plan to grasp? NeuroImage, 36(Suppl 2), T94–T108. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.031
  • Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(6), 1479–1492. doi:10.1037/a0010781
  • Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(5), 880–888. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.880