512
Views
14
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Lexical processing and distributional knowledge in sound–spelling mapping in a consistent orthography: A longitudinal study of reading and spelling in dyslexic and typically developing children

ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 163-186 | Received 05 Apr 2017, Accepted 15 Sep 2017, Published online: 23 Oct 2017

References

  • Angelelli, P., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., Zoccolotti, P., & Luzzatti, C. (2004). Characteristics of writing disorders in Italian dyslexic children. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 17, 18–31. doi: 10.1097/00146965-200403000-00003
  • Angelelli, P., Marinelli, C., & Burani, C. (2014). The effect of morphology on spelling and reading accuracy: A study on Italian children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–10. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01373
  • Angelelli, P., Marinelli, C. V., De Salvatore, M., & Burani, C. (2017). Morpheme-based reading and spelling in Italian children with developmental dyslexia and dysgraphia. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice. doi:10.1002/dys.1554
  • Angelelli, P., Marinelli, C. V., Iaia, M., Notarnicola, A., Costabile, D., Judica, A., … Luzzatti, C. (2016). DDO-2 Diagnosi dei disturbi ortografici in età evolutiva [Diagnosis of Orthographic Deficits in Childhood]. Trento: Erickson.
  • Angelelli, P., Marinelli, C. V., Putzolu, A., Notarnicola, A., Iaia, M., & Burani, C. (2017). Learning to spell in a language with transparent orthography: Distributional properties of orthography and whole-word lexical processing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 7, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1275715
  • Angelelli, P., Marinelli, C. V, & Zoccolotti, P. (2010). Single or dual orthographic representations for reading and spelling? A study on Italian dyslexic and dysgraphic children. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27, 305–333. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2010.543539
  • Angelelli, P., Notarnicola, A., Judica, A., Zoccolotti, P., & Luzzatti C. (2010). Spelling impairments in Italian dyslexic children: Phenomenological changes in primary school Cortex, 46, 1299–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.015
  • Angelelli, P., Notarnicola, A., Marcolini, S., & Burani, C. (2014). Interaction between the lexical and sublexical spelling procedures: A study on Italian primary school children. Rivista Italiana di Psicolinguistica Applicata/Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 23–43.
  • Angelelli, P., Putzolu, A., Iaia, M., Marinelli, C. V., Gasperini, F., Chilosi, A. M., & Brizzolara, D. (2016). Spelling impairments in Italian dyslexic children with and without a history of early language delay. Are There any Differences? Frontiers in Psychology - Educational Psychology, 7, 1–13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00527
  • Apel, K., Wolter, J. A., & Masterson J. J. (2006). Effects of phonotactic and orthotactic probabilities during fast mapping on 5-year-olds’ learning to spell. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 21–42. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2901_3
  • Arciuli, J., Monaghan, P., & Seva, N. (2010). Learning to assign lexical stress during reading aloud: Corpus, behavioral, and computational, investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 180–196. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.03.005
  • Arciuli, J., & Simpson, I. C. (2012). Statistical learning is related to reading ability in children and adults. Cognitive Science, 36, 286–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01200.x
  • Barca, L., Burani, C., Di Filippo, G., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Italian developmental dyslexic and proficient readers: Where are the differences? Brain and Language, 98, 347–351. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.05.001
  • Barry, C., & De Bastiani, P. (1997). Lexical priming of nonword spelling in the regular orthography of Italian. Reading and Writing, 9, 499–517. doi: 10.1023/A:1007907431371
  • Barry, C., & Seymour, P. H. K. (1988). Lexical priming and sound-to-spelling contingency effects in nonword spelling. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 40, 5–40. doi: 10.1080/14640748808402280
  • Bergmann, J., & Wimmer, H. (2008). A dual-route perspective on poor reading in a regular orthography: Evidence from phonological and orthographic lexical decisions. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 653–676. doi: 10.1080/02643290802221404
  • Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2004). Stress regularity or consistency? Reading aloud Italian polysyllables with different stress patterns. Brain and Language, 90, 318–325. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00444-9
  • Burani, C., Paizi, D., & Sulpizio, S. (2014). Stress assignment in Italian: Friendship outweighs dominance. Memory & Cognition, 42, 662–675. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0379-5
  • Burani, C., Thornton, A. M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2017). Literacy acquisition in Italian. In L. Verhoeven & C. Perfetti (Eds.), Reading acquisition across languages and writing systems: An international handbook (pp. 181–210). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Burden, V. (1989). A comparison of priming effects on the nonword spelling performance of good and poor spellers. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 43–65. doi: 10.1080/02643298908253284
  • Campbell, R. (1983). Writing nonwords to dictation. Brain and Language, 19, 153–178. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(83)90061-5
  • Campbell, R. (1987). One or two lexicons for reading and writing words: Can misspellings shed any light? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4(4), 487–499. doi: 10.1080/02643298708252049
  • Colombo, L. (1992). Lexical stress effect and its interaction with frequency in word pronunciation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 987–1003. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.987
  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Ziegler, J., & Langdon, R. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204
  • Cornoldi, C., & Colpo, G. (1998). Prove di Lettura MT. Guida alluso [The MT Reading Test: User Manual]. Firenze, IT: Organizzazioni Speciali.
  • De Mauro, T. (1989). VELI. Vocabolario elettronico della lingua Italiana [VELI. Electronic dictionary of Italian language]. Milano: IBM Italia.
  • Folk, J. R., Rapp, B., & Goldrick, M. (2002). The interaction of lexical and sublexical information in spelling: What’s the point? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 653–671. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000184
  • Folk, J. R., & Rapp, B. (2004). Interaction of lexical and sublexical information in spelling: Evidence from nonword spelling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 565–585. doi:10.1017/S0142716404001274
  • Frith U. (1980). Cognitive processes in spelling. London: Academic Press.
  • Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00054
  • Hanley, J. R., Hastie, K., & Kay, J. (1992). Developmental surface dyslexia and dysgraphia: An orthographic processing impairment. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 44, 285–319. doi: 10.1080/02724989243000046
  • Hillis, A. E., Rapp, B. C., & Caramazza, A. (1999). When a rose is a rose in speech but a tulip in writing. Cortex, 35, 337–356. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70804-9
  • Houghton, G., & Zorzi, M. (2003). Normal and impaired spelling in a connectionist dual–route architecture. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 115–162. doi: 10.1080/02643290242000871
  • Lété, B., Peereman, R., & Fayol, M. (2008). Consistency and word-frequency effects on spelling among !rst- to !fth-grade French children: A regression-based study. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 952–977. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2008.01.001
  • Luce, P. A., & Large, N. R. (2001). Phonotactics, density, and entropy in spoken word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 565–581. doi: 10.1080/01690960143000137
  • Marconi, L., Ott, M., Pesenti, E., Ratti, D., & Tavella, M. (1993). Lessico elementare: Dati statistici sullitaliano scritto e letto dai bambini delle elementari [Elementary lexicon: Statistical data for Italian written and read by elementary school children]. Bologna: Zanichelli.
  • Marinelli, C. V., Angelelli, P., Notarnicola, A., & Luzzatti, C. (2009). Do Italian dyslexic children use the lexical reading route efficiently? An orthographic judgment task. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 333–351. doi: 10.1007/s11145-008-9118-x
  • Marinelli, C. V., Romani, C., Burani, C., McGowan, V. A., Zoccolotti, P., & Perea, M. (2016). Costs and benefits of orthographic inconsistency in reading: Evidence from a cross-linguistic comparison. PlosOne, 11(6), e0157457. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157457
  • Marinelli, C. V., Romani, C., Burani, C., & Zoccolotti, P. (2015). Spelling acquisition in English and Italian: A cross-linguistic study. Frontiers in Psychology - Educational Psychology, 6, 1–13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01843
  • Marinelli, C. V., Traficante, D., Zoccolotti, P., & Burani, C. (2013). Orthographic neighbourhood-size effects on the reading aloud of Italian children with and without dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 333–349. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2012.723080
  • Marinelli, C. V., Traficante, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2014). Does pronounceability modulate the letter string deficit of children with dyslexia? Evidence from lexical decision task. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–16. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01353
  • Mason, M. (1975). Reading ability and letter search time: Effects of orthographic structure defined by single-letter positional frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 146–166. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.104.2.146
  • McCloskey, M., Macaruso, P., & Rapp, B. (2006). Grapheme-to-lexeme feedback in the spelling system: Evidence from dysgraphia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 278–307. doi:10.1080/02643290442000518
  • Notarnicola, A., Angelelli, P., Judica, A., & Zoccolotti, P. (2012). Development of spelling skills in a shallow orthography: The case of Italian language. Reading and Writing, 25, 1171–1194. doi: 10.1007/s11145-011-9312-0
  • Pacton, S., Fayol, M., & Perruchet, P. (2002). The acquisition of untaught orthographic regularities in French. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 121–136). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
  • Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning out of the lab: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 401–426. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.401
  • Pacton, S., Sobaco, A., Fayol, M., & Treiman, R. (2013). How does graphotactic knowledge influence children’s learning of new spellings? Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–10. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00701
  • Paizi, D., Zoccolotti, P., & Burani, C. (2011). Lexical stress assignment in Italian developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 24, 443–461. doi: 10.1007/s11145-010-9236-0
  • Perruchet, P., & Pacton, S. (2006). Implicit learning and statistical learning: One phenomenon, two approaches. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 233–238. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.006
  • Protopapas, A., & Gerakaki, S. (2009). Development of processing stress diacritics in reading Greek. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 453–483. doi: 10.1080/10888430903034788
  • Pruneti, C. A., Fenu, A., Freschi, G., Rota, S., Cocci, D., Marchionni, M., … Baracchini Murratorio, G. (1996). Aggiornamento alla standardizzazione italiana del test delle Matrici Progressive Colorate di Raven (CPM). Bollettino di Psicologia Applicata, 217, 51–57.
  • Rapp, B., Epstein, C., & Tainturier, M. J. (2002). The integration of information across lexical and sublexical processes in spelling. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 1–29. doi: 10.1080/0264329014300060
  • Rapp, B., Folk, J., & Tainturier, M. J. (2000). Word reading. In B. Rapp (Ed.), What deficits reveal about the human mind: A handbook of cognitive neuropsychology (pp. 233–262). Philadelphia,PA: Psychology Press.
  • Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.523
  • Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Barnes, M. A., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1984). When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition?. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 383–404. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90270-6
  • Seymour, P. H. K., & Dargie, A. (1990). Associative priming and orthographic choice in nonword spelling. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2, 395–410. doi: 10.1080/09541449008406215
  • Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Mancini, M., Martelli, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Length effect in word naming in reading: Role of reading experience and reading deficit in Italian readers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 217–235. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2702_2
  • Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words: Phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 1321–1337. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2001/103
  • Sulpizio, S., Arduino, L. S., Paizi, D., & Burani, C. (2013). Stress assignment in reading Italian polysyllabic pseudowords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 51–68. doi: 10.1037/a0028472
  • Sulpizio, S., & Colombo, L. (2013). Lexical stress, frequency and stress neighborhood effects in the early stages of Italian reading development. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2073–2084. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.785577
  • Tainturier, M. J., Bosse, M. L., Roberts, D. J., Valdois, S., & Rapp, B. (2013). Lexical neighborhood effects in pseudoword spelling. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–8. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00862
  • Temple, M. C. (1986). Developmental dysgraphias. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 38, 77–110. doi: 10.1080/14640748608401586
  • Traficante, D., Marcolini, S., Luci, A., Zoccolotti, P., & Burani, C. (2011). How do roots and suffixes influence reading of pseudowords: A study of young Italian readers with and without dyslexia. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 777–793. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.496553
  • Treiman, R., & Cassar, A. G. (1997). Spelling acquisition in English. In C. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp. 61–80). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2006). Spelling as statistical learning: Using consonantal context to spell vowels. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 642–652. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.642
  • Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell: A study of first-grade children. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, A., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169–193. doi: 10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00139-5
  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Judica, A., & Martelli, M. (2009). Reading development in an orthographically regular language: Effects of length, frequency, lexicality and global processing ability. Reading and Writing, 22, 1053–1079. doi: 10.1007/s11145-008-9144-8
  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Judica, A., Orlandi, M., & Spinelli, D. (1999). Markers of developmental surface dyslexia in a language (Italian) with high grapheme-phoneme correspondence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 191–216. doi: 10.1017/S0142716499002027

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.