1,922
Views
27
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The impact of developmental dyslexia and dysgraphia on movement production during word writing

, , &
Pages 219-251 | Received 07 Feb 2017, Accepted 21 Sep 2017, Published online: 20 Nov 2017

References

  • Abbott, R., Berninger, V., & Fayol, M. (2010). Longitudinal relationships of levels of language in writing and between writing and reading in grades 1 to 7. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 281–298.
  • Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among developmental skills and writing skills in primary and intermediate grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 478–508.
  • Afonso, O., & Álvarez, C. J. (2011). Phonological effects in handwriting production: Evidence from the implicit priming paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1474–1483.
  • Afonso, O., Álvarez, C. J., & Kandel, S. (2015). Effects of grapheme-to-phoneme probability on writing durations. Memory & Cognition, 43, 579–592.
  • Afonso, O., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2015). Spelling impairments in spanish dyslexic adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 3. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00466
  • Afonso, O., Suárez-Coalla, P., González-Martín, N., & Cuetos, F. (2017). The impact of word frequency on peripheral processes during handwriting: A matter of age. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13, 1–9.
  • Agresti, A. (2013). Categorical data analysis (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Álvarez, C. J., Cottrell, D., & Afonso, O. (2009). Writing dictated words and picture names: Syllabic boundaries affect execution in spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 205–223.
  • Angelelli, P., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., Zoccolotti, P., & Luzzati, C. (2004). Characteristics of writing disorders in Italian dyslexic children. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 17, 18–31.
  • Angelelli, P., Notarnicola, A., Judica, A., Zoccolotti, P., & Luzzati, C. (2010). Spelling impairments in Italian dyslexic children: Phenomenological changes in primary school. Cortex, 46, 1299–1311.
  • Bar, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278.
  • Berninger, V. (2006). A developmental approach to learning disabilities. In I. Siegel, & A. Renninger (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, child psychology and practice. IV (pp. 420–452). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Thomson, J., & Raskind, W. (2001). Language phenotype for reading and writing disability: A family approach. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 59–106.
  • Berninger, V., Nielsen, K., Abbott, R., Wijsman, E., & Raskind, W. (2008). Writing problems in developmental dyslexia: Under-recognized and under-treated. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 1–21.
  • Berninger, V., Richards, T., & Abbott, R. (2015). Differential diagnosis of dysgraphia, dyslexia, and OWL LD: Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Reading and Writing. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 28, 1119–1153.
  • Berninger, V., & Winn, W. (2006). Implications of advancements in brain research and technology for writing development, writing instruction, and educational evolution. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 96–114). New York, NY: Guilford.
  • Berninger, V., Yates, C., Cartwright, A., Rutberg, J., Remy, E., & Abbott, R. (1992). Lower level developmental skills in beginning writing. Reading and Writing. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 257–280.
  • Bosse, M. L., Kandel, S., Prado, C., & Valdois, S. (2014). Does visual attention span relate to eye movements during reading and copying? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 81–85.
  • Bosse, M. L., & Valdois, S. (2009). Influence of the visual attention span on child reading performance: A cross sectional study. Journal of Research in Reading, 32, 230–253.
  • Brady, S., & Shankweiler, D. (1991). Phonological processes in literacy. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Brun-Henin, F., Velay, J.-L., Beecham, Y., & Cariou, S. (2013). Troubles d’écriture et dyslexie: revue théorique, aspects cliniques et approche expérimentale. Développements, 4–27.
  • Buchwald, A., & Falconer, C. (2014). Cascading activation from lexical processing to letter-level processing in written word production. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31, 606–621.
  • Caramazza, A., & Miceli, G. (1990). The structure of graphemic representations. Cognition, 37, 243–297.
  • Chilosi, A. M., Brizzolara, D., Lami, L., Pizzoli, C., Gasperini, F., Pecini, C., … Zoccolotti, P. (2009). Reading and spelling disabilities in children with and without a history of early language delay: A neuropsychological and linguistic study. Child Neuropsychology, 15(6), 582–604.
  • Connelly, V., Campbell, S., MacLean, M., & Barnes, J. (2006). Contribution of lower order skills to the written composition of college students with and without dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 175–196.
  • Connelly, V., Dockrell, J. E., & Barnett, J. (2005). The slow handwriting of undergraduate students constrains overall performance in exam essays. Educational Psychology, 25, 99–107.
  • Costa, V., Fischer-Baum, S., Capasso, R., Miceli, G., & Rapp, B. (2011). Temporal stability and representational distinctiveness: Key functions of orthographic working memory. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 28, 338–62.
  • Delattre, M., Bonin, P., & Barry, C. (2006). Written spelling to dictation: Sound-to-spelling regularity affects both writing latencies and durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1330–1340.
  • Di Betta, A. M., & Romani, C. (2006). Lexical learning and dysgraphia in a group of adults with developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 376–400.
  • Dienes, Z. (2016). How Bayes factors change scientific practice. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 72, 78–89.
  • Friend, A., & Olson, R. K. (2008). Phonological spelling and reading deficits in children with spelling disabilities. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12(1), 90–105.
  • Frith, U. (1986). A developmental framework for developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 36, 67–81.
  • Goulandris, N. K., & Snowling, M. (1991). Visual memory deficits: A plausible cause of developmental dyslexia? Evidence from a single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 127–154.
  • Graham, S., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Abbott, S., & Whitaker, D. (1997). The role of mechanics in composing of elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(1), 170–182.
  • Graham, S., Berninger, V., Weintraub, N., & Schafer, W. (1998). Development of handwriting speed and legibility in grades 1–9. The Journal of Educational Research, 92, 42–52.
  • Guinet, E., & Kandel, S. (2010). Ductus: A software package for the study of handwriting production. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 326–332.
  • Halsband, U., & Lange, R. K. (2006). Motor learning in man: A review of functional and clinical studies. Journal of Physiology, 99, 414–424.
  • Hanley, J. R., Hastie, K., & Kay, J. (1992). Developmental surface dysLexia and dysgraphia: An orthographic processing impairment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 34–43.
  • Hayes, J. R. (2012). Modeling and remodeling writing. Written Communication, 29, 369–388.
  • Jacquier-Roux, M., Lequette, C., Pouget, G., Valdois, S., & Zorman, M. (2010). Batterie Analytique du Langage Écrit. Grenoble: Laboratoire cogni-sciences, Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition.
  • Jacquier-Roux, M., Valdois, S., & Zorman, M. (2002). Odedys: Outil de dépistage des dyslexies. Grenoble: Laboratoire cogni-sciences, IUFM de Grenoble.
  • Jones, D. (2004). Automaticity of the transcription process in the production of written text. (Doctor of philosophy thesis). Graduate School of Education, University of Queensland, Australia.
  • Kandel, S., Álvarez, C. J., & Vallée, N. (2006). Syllables as processing units in handwriting production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 18–31.
  • Kandel, S., Peereman, R., & Ghimenton, A. (2013). Further evidence for the interaction between central and peripheral processes: The impact of double letters in writing English words. Frontiers in Psychology (Research Topic “Writing Words : From Brain to Hand(s)” – Section Cognitive Science), 4, 729.
  • Kandel, S., Peereman, R., & Ghimenton, A. (2014). How do we code the letters of a word when we have to write it? Investigating double letter representation in French. Acta Psychologica, 148, 56–62.
  • Kandel, S., Peereman, R., Grosjacques, G., & Fayol, M. (2011). For a psycholinguistic model of handwriting production: Testing the syllable-bigram controversy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1310–1322.
  • Kandel, S., & Perret, C. (2015a). How does the interaction between spelling and motor processes build up during writing acquisition? Cognition, 136, 325–336.
  • Kandel, S., & Perret, C. (2015b). How do movements to produce letters become automation during writing acquisition? Investigating the development of motor anticipation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 39, 113–120.
  • Kandel, S., & Spinelli, E. (2010). Processing complex graphemes in handwriting production. Memory & Cognition, 38(6), 762–770.
  • Kandel, S., Spinelli, E., Tremblay, A., Guerassimovitch, H., & Álvarez, C. J. (2012). Processing prefixes and suffixes in handwriting production. Acta Psychologica, 140, 187–195.
  • Kandel, S., & Valdois, S. (2006). Syllables as functional units in a copying task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 432–452.
  • Korkman, M., Kiri, U., & Kemp, S. (1998). Nepsy: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2014). Lmertest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models ( lmer objects of lme4 packages. Retrieved from URL http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest/
  • Lambert, E., Alamargot, D., Larocque, D., & Caporossi, G. (2011). Dynamics of the spelling process during a copy task: Effects of regularity and frequency. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 65(3), 141–150.
  • Lambert, E., Sausset, S., & Rigalleau, F. (2015). The ortho-syllable as a processing unit in handwriting: The mute e effect. Reading and Writing, 28, 683–698.
  • Lefavrais, P. (1967). Test de l’Alouette [The Lark test] (2nd ed.). Paris: Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.
  • Lété, B., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Colé, P. (2004). Manulex: A grade-level lexical database from French elementary school readers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 156–166.
  • Lussier, F., Guerin, F., Dufresne A., & Lassonde, M. (1998). Etude normative developpementale des fonctions executives: la tour de Londres. ANAE, 47, 42–52.
  • Maeland, A. F., & Karlsdottir, R. (1991). Development of reading, spelling, and writing skills from third to sixth grade in normal and dysgraphic children. In J. Wann, A. M. Wing, & N. Søvik (Eds.), Development of graphic skills (pp. 179–189). London: Academic Press.
  • Maggio, S., Lété, B., Chenu, F., Jisa, H., & Fayol, M. (2012). Tracking the mind during writing: Immediacy, delayed, and anticipatory effects on pauses and writing rate. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 2131–2151.
  • Martinet, C., & Valdois, S. (1999). L’apprentissage de l’orthographe d’usage et ses troubles dans la dyslexie developpementale de surface. / learning to spell words: Difficulties in developmental surface dyslexia. L’Année Psychologique, 99, 577–622.
  • Martlew, M. (1992). Handwriting and spelling: Dyslexic children’s abilities compared with children of the same chronological age and younger children of the same spelling level. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 375–390.
  • Medwell, J., & Wray, D. (2007). Handwriting: What do we know and what do we need to know? Literacy, 41, 10–15.
  • Menghini, D., Finzi, A., Carlesiom, G. A., & Vicari, S. (2011). Working memory impairment in children with developmental dyslexia: Is it just a phonological deficit? Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 199–213.
  • Meulenbroek, R.G.J., & van Galen, G.P. (1989a). The acquisition of skilled handwriting: Discontinuous trends in kinematic variables. In A. M. Colley, J. R. Beech (Eds.) Cognition and action in skilled behaviour (pp. 93–101). North Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers BV.
  • Meulenbroek, R. G. J., & van Galen, G. P. (1989b). The production of connecting strokes in cursive writing: Developing co-articulation in 8 to 12 year-old children. In R Plamondon, CY Suen, & ML Simner (Eds.), Computer recognition and human production of handwriting (pp. 105–118). Singapore: World Scientific.
  • Meulenbroek, R. G. J., & van Galen, G. P. (1990). Perceptual-motor complexity of printed and cursive letters. The Journal of Experimental Education, 58, 95–110.
  • Mojet, W. (1991). Characteristics of developing handwriting skills in elementary education. In J Wann, A Wing, & N Sovik (Eds.), Development of graphic skills (pp. 53–75). London: Academic Press.
  • Mortimore, T., & Crozier, R. (2006). Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills in higher education. Studies in Higher Education, 31, 235–251.
  • Mousty, P., Leybaert, J., Alegria, J., Content, A., & Morais, J. (1994). Belec: Une batterie d’évaluation du langage écrit et de ses troubles. In J. Grégoire, & B. Piérart (Eds.), Evaluer les troubles de la lecture : les nouveaux modèles théoriques et leurs implications diagnostiques (pp. 127–145). Bruxelles: De Boeck.
  • Nation, K., Angell, P., & Castles, A. (2007). Orthographic learning via self-teaching in children learning to read English: Effects of exposure, durability, and context. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 71–84.
  • New, B., Pallier, C., Brysbaert, M., & Ferrand, L. (2004). Lexique 2: A new French lexical database. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 516–524.
  • Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001). Dyslexia, development and the cerebellum. Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 515–16.
  • Overvelde, A., & Hulstijn, W. (2011). Handwriting development in grade 2 and grade 3 primary school children with normal, at risk, or dysgraphic characteristics. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 540–548.
  • Paz-Villagran, V., Gilhodes, J.-C., & Velay, J.-L. (2013). Les enfants dysgraphiques sont-ils réellement plus lents que les autres? Développements, 38–44.
  • Peereman, R., Lété, B., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2007). Manulex-infra: Distributional characteristics of grapheme-phoneme mappings, and infralexical and lexical units in child-directed written material. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 579–589.
  • Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. New York, NY, USA: Springer.
  • Pitt, M. A., & Myung, I. J. (2002). When a good fit can be bad. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 421–425.
  • Qu, Q., Damian, M. F., Zhang, Q., & Zhu, X. (2011). Phonology contributes to writing: Evidence from written production in nonalphabetic language. Psychological Science, 22, 1107–1112.
  • Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212–218.
  • Ramus, F., Marshall, C. R., Rosen, S., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2013). Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: Towards a multidimensional model. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 136, 630–645.
  • Ramus, F., & Szenkovits, G. (2008). What phonological deficit? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 129–141.
  • Rapp, B., Epstein, C., & Tainturier, M.-J. (2002). The integration of information across lexical and sublexical processes in spelling. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 1–29.
  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1998). Progressive matrices standard (PM38). Paris: EAP.
  • R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R foundation for statistical computiong. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from URL http://www.R-project.org/
  • Rodriguez, C., & Villaroel, R. (2016). Predicting handwriting difficulties through spelling processes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 50(5), 504–510.
  • Rosenblum, S., Weiss, P. L., & Parush, S. (2003). Product and process evaluation of handwriting difficulties: A review. Educational Psychology Review, 15, 41–81.
  • Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t-tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 225–237.
  • Roux, S., McKeeff, T. J., Grosjacques, G., Afonso, O., & Kandel, S. (2013). The interaction between central and peripheral processes in handwriting production. Cognition, 127, 235–241.
  • Rüsseler, J., Probst, S., Sönke, J., & Münte, T. (2003). Recognition memory for high- and Low-frequency words in adult normal and dyslexic readers: An event-related brain potential study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology (Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition: Section A), 25, 815–829.
  • Saksida, A., Ianuzzi, S., Bogliotti, C., Chaix, Y., Démonet, J.-F., … Ramus, F. (2016). Phonological skills, visual attention span, and visual stress in developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1503–1516.
  • Sausset, S., Lambert, E., Olive, T., & Larocque, D. (2012). Processing of syllables during handwriting: Effects of graphomotor constraints. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1872–1879.
  • Savage, R., Pillay, V., & Melidona, S. (2008). Rapid serial naming is a unique predictor of spelling in children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(3), 235–250.
  • Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (2005). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
  • Schmidt, R. a. R. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82(4), 225–260.
  • Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Statistics, 6, 461–464.
  • Seymour, P. H., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94(2), 143–174.
  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.
  • Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 95–129.
  • Slaoui, Y., & Nuel, G. (2014). Parameter estimation in a hierarchical random intercept model with censored response: An approach using a SEM algorithm and gibbs sampling. Sankhȳa : The Indian Journal of Statistics, 76, 210–233.
  • Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. J. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An Introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage Publishers.
  • Snowling, M. J., Goulandris, N., Bowbly, M., & Howell, P. (1986). Segmentation and speech perception in relation to reading skill: A developmental analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 489–507.
  • Søvik, N., & Arntzen, O. (1986). A comparative study of the writing/spelling performances of “normal”, dyslexic, and dysgraphic children. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 1, 85–101.
  • Strattman, K., & Hodson, B. W. (2005). Variables that influence decoding and spelling in beginning readers. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 21, 165–190.
  • Sumner, E., Connelly, V., & Barnett, A. L. (2012). Children with dyslexia are slow writers because they pause more often and not because they are slow at handwriting execution. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 40, 1441–1447.
  • Sumner, E., Connelly, V., & Barnett, A. L. (2014). The influence of spelling ability on handwriting production: Children With and without dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1441–1447.
  • Valdois, S., Bidet-Ildei, C., Lassus-Sangosse, D., Reilhac, C., N’Guyen, M. A., Guinet, E., & Orliaguet, J. P. (2011). A visual processing but no phonological disorder in a child with mixed dyslexia. Cortex, 47, 1197–1218.
  • Van Galen, G. P. (1991). Handwriting: Issues for a psychomotor theory. Human Movement Science, 10, 165–191.
  • Wechsler, D. (2005). WISC-IV: Echelle d’Intelligence de Wechsler pour Enfants [Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children] (4th ed.). Paris: Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.
  • Wolff, P. H., Cohen, C., & Drake, C. (1984). Impaired motor timing control in specific reading retardation. Neuropsychologia, 22, 587–600.
  • World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: Author.
  • Zhang, Q., & Damian, M. F. (2010). Impact of phonology on the generation of handwritten responses: Evidence from picture-word interference tasks. Memory & Cognition, 38, 519–528.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.