346
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Word deafness with preserved number word perception

, &
Pages 415-429 | Received 13 Nov 2017, Accepted 15 Aug 2018, Published online: 03 Sep 2018

References

  • Amalric, M., & Dehaene, S. (2016). Origins of the brain networks for advanced mathematics in expert mathematicians. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(18), 4909–4917. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1603205113
  • Anderson, S. W., Damasio, A. R., & Damasio, H. (1990). Troubled letters but not numbers: Domain specific cognitive impairments following focal damage in frontal cortex. Brain, 113(3), 749–766. doi: 10.1093/brain/113.3.749
  • Bencini, G. M., Pozzan, L., Bertella, L., Mori, I., Pignatti, R., Ceriani, F., & Semenza, C. (2011). When two and too don’t go together: A selective phonological deficit sparing number words. Cortex, 47(9), 1052–1062. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.03.013
  • Brannon, E. M., & Terrace, H. S. (1998). Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. Science, 282(5389), 746–749. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5389.746
  • Breedin, S. D., Saffran, E. M., & Coslett, H. B. (1994). Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11(6), 617–660. doi: 10.1080/02643299408251987
  • Caño, A., Rapp, B., Costa, A., & Juncadella, M. (2008). Deafness for the meanings of number words. Neuropsychologia, 46(1), 63–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.08.008
  • Cantlon, J. F. (2012). Math, monkeys, and the developing brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(Supplement 1), 10725–10732. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201893109
  • Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Mahon, B., & Caramazza, A. (2003). What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits? A critical review of the clinical evidence. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3–6), 213–261. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000266
  • Cappelletti, M., Kopelman, M. D., Morton, J., & Butterworth, B. (2005). Dissociations in numerical abilities revealed by progressive cognitive decline in a patient with semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(7), 771–793. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000293
  • Caramazza, A., & Mahon, B. Z. (2003). The organization of conceptual knowledge: The evidence from category-specific semantic deficits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(8), 354–361. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00159-1
  • Caramazza, A., & Shelton, J. R. (1998). Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain: The animate-inanimate distinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(1), 1–34. doi: 10.1162/089892998563752
  • Cipolotti, L., Butterworth, B., & Denes, G. (1991). A specific deficit for numbers in a case of dense acalculia. Brain, 114, 2619–2637. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.6.2619
  • Cohen, J., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J. (1993). Psyscope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 25(2), 257–271. doi: 10.3758/BF03204507
  • Cohen, L., Verstichel, P., & Dehaene, S. (1997). Neologistic jargon sparing numbers: A category-specific phonological impairment. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(7), 1029–1061. doi: 10.1080/026432997381349
  • Crannell, C. W., & Parrish, J. N. (1957). A comparison of immediate memory span for digits, letters, and words. The Journal of Psychology, 44, 319–327. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1957.9713089
  • Crawford, J. R., & Howell, D. C. (1998). Comparing an individual’s test score against norms derived from small samples. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12(4), 482–486. doi: 10.1076/clin.12.4.482.7241
  • Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K. (2003). The selective impairment of fruit and vegetable knowledge: A multiple processing channels account of fine-grain category specificity. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3–6), 355–372. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000220
  • Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1986). The syllable’s differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 25(4), 385–400. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(86)90033-1
  • Dehaene, S. (2011). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Delazer, M., Lochy, A., Jenner, C., Domahs, F., & Benke, T. (2002). When writing 0 (zero) is easier than writing O (o): A neuropsychological case study of agraphia. Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 2167–2177. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00044-1
  • Dell, G. S., Martin, N., & Schwartz, M. F. (2007). A case-series test of the interactive two-step model of lexical access: Predicting word repetition from picture naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(4), 490–520. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.05.007
  • Dial, H., & Martin, R. (2017). Evaluating the relationship between sublexical and lexical processing in speech perception: Evidence from aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 96, 192–212. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.009
  • Dotan, D., & Friedmann, N. (2015). Steps towards understanding the phonological output buffer and its role in the production of numbers, morphemes, and function words. Cortex, 63, 317–351. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.014
  • Dunn, D. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
  • Farah, M. J., & Wallace, M. A. (1992). Semantically-bounded anomia: Implications for the neural implementation of naming. Neuropsychologia, 30(7), 609–621. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90066-U
  • Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 307–314. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002
  • Goldinger, S. D., & Azuma, T. (2003). Puzzle-solving science: The quixotic quest for units in speech perception. Journal of Phonetics, 31(3), 305–320. doi: 10.1016/S0095-4470(03)00030-5
  • Gordon, P. (2004). Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia. Science, 306(5695), 496–499. doi: 10.1126/science.1094492
  • Grotheer, M., Ambrus, G. G., & Kovács, G. (2016). Causal evidence of the involvement of the number form area in the visual detection of numbers and letters. NeuroImage, 132, 314–319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.069
  • Han, Z., Shao, A., & Bi, Y. (2011). Double dissociations of word and number processing in auditory and written modalities: A case study. Neurocase, 17(5), 418–424. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2010.532140
  • Hart, J., & Gordon, B. (1992). Neural subsystems for object knowledge. Nature, 359(6390), 60–64. doi: 10.1038/359060a0
  • Huth, A. G., de Heer, W. A., Griffiths, T. L., Theunissen, F. E., & Gallant, J. L. (2016). Natural speech reveals the semantic maps that tile human cerebral cortex. Nature, 532(7600), 453–458. doi: 10.1038/nature17637
  • Jones, G., & Macken, B. (2015). Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: Why digit span measures long-term associative learning. Cognition, 144, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.009
  • Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19(1), 1–36. doi: 10.1097/00003446-199802000-00001
  • Marangolo, P., Piras, F., & Fias, W. (2005). “I can write seven but I can’t say it”: A case of domain-specific phonological output deficit for numbers. Neuropsychologia, 43(8), 1177–1188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.001
  • Martin, R. C., Lesch, M. F., & Bartha, M. C. (1999). Independence of input and output phonology in word processing and short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 41(1), 3–29. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2637
  • McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1–86. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0
  • McCloskey, M., Schubert, T., Rothlein, D., Rapp, B., Slonim, D., & Van Den Heuvel, K. (2013). Investigation and rehabilitation of a new acquired reading disorder. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 94, 203–204. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.09.100
  • McCrink, K., Spelke, E. S., Dehaene, S., & Pica, P. (2013). Non-symbolic halving in an Amazonian indigene group. Developmental Science, 16(3), 451–462. doi: 10.1111/desc.12037
  • Nieder, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Representation of number in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 185–208. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135550
  • Norris, D. (1994). Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 52(3), 189–234. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90043-4
  • Roach, A., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Grewal, R. S., & Brecher, A. (1996). The Philadelphia naming test: Scoring and rationale. Clinical Aphasiology, 24, 121–133.
  • Sacchett, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (1992). Calling a squirrel a squirrel but a canoe a wigwam: A category-specific deficit for artefactual objects and body parts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9(1), 73–86. doi: 10.1080/02643299208252053
  • Samson, D., & Pillon, A. (2003). A case of impaired knowledge for fruit and vegetables. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3–6), 373–400. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000329
  • Shum, J., Hermes, D., Foster, B. L., Dastjerdi, M., Rangarajan, V., Winawer, J., … Parvizi, J. (2013). A brain area for visual numerals. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(16), 6709–6715. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4558-12.2013
  • Slevc, L. R., & Shell, A. R. (2015). Auditory agnosia. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 129, 573–587. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-62630-1.00032-9
  • Starrfelt, R. (2007). Selective alexia and agraphia sparing numbers—A case study. Brain and Language, 102(1), 52–63. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.09.005
  • Wolmetz, M., Poeppel, D., & Rapp, B. (2011). What does the right hemisphere know about phoneme categories? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(3), 552–569. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21495

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.